19.10.2014 Views

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

20<br />

Research Report<br />

The British <strong>Dyslexia</strong> Association’s prevalence estimates of either 4 per cent for severe<br />

dyslexia or 10 per cent to include mild dyslexia have been described as ‘both theoretically and<br />

technically contentious’ (Pumfrey, 20<strong>01</strong>). Lower, but possibly no less contentious, are the<br />

estimate of 2.29 per cent for (what was then called) ‘specific reading retardation’ obtained<br />

from the Child Health and Education Study cohort (Rodgers, 1983) and the figure of 2.08 per<br />

cent given not as a prevalence estimate but as representing ‘the children in the cohort about<br />

whose dyslexia one can have the most confidence’ (Miles et al., 1993). In the original<br />

standardisation sample for the British Ability Scales (BAS), 8.5 per cent were defined as<br />

showing general reading backwardness, while a further 3.2 per cent were defined as showing<br />

‘specific reading retardation’ (Tyler & Elliott, 19<strong>88</strong>). Because ‘specific reading retardation’ is a<br />

wider concept than developmental dyslexia, the prevalence of dyslexia must have been less<br />

than 3.2 per cent in the BAS standardisation sample.<br />

However, where dyslexics are identified by IQ-discrepancy methods, prevalence estimates<br />

may be particularly sensitive to teaching techniques; reading difficulty rates are higher with<br />

whole-language methods than with systematic and explicit phonics and highest of all among<br />

socially disadvantaged learners (Chapman et al., 20<strong>01</strong>; Nicholson, 1997). With all such<br />

estimates, unstable diagnoses raise further doubts about their reliability (Badian, 1999;<br />

Haslum, 1989).<br />

It may well be that in asking about prevalence we are posing an inappropriate question. If we<br />

take the perspective that ‘developmental disorders are not diseases that one does or does not<br />

have but are behaviorally defined dimensional traits along a continuum with fuzzy edges and a<br />

wide range of severity’ (Rapin, 2002), then questions about prevalence are misconceived;<br />

‘there is no crisp partition between normalcy and disorder … even when there is no<br />

controversy regarding the identification of prototypic exemplars’ (Rapin, 2002).<br />

None of these difficulties should worry adult literacy tutors. If a reliable estimate of the<br />

prevalence of dyslexia in the general population were ever available, tutors should not expect<br />

to find the same rate among their students, nor is it likely that any student rate would be<br />

stable over place, time and first-language status.<br />

Adult literacy tutors may nevertheless appreciate a word of warning about the high prevalence<br />

sometimes reported for dyslexia among offenders (e.g. Alm & Andersson, 1997; Davies &<br />

Byatt, 1998; Kirk & Reid, 20<strong>01</strong>; Morgan, 1997; Turner et al., 2000). As the studies in question<br />

do not differentiate dyslexics from ordinary poor readers, their prevalence estimates must be<br />

inflated (Rice, 2000; Samuelsson et al., 2003; Snowling et al., 2000). A history of childhood<br />

disadvantage is more frequent among prisoners than it is in the general population (Dodd &<br />

Hunter, 1992; Social Exclusion Unit, 2002); between childhood disadvantage and low literacy,<br />

a causal association is well-documented (Cox & Jones, 1983; Davie et al., 1972; Kolvin et al.,<br />

1990; MacKay, 1999; Mortimore & Blackstone, 1982; Nicholson, 1997; Richman, Stevenson, &<br />

Graham, 1982; Tough, 1982). Accordingly, it is reasonable to expect that prisoners’ reading<br />

skills will reflect their social circumstances and ability levels (Putnins, 1999), which is what<br />

systematic enquiries have found (Black et al., 1990; Haigler et al., 1994).<br />

In passing, tutors are advised that the frequently cited ALBSU study is an unreliable guide to<br />

prisoner literacy, because of its haphazard sampling and poor data quality — problems that<br />

were not reported on publication (ALBSU, 1994).<br />

After social and educational background variables have been taken into account, there

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!