19.10.2014 Views

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

38<br />

Research Report<br />

function are compelling. Cerebral specialisation depends as much on specific experiences as<br />

on ‘pre-wiring’, so that researchers need to explain a complex pattern of associated<br />

impairments, not a highly selective deficit (Bishop, 1997; Gilger & Kaplan, 20<strong>01</strong>). The<br />

assumption of residual normality against a background of normal development is difficult to<br />

maintain when both compensation and disruption result from initially undamaged cognitive<br />

functions developing in untypical ways (Karmiloff-Smith, 1998; Thomas & Karmiloff-Smith,<br />

2002).<br />

Empirical evidence, too, suggests that the analogy is invalid (Ellis, 1985; Snowling et al., 1996).<br />

An early study reported that developmental dyslexics resembled ‘surface’ but not ‘phonological’<br />

alexics in that they were qualitatively similar to younger, normal readers (Baddeley et al.,<br />

1982). Some later studies have reported a phonological-to-surface continuum (Bryant & Impey,<br />

1986; Ellis et al., 1996; Racket al., 1993), which is also seen in ‘normal’ readers (Bryant &<br />

Impey, 1986; Ellis, 1985) and reports of distinct phonological and surface subtypes of<br />

developmental dyslexia (e.g. Curtin et al., 20<strong>01</strong>) might be explained as outcomes of teaching<br />

method and strategic choice, not as outcomes of biological constraint (Hendriks & Kolk, 1997;<br />

Manis et al., 1996; Murphy & Pollatsek, 1994; Thomson, 1999).<br />

There is further evidence for rejecting the assumption that ‘surface’ and ‘phonological’ dyslexia<br />

are alternative or complementary manifestations of an organic dysfunction. ‘Surface’ dyslexics<br />

are characterised by developmental delay, whereas ‘phonological’ dyslexics are characterised<br />

by a cognitive deficit (Griffiths & Snowling, 2002; Gustafson, 20<strong>01</strong>; Sprenger-Charolles et al.,<br />

2000; Stanovich et al., 1997). Unlike ‘surface dyslexia’, ‘phonological dyslexia’ is associated with<br />

naming-speed deficits (Wolf & Bowers, 1999) and also with deficits in the transient visual<br />

system (Borsting et al., 1996; Spinelli et al., 1997).<br />

In theory, the ‘surface dyslexia’ profile could be caused by any or all of lead exposure, prenatal<br />

exposure to alcohol, influenza viruses and other causes of congenital malformation, or by<br />

intensive instruction in phonological processing (Castles et al., 1999). Empirical evidence<br />

suggests that it might be explained by insufficient exposure to print (Griffiths & Snowling,<br />

2002; Gustafson, 20<strong>01</strong>; Sprenger-Charolles et al., 2000) and lower verbal ability (although not<br />

necessarily less potential for intellectual development). This might offer a parsimonious<br />

explanation for the association between ‘surface dyslexia’ and socio-economic adversity<br />

(Bishop, 20<strong>01</strong>; Samuelsson et al., 2000), since higher and lower IQ groups of reading-disabled<br />

children differ significantly on several home literacy variables, including parental education,<br />

books in the home and being read to (Wadsworth et al., 2000), all of which relate to print<br />

exposure.<br />

Whether reading difficulties are attributable to delay (the ‘surface’ subtype) or to deficit (the<br />

‘phonological’ subtype), they are of equally high priority for intervention. However, if the<br />

‘phonological’ dyslexia profile alone is robust in reading-level comparisons (Sprenger-<br />

Charolles et al., 2000; Stanovich et al., 1997), then perhaps most developmental ‘surface’<br />

dyslexia should be thought of as ordinary poor reading, leaving only the ‘phonological’ subtype<br />

to be properly considered as an organic dysfunction. Such a change would have important<br />

implications for estimates of the prevalence of developmental dyslexia, should it be<br />

conceptualised as a discrete condition. Although this change would have no implications for<br />

estimates of the overall numbers of people with reading skills deficits, it might affect<br />

estimates of the cost and duration of preventive measures and remedial interventions and their<br />

probable outcomes, by a downward adjustment of the numbers estimated to require intensive<br />

remediation over a long period or to need support in other ways.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!