19.10.2014 Views

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Developmental dyslexia in adults: a research review 39<br />

<strong>Dyslexia</strong> subtypes do not occur naturally; they are artefacts of research and teaching<br />

strategies.<br />

Is there any bias in our perception of dyslexia?<br />

In literacy practices there are cross-cultural differences. Some, like differences between<br />

English and Chinese writing and spelling systems, are obvious (Gleitman & Rozin, 1977);<br />

others, like differences between English and Chinese readers in brain morphology and<br />

function, are not (Hsieh et al., 20<strong>01</strong>). In dyslexia, too, there are cross-cultural differences<br />

reflecting differences between writing and spelling systems (Habib et al., 2000; Paulesu et al.,<br />

20<strong>01</strong>); there are also cross-cultural similarities (Ho et al., 2000; McBride-Chang & Kail, 2002).<br />

In passing, it deserves mention that not even the Chinese are ‘Chinese’ (as opposed to<br />

‘Phonecian’) readers, since Chinese characters contain not only semantic but also phonetic<br />

elements (McBride-Chang & Ho, 2000). However, dyslexia is more evident in alphabetic<br />

writing systems than it is in nonalphabetic systems, like Chinese. It is also more evident in a<br />

morphophonemic spelling system with complex relationships between phonemes and<br />

graphemes, like English, than it is in a phonemic system with simple relationships between<br />

phonemes and graphemes, like Finnish. In these respects, there is cultural bias.<br />

Writing systems are not the only potential sources of cultural bias in dyslexia. Differences in<br />

socio-economic status—the standard proxy measure of differences in educational and social<br />

opportunity—have long been implicated in the debate about dyslexia. Here, there is a paradox.<br />

Some parents may believe that exceptional abilities in music or mathematics are ‘innate’<br />

when their children have no obvious flair for them, but if their children are found to be<br />

deficient in a skill such as reading they will blame environmental influences such as poor<br />

teaching (Bateson & Martin, 1999). Other parents accept personal responsibility even while<br />

denying it, as they embrace the social cachet of dyslexia (which might be heritable) while<br />

disclaiming the social stigma of low intelligence (which is also heritable). A clinical<br />

psychiatrist has observed that when mothers are asked what makes their children tick, they<br />

‘plump for whichever nature or nurture theories most conveniently let them off the hook—<br />

largely, of course, to avoid guilt’ (Oliver James, in The Guardian, 25 September 2002, G2, page<br />

8).<br />

It is probable that there is a white-collar or class bias in reporting dyslexia; it would be very<br />

surprising if there were not. However, reporting bias could be expected in clinic (or special<br />

school) samples, but not in community (epidemiological) samples, because epidemiological<br />

sampling is less likely to be affected by the economic and social factors that bias clinic<br />

referral and special school enrolment. A systematic study has investigated this question in a<br />

community sample, avoiding the use of an IQ-discrepancy formula. It found that the<br />

prevalence of dyslexia—and ‘any attempt to determine the prevalence of dyslexia should be<br />

treated with caution’ (Miles & Miles, 1999)—varied only at the extremes of the social scale<br />

(Miles et al., 1994). Because of the uneven distribution of IQ scores across the social<br />

spectrum (Mackintosh, 1998; Neisser et al., 1996; Sternberg, 2000), a prevalence estimate<br />

based on an IQ-discrepancy operationalisation of the dyslexia concept would necessarily be<br />

higher for middle-class than for working-class groups (e.g. Wadsworth et al., 2000) and this<br />

would reflect identification bias but not cultural bias. Nevertheless, it may be that the literacy<br />

difficulties in most children, even those from middle-class backgrounds, are caused by<br />

experiential and instructional deficits, not by basic deficits in cognitive abilities (Vellutino et<br />

al., 1996).

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!