19.10.2014 Views

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

01 NRDC Dyslexia 1-88 update - Texthelp

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Developmental dyslexia in adults: a research review 19<br />

People can be described as ‘dyslexic’ on a variety of criteria, not all of which are compatible.<br />

What kind of causal process might result in dyslexia?<br />

Once more, the theoretical pathway divides. There might be either one cause or more than<br />

one cause of ‘dyslexia’. If there is only one cause, it has to be either biological or experiential.<br />

However, if there is more than one cause, the causes might be either biological, or<br />

experiential, or part biological and part experiential. If there is more than one cause, the<br />

causes might take effect separately or in combination. If the causes take effect in<br />

combination, they might do so independently, in a ‘main effect’ model; or one cause might<br />

mediate the effect of another cause, whether by exacerbating it or by alleviating it, in a static<br />

‘interactional’ model; or there might be continuing and progressive interplay over time<br />

between biological and experiential causes, in a dynamic ‘transactional’ model (Gottlieb &<br />

Halpern, 2002; Gottlieb et al., 1998; Sameroff & Chandler, 1975).<br />

These causes may take effect at different developmental stages. They may be immediate or<br />

remote, with respect to learning how the alphabet encodes the spoken language. A cause at<br />

one stage in development might be supplemented by a second cause at a later stage. One<br />

cause might mediate the effect of another. At every stage from conception onwards, some<br />

event might affect a person’s acquisition of alphabetic skills, to their benefit or detriment.<br />

There can be many different courses of individual development before a learner reaches the<br />

final common pathway of failure to acquire alphabetic skills at the expected age and with a<br />

fluency that makes reading for meaning both informative and enjoyable.<br />

In short, there may be no single causal process. If it turns out that there is indeed no single<br />

causal process, then no single theory of reading failure can explain every individual failure.<br />

Indeed, it might be that no single theory could fully explain any individual failure or, at the<br />

extreme, that a fully-developed theory might explain one case only.<br />

This analysis has used the metaphor of a dividing pathway. In an ideal world, there would be<br />

only a single pathway. On the landscape of ‘dyslexia’, however, there are many pathways, and<br />

it seems that almost every one is—rightly or wrongly— signposted to our destination.<br />

Samples of the evidence for these observations can be found in Appendices 1 and 2.<br />

<strong>Dyslexia</strong> might be a final pathway common to many causal processes.<br />

What is the prevalence of dyslexia?<br />

Although people sometimes speak of the ‘incidence’ of dyslexia, incidence is the frequency<br />

with which new cases occur and are diagnosed over a period of time (Barker et al., 1998). The<br />

proportion of dyslexic individuals in a population at any one time is properly called the<br />

‘prevalence rate’ (Everitt & Wykes, 1999), although ‘prevalence’ is the term in general use.<br />

The previous section implies that any estimate of the prevalence of dyslexia will reflect the<br />

chosen definition and how it is operationalised. No two of those definitions could possibly<br />

identify the same individuals, or the same number of individuals, in any population. It has<br />

been shown that prevalence estimates for dyslexia are susceptible to definitional<br />

manipulation over a wide range (Snowling et al., 2000a). In the absence of a definition that<br />

provides unequivocal identification criteria, all statements about prevalence are guesses; they<br />

are value judgements, not scientific facts (Kavale & Forness, 2000). All the same, there can be<br />

no doubt that the higher the estimate is, the more likely it will be to confound dyslexia with<br />

ordinary reading difficulty.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!