19.10.2014 Views

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

34<br />

Research Report<br />

The statistical findings are mixed and do not present irrefutable evidence for differences in<br />

either kind or degree between dyslexic and non-dyslexic people.<br />

The psychometric evidence<br />

Although ‘dyslexics’ are easily shown to differ from good (or even adequate) readers on a<br />

number of reading-related assessments, we need to ask whether the same assessments<br />

differentiate ‘dyslexic’ from ‘non-dyslexic’ (i.e. ordinary) poor readers (Stanovich, 19<strong>88</strong>). It now<br />

seems that a number of reading-related characteristics once thought to be specific to<br />

dyslexia are shared with other novice readers (Ellis, 1985; Taylor et al., 1979). These<br />

characteristics include reversal errors with letters of the alphabet (Fowler et al., 1977; Mann<br />

& Brady, 19<strong>88</strong>; Worthy & Viise, 1996), directional sequencing errors (Vellutino, 1979),<br />

pseudoword reading difficulties (Bishop, 20<strong>01</strong>), phonemic segmentation difficulties (Cole &<br />

Sprenger-Charolles, 1999; Metsala, 1999) and spelling of sight vocabulary (Scarborough,<br />

1984). The characteristics are also shared with neurologically normal people who have had no<br />

opportunity to learn how to read (Castro-Caldas et al., 1998; Kolinsky et al., 1994; Lukatela et<br />

al., 1995; Morais et al., 1979). Moreover, between-group differences on those measures are<br />

also associated with differences between phonics-based and whole-word methods of teaching<br />

reading (Alegria et al., 1982; Thompson & Johnston, 2000). Students tend to perceive words in<br />

the way that they are taught to perceive them (Huey, 1908). This appears to be the case<br />

whether or not they are taught in a transparent orthography (Cardoso-Martins, 20<strong>01</strong>), where<br />

each phoneme is always or almost always represented by the same grapheme, as in Finnish<br />

(always) or Italian (almost always).<br />

Although dyslexic people are believed by some scholars to differ dimensionally rather than<br />

categorically from non-dyslexics (Shaywitz et al., 1992), this may not mean that the most<br />

impaired readers are dyslexic (see also page 61). When IQ-discrepancy criteria have been<br />

used to distinguish dyslexics from ordinary poor readers, the ordinary poor readers have<br />

obtained lower scores than dyslexics on memory span, segmentation, and rhyme tasks<br />

(Fawcett et al., 20<strong>01</strong>) and also on a pseudoword repetition task (Jorm et al., 1986a).<br />

With respect to differences unrelated to reading, a number of apparently interesting<br />

associations between ‘dyslexia’ and other variables (such as blue-collar social status,<br />

inconsistent left-handedness and autoimmune disorders such as hay fever) have been found<br />

to disappear when subject to the scrutiny of multivariate techniques of data analysis (Haslum,<br />

1989). This finding is an example of a general warning seldom heeded in folk psychology:<br />

namely, that simple bivariate correlations do not establish a causal relationship and cannot<br />

determine a causal direction. A more specific warning, against reification, is also appropriate:<br />

‘Categorical diagnoses do not refer to real discrete entities; they are only meaningful as<br />

approximate descriptions that remind the clinician of prominent characteristics of specific<br />

combinations of quantitative traits’ (Cloninger, 2000).<br />

Psychometric findings suggest that differences between dyslexic and non-dyslexic readers<br />

are differences in degree, not differences in kind.<br />

Are diagnoses of dyslexia stable across methods of ascertainment?<br />

A sample of dyslexic people defined by IQ-discrepancy using a cut-off of one standard<br />

deviation (1 SD) below the mean on a continuous measure of decoding skill will be about

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!