12.09.2014 Views

The Category P Features, Projections, Interpretation

The Category P Features, Projections, Interpretation

The Category P Features, Projections, Interpretation

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

23<br />

1999). 10 It is easy to demonstrate that the notions etymological source and<br />

derivational input are fundamentally distinct. For example, the English preposition<br />

past (e.g. <strong>The</strong> horse raced past the barn) is claimed to be related to the verb passed<br />

(Jespersen 1924). Note, however, that the form of the verb is already an inflected<br />

form (-ed [+past]), which is very unusual as a basis for a morphological derivation.<br />

As an additional example consider the Hebrew preposition li-fney (‘before’, ‘in front<br />

of’), which is related to the noun panim (‘face’). Note, however, that rather than being<br />

related to the free form panim (e.g. ha-panim šel ha-yalda (‘the face of the girl’)), lifney<br />

is related to the construct state (CS) form of panim, namely pney (e.g. pney hayalda<br />

(‘[the] face [of] the girl’)). 11 Furthermore, despite its nominal source, li-fney can<br />

combine with its complement forming a CS only: li-fnei ha-mesiba vs. *le-panim šel<br />

ha-mesiba (‘before of the party’), as opposed to the majority of Hebrew Ns for which<br />

construct or free state combinations are interchangeable (Siloni 2002). <strong>The</strong> noted nonflexionality<br />

of P is arguably closely related to the absence of prepositional<br />

morphology. More specifically, there are no derivational affixes that subcategorize for<br />

Ps, on a par with affixes such as –ness, -ity in English, for instance, which<br />

subcategorize for adjectives to form nouns (e.g. happiness, sincerity). Consequently,<br />

nouns, verbs or adjectives are not derived systematically from prepositions (and vice<br />

versa). 12<br />

Ps can be affixal or null: Whether P can be phonetically null (namely a set of<br />

abstract features) is a matter of analysis: Emonds (1985) argues that all semantic<br />

Cases are achieved through an empty P; Kayne (1984) proposes that structurally<br />

governing Ps, such as the Dative to in English, can be phonetically null; Den Dikken<br />

(1995) defines the circumstances that allow P to be phonetically null. Note that only<br />

the functional heads, C, T, D, are widely assumed to be present in the syntactic<br />

structure, regardless of their phonetic realization. Thus the analyses just mentioned<br />

are consistent with the hypothesis that P is a functional category.<br />

10 Probably due to their various sources, there are languages where (some) prepositions seem to behave<br />

as a subclass of verbs, adjectives or nouns (e.g. Navajo (Hale and Paltero 1986); Hebrew (Siloni<br />

2002); Modern Greek (Terzi 2001)).<br />

11 <strong>The</strong> p/f alternation is due to Hebrew spirantization, p being the underlying phoneme and f its<br />

allomorph.<br />

12 In some languages such as German and Dutch prepositions are used quite extensively in<br />

compounding with verbs (Zwarts 1997). Crucially though, they never constitute the head of the<br />

compound (unlike lexical categories, which may head the compound they are part of).

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!