19.10.2014 Views

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Developmental dyslexia in adults: a research review 35<br />

three times as large as a sample identified in the same population by a cut-off of 2 SD below<br />

the mean on the same measure. Because human traits are distributed in multidimensional<br />

space (Cloninger et al., 1997), the two groups will also differ in kind as well as in size (Ellis,<br />

1985). In neither case will extreme scores indicate a reason for their extremity (Frith, 20<strong>01</strong>).<br />

Groups defined on quantitative measures such as IQ-discrepancy are by their nature<br />

heterogeneous and different patterns of heterogeneity result from different definitions.<br />

Diagnostic criteria inferred from one group may thus be too broad, or too narrow, for the<br />

other group.<br />

Some problems in making IQ-related identifications have been analysed in a study of children<br />

with ‘specific reading retardation’ (Bishop & Butterworth, 1980), a construct which despite its<br />

similarity to ‘dyslexia’ carries no implication as to cause and does not imply a single cause<br />

(Yule & Rutter, 1976). The argument runs like this (Bishop & Butterworth, 1980):<br />

■<br />

■<br />

■<br />

If learners with ‘specific reading retardation’ are defined as those with a nonverbal IQ of 90 or<br />

more who are reading 12 months or more below age level, then, since reading is strongly<br />

related to verbal IQ, poor readers would generally have low verbal IQs. However, since verbal<br />

IQ and nonverbal IQ are positively correlated, most specifically reading-retarded learners<br />

would have low nonverbal IQs, yet the definition would exclude those learners.<br />

If, on the other hand, specifically reading-retarded learners are defined as those with a fullscale<br />

IQ of 90 or more who are reading 12 months below age level, then the poor readers by<br />

this definition (who have a low verbal IQ as poor readers) would have a relatively high<br />

nonverbal IQ and so there would be a larger proportion of learners with verbal-nonverbal<br />

discrepancy among the specifically reading-retarded group.<br />

Alternatively, if specifically reading-retarded learners are defined as those with a verbal IQ of<br />

90 or more who are reading 12 months or more below age level, then there would be no<br />

verbal-nonverbal discrepancy relationship with specific reading retardation. What is more, if<br />

verbal IQ were the basis for selection, it would increase the proportion of males classified as<br />

specifically reading-retarded.<br />

Learners might be most appropriately described as specifically ‘reading-retarded’ only if their<br />

reading ability is disproportionately poor in relation to their verbal (rather than nonverbal or<br />

full-scale) IQ, but such people are rare and their rarity would frustrate researchers (Bishop &<br />

Butterworth, 1980). Nevertheless, to define specific reading retardation in terms of poor<br />

reading relative to nonverbal IQ would include many learners whose reading is quite<br />

consistent with their low verbal ability and could not, on this account, be regarded as<br />

‘unexpected’ (Bishop & Butterworth, 1980).<br />

This is not merely a matter of fine-tuning. In a study in which several different methods of<br />

identification were compared, the criteria for dyslexia were progressively relaxed until every<br />

poor reader was included, at which point the lowest and highest estimates of prevalence were<br />

found to differ by a factor of seven (Snowling et al., 2000).<br />

In brief, people may be ‘dyslexic’ according to one method of identification but not according<br />

to a different method. In as much as ‘dyslexia’ is a construct, the characteristics of ‘dyslexics’<br />

are necessarily artefacts resulting from the identification procedures; they may not<br />

necessarily reflect an innate cognitive dysfunction in the people identified by those<br />

procedures. Most research findings reflect statistical tendencies, not systematic rules; the<br />

given effect is sometimes present in only a minority of subjects and caution is needed in<br />

drawing explanatory models from such studies (Habib, 2000).

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!