09.09.2014 Views

Lightness and Brightness and Other Confusions

Lightness and Brightness and Other Confusions

Lightness and Brightness and Other Confusions

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

tions for mapping colours of light, visible to the average human, in a way that<br />

allows their reliable scientific definition <strong>and</strong> comparison. Another starting<br />

point for the work was Hermann Grassmann’s (1809–1877) three laws of<br />

additive colour mixture. 81 This simple psychophysical law laid the ground for<br />

the colour matching experiments involving three ‘primary lights’. As Fred<br />

Billmayer <strong>and</strong> Max Saltzman say in their book Principles of Color Technology,<br />

there is nothing magical about these monochromatic lights. The CIE<br />

could have chosen any other set of colours, provided that they are sufficiently<br />

wide apart in the spectrum. (Billmayer & Saltzman 1981, p 40; Kuehni 1983, p<br />

73). 82 In fact there is no set of three primary colours with which it is possible<br />

to mix all possible colours visible to the human eye. So, in order to derive a<br />

system for describing all colours as perceived by humans that was based on<br />

three primaries, negative values were assigned to some of the colours in the<br />

equation. This led to the designation of the three ‘imaginary primaries’ X, Y<br />

<strong>and</strong> Z. They became imaginary, because in the geometry of the CIE Yxy colour<br />

space, for mathematical reasons, they had to be placed outside the gamut of<br />

human colour vision.<br />

The history of the CIE-colour spaces (there are several) is one of transformations,<br />

technical compromises <strong>and</strong> abstractions. Therefore the so-called RGBprimaries,<br />

which are very much a child of this enterprise, should be treated<br />

with the same coolness as any other technological colour application. There<br />

may be a set of three primary lights that excite the colour sensitive cones<br />

maximally, but four or five would do it even better. The CIE 1931 Yxy colour<br />

space should therefore not be regarded as a model of how the human colour<br />

vision system functions. Neither should it be used as source for drawing conclusions<br />

about how many <strong>and</strong> what are the primary colours – or at least not<br />

outside the logic of the CIE system.<br />

<br />

Grassmann’s First Law: Any colour can be matched by a linear combination of three<br />

other colours, provided that none of those three could be matched by a combination of<br />

the other two. Second Law: A mixture of any two colours can be matched by linearly<br />

adding together the mixtures of any three other colours that individually match the two<br />

source colours. Third Law: Colour matching persists at all luminances. This law fails at<br />

very low light levels, where scotopic vision (rod-receptors) takes over from photopic<br />

vision (cone receptors).<br />

In this sense the primaries underlying the CIE 1931 Yxy space are somewhat like<br />

ordnance survey points in triangulation: one could start anywhere <strong>and</strong> still end up with<br />

an accurate measurement of the terrain.<br />

83

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!