09.09.2014 Views

Lightness and Brightness and Other Confusions

Lightness and Brightness and Other Confusions

Lightness and Brightness and Other Confusions

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

elation to the light source or observer. This creates an uneven distribution of<br />

luminances even when the illuminance is even – as for instance in spherical<br />

objects that tend to display deep shadows <strong>and</strong> bright highlights at the same<br />

time. The same is true, of course, for any shape of three-dimensional object in<br />

normal directional light.<br />

We are able to separate the forms of objects surrounding us from the everchanging<br />

patterns of light <strong>and</strong> shadow. Furthermore we are able separate the<br />

patterns of illuminance from the patterns of luminance. In all but the most<br />

confusing cases we are able, without effort, to separate the colours of highlight,<br />

shadow <strong>and</strong> half-shadow in a spherical object from what we experience<br />

as the ‘real colour’ or ´substance colour’ <strong>and</strong> the ‘real lightness’ of the object<br />

itself. 69 The same goes for a flat surface in dappled or uneven light. We do not<br />

say or think that a white wall lit in this way has different lightnesses, but that<br />

it is lit unevenly. In the language of perceptual science, it has different<br />

brightnesses. The confusion in the use of the terms lightness <strong>and</strong> brightness<br />

comes from their everyday use. <strong>Brightness</strong> is in most languages limited to<br />

describing phenomenally self-luminous objects <strong>and</strong> things, such as the sun,<br />

the sky, artificial light sources, but it can also describe ambient or incident<br />

light: a bright day, a bright room, etc. <strong>Brightness</strong> is also in everyday speech of<br />

many languages used to describe the vividness of colour (a bright pink dress,<br />

a bright blue flower), which confuses the issue even further. Since our capacity<br />

to apprehend the lightness of objects despite their varying brightness<br />

(or luminances) is very robust <strong>and</strong> requires no conscious effort (we are indeed<br />

mostly unaware of this separation), there has been little need to develop<br />

separate words or separate ways of using the word brightness to describe a)<br />

the variations of illuminance <strong>and</strong> b) variations of luminance of our surroundings.<br />

It is only when we pause to take a closer look at the nature of seeing,<br />

that this need arises. 70<br />

The human visual system has also a remarkable capacity for adaptation to<br />

intensities of light, both globally <strong>and</strong> locally. 71 If this were not so, we would<br />

<br />

See also the section The Real colour of objects, pp. 92–100, in this article.<br />

<br />

In everyday speech we do not refer to two walls painted in the same colour in the<br />

manner: ”The wall in next room is the same white colour, although less bright”. Neither<br />

do we often say: ”The wall was of a uniform brown, although you could never see it as<br />

such, because of its varying brightness, due to the pattern of shadows falling on it”. We<br />

apprehend the reflectances as identical <strong>and</strong> uniform, despite the variations in luminance<br />

– even to the point of being unaware of the luminance variation. For further<br />

discussion about modes of attention see Merleau-Ponty (2002), pp 30–59, as well as<br />

the section Identity colour <strong>and</strong> modes of attention in this article <strong>and</strong> Natural Experiences<br />

<strong>and</strong> Physical Abstractions, pp 23–24 in this volume.<br />

<br />

<br />

The human visual system is capable of producing perceptions over an amazing luminance<br />

range of 1–10 000 000 000 000 (ten trillion) cd/m 2 , representing at one end the<br />

absorption of one photon in a rod receptor (enough to create a tiny flash) <strong>and</strong> at the<br />

74

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!