19.10.2014 Views

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Developmental dyslexia in adults: a research review 25<br />

Interpretational issues<br />

Do ‘dyslexic’ brains differ from ‘normal’ brains?<br />

Introduction<br />

By now it will be clear, both from the preceding sections on conceptual and methodological<br />

issues and also from Appendixes 1, 2 and 3 (dyslexia definitions and their operationalisation<br />

in research), that the interpretation of evidence for a qualitative distinction between<br />

developmental dyslexia and ‘ordinary’ problems with literacy learning is anything but<br />

straightforward. Could it be true that researchers have ‘misconstrued their object of study—<br />

unexplained underachievement—interpreting it neurologically and ignoring classroom<br />

practices and events’ (Carrier, 1983), so that the theory masks societal forces as they affect<br />

academic performance? Are people justified in believing that the neurological studies validate<br />

dyslexia as a qualitatively distinct condition? How should the neurological evidence be<br />

interpreted? Also, can we afford to forget that ‘the diagnosis of dyslexia is itself a theory,<br />

distinguishing reading failure arising ultimately from internal rather than solely external<br />

reasons, but a rather unspecified one’ (Frith, 20<strong>01</strong>)?<br />

For a long time, most of our knowledge about the workings of the brain was gained in the<br />

course of autopsies on people whose previously normal abilities had been compromised by<br />

head injuries or strokes. These findings suggested that people are born with a brain rather<br />

like a Swiss Army knife, with a modular component for every purpose—an analogy that now<br />

seems false (Karmiloff-Smith, 1992; Quartz & Sejnowski, 1997). Accordingly, loss of function<br />

was not a good guide to what happens in developmental disorders (Alarcón et al., 1999;<br />

Karmiloff-Smith, 1998; Thomas & Karmiloff-Smith, 2002). In any case, there have been very<br />

few autopsy studies of people who had experienced difficulty in learning to read, write and<br />

spell.<br />

Over the past 15 years, a number of brain imaging (or ‘scanning’) techniques have been<br />

introduced. So there are now two sources of evidence for differences between ‘dyslexic’ and<br />

‘normal’ brains. From post-mortem studies, there is evidence of differences in brain<br />

structure; from brain imaging studies of living people (or in vivo studies), there is evidence of<br />

differences in brain structure and function. To date, the in vivo studies are almost without<br />

exception cross-sectional. However, in future, prospective longitudinal imaging studies may<br />

show how brains change as people acquire complex skills.<br />

The table and figures in Appendix 10 may help readers to locate the brain areas named in the<br />

following sub-sections.<br />

Evidence from post-mortem studies<br />

Structurally, the brains in the autopsy studies reveal anomalies at two levels of analysis<br />

(Galaburda et al., 1989). At the microscopic (or neuronal) level, researchers have found<br />

abnormal outgrowths known as ‘ectopias’ and abnormal infoldings known as ‘microgyria’,<br />

visible on the surface of the brain (Galaburda et al., 1989). In addition to these abnormalities<br />

in the outer layer of the brain, or cortex, researchers have found a magnocellular defect<br />

within the inner chamber of the brain, in a part of the thalamus known as the lateral<br />

geniculate nucleus, which is activated during visual processing (Livingstone et al., 1991). At<br />

the macroscopic level, researchers have found an unusual symmetry in that part of the<br />

temporal lobe known as the planum temporale (Galaburda et al., 1989), which is normally<br />

larger in the left hemisphere, where it is activated in tasks related to language, than in the<br />

right hemisphere (Shapleske et al., 1999).

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!