19.10.2014 Views

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

36<br />

Research Report<br />

Thus, if it is a valid observation that ‘standards of clinical practice in the learning disabilities<br />

field have, at various times in its history, reflected more than 50 per cent pseudoscience or<br />

unverified, virtually armchair speculation about human abilities’ (Stanovich, 1999), there can<br />

be scant justification for expecting any consistency in the diagnoses that adult ‘dyslexics’ may<br />

have received as children. The evidence for inconsistency is overwhelming—unsurprisingly,<br />

given the difficulty of operationalising any definition of dyslexia so as to exclude all ordinary<br />

poor readers—and false positives may outnumber true positives severalfold.<br />

Diagnoses of dyslexia are unstable across identification methods.<br />

A diagnosis of dyslexia may be a ticket of eligibility for a particular form of learning<br />

provision rather than a scientific statement.<br />

Are diagnoses of dyslexia stable over time?<br />

In cases where the method of identification is held constant, there is no guarantee that it will<br />

identify the same people as ‘dyslexic’ over time, even though dyslexia is conceptualised as a<br />

lifelong condition. This is particularly likely to be so where identification has been limited to<br />

measures at the behavioural level (Frith, 1999). Several serious problems confront the<br />

researcher or clinician seeking to evaluate adults who report childhood reading and writing<br />

problems: they are often self-selected volunteers, potentially unrepresentative of the<br />

population at large; their elementary school records are not available to document their<br />

school-age reading achievement; diagnostic techniques designed for children may have<br />

norms that are unsuitable for adults; and adults with childhood reading problems might have<br />

adopted compensatory strategies as a result of remedial teaching or self-instruction<br />

(Scarborough, 1984).<br />

So, while the concept of dyslexia assumes that there is a stable group of people who are<br />

underachieving in literacy and whose classification is neither age- nor test-specific, this kind<br />

of stability may not exist (Wright et al., 1996). Some of the apparent change over time may be<br />

attributable to measurement error (Fergusson et al., 1996). As with specific language<br />

impairment (NICHD, 2000), a nonlinear developmental path, with spurts and plateaux, may<br />

lead to unstable perceptions of ‘impairment’ and ‘recovery’. Where the concept of ‘reading<br />

disability’ embraces all poor readers, detailed assessment of ‘recovered’ and ‘non-recovered’<br />

readers has suggested that intelligence, language ability, working memory and the adoption<br />

of a ‘word decoding’ approach to reading and spelling may play a role in ‘recovery’ (Waring et<br />

al., 1996). However, change is not always for the better: in a cohort of children with specific<br />

language impairment, the prevalence of ‘specific reading retardation’ has been found to<br />

increase between the ages of eight-and-a-half and 15 years (Snowling et al., 2000).<br />

Stability of classification is a separate issue within the underachieving group. The question of<br />

subtypes will be examined at greater length in the following section. Meanwhile, it is<br />

sufficient to take note of findings that the characteristics commonly used as a basis for the<br />

classification of developmental dyslexia into ‘phonological’ and ‘surface’ subtypes may reflect<br />

differences in teaching method and strategic choice rather than differences in learning<br />

aptitude (Baron, 1979; Hendriks & Kolk, 1997; Zabell & Everatt, 2002) and that over a twoyear<br />

period, readers have been observed to move from one of these subtypes to the other<br />

(Snowling & Nation, 1997). Proportions in the subtypes may also vary, not only according to<br />

the language in which subjects are assessed but also according to the method used to classify<br />

them and, in a given language, according to the measure used; furthermore, longitudinal data

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!