19.10.2014 Views

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Developmental dyslexia in adults: a research review 43<br />

‘simple model’ of reading (Gough & Tunmer, 1986) with the observation that the critical<br />

difficulty in dyslexia lies in decoding at the single-word level rather than in comprehending<br />

passages of text (Snowling, 1991). It is worth bearing in mind that word recognition difficulties<br />

are not exclusive to dyslexia; they are also characteristic of poor readers who do not meet the<br />

diagnostic criteria for dyslexia and of good readers who are beginning to read (Seidenberg et<br />

al., 1986).<br />

Nevertheless, while locating the critical difficulty in dyslexia at the level of word recognition,<br />

commentators acknowledge that adult dyslexics also have problems with reading<br />

comprehension and other aspects of literacy (Nicolson et al., 1993). Comprehension is made<br />

more difficult for all people with poor reading skills when they have to allocate attentional<br />

resources to the task of decoding (Sabatini, 2002); it can be difficult enough for ‘good’ readers<br />

when they are faced with unusual texts (like the present one). Listening comprehension,<br />

despite the belief that it is unimpaired in dyslexia, will inevitably become more difficult when<br />

the spoken language uses vocabulary normally acquired through reading or when it employs<br />

the more complex syntax of the written language—two difficulties either created or<br />

exacerbated by low exposure to print—or when utterance length makes greater demands on<br />

verbal memory. There may be a case for locating the core difficulty of dyslexia not in word<br />

recognition without further specification but in rapid and automatic word recognition, where<br />

impairments have serious implications for reading comprehension.<br />

Similarly, this account of reading does not imply that difficulty in decoding is the only problem<br />

for poor readers in general: for these people, especially, comprehension is an important<br />

independent source of difficulty (Cornoldi & Oakhill, 1996). Neither does it imply that, where<br />

comprehension is the critical difficulty in low literacy, any failure of comprehension is<br />

necessarily accompanied by difficulties in decoding regular words, as there is more than one<br />

point at which the acquisition of literacy can founder (Spear-Swerling & Sternberg, 1994).<br />

Although phonological awareness is a good predictor of reading development at the outset<br />

(McBride-Chang & Kail, 2002), it may be a much less efficient predictor of reading ability in<br />

later years (de Jong & van der Leij, 1999), when readers come to rely on it less and less<br />

because of their increasingly automatised (or ‘direct route’) word recognition (Doctor &<br />

Coltheart, 1980). Of special concern is the ‘slump’ observed in children who have succeeded<br />

in learning to decode familiar, regularly-spelled words but who lose momentum when they<br />

encounter less familiar words, particularly when those words are irregularly spelled or have<br />

abstract meanings (Chall et al., 1990). This slump is associated with socio-economic<br />

disadvantage, where the quality of language in the home may be a less than optimal<br />

preparation for schooling (Bernstein, 1971; Feagans & Farran, 1982).<br />

Although the difficulties of dyslexic people do not stop at single-word decoding, the present<br />

review confines its attention to this problem.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!