09.09.2014 Views

Lightness and Brightness and Other Confusions

Lightness and Brightness and Other Confusions

Lightness and Brightness and Other Confusions

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

What Gregory is saying is that at the extreme edges of the retina the proximal<br />

stimulus created no experience of anything, yet it triggered a reflex, an action<br />

of rotating the eyes <strong>and</strong> head to bring the stimulus into the domain of seeing.<br />

Gregory also points out that the prehistory of our vision is thus “embalmed in<br />

our retinas”. (Ibid.) The cellular structures <strong>and</strong> processes being more primitive<br />

the further they are from the fovea. He also reminds us “… it is eyes quite<br />

high up in the evolutionary scale that produce signals in the absence of<br />

movement”. (Ibid.) This would suggest that a proximal stimulus that triggers<br />

an automatic reflex, but does not enter consciousness as an experience, does<br />

not qualify for seeing.<br />

Attention<br />

Very often we have the experience of not seeing something that we know or<br />

we are told is (or was) in our field of vision. Sometimes we fail to see things<br />

happening before our eyes because our attention is focused on other things.<br />

These distracting signals can be local parts of the global visual stimulus or<br />

non-visual percepts, such as sounds or thoughts. ”Did you not notice that car<br />

coming from the left?” asks the driving-instructor. ”No, I didn’t see it at all!”<br />

replies the alarmed student, although the car (the distal stimulus) has caused,<br />

without him knowing, millions of retinal <strong>and</strong> brain cells to fire in his head.<br />

The student failed to pay attention to this particular visual percept, because<br />

he was concentrating on other, perhaps even more pressing, stimuli or<br />

thoughts. Our eyes <strong>and</strong> brains are being continually bombarded with potential<br />

information about our surroundings <strong>and</strong> our relation to them. The act of<br />

seeing is the effort to filter the relevant visual information from the irrelevant.<br />

Looking is the act of attending to parts or levels of what is seen. Visual attention<br />

has many levels <strong>and</strong> forms. If seeing is visual experience – <strong>and</strong> therefore<br />

part of consciousness – we then require attention for seeing.<br />

There is a very famous <strong>and</strong> quite entertaining demonstration, The Invisible<br />

Gorilla by Christopher Chabris <strong>and</strong> Daniel Simons (1999), which illustrates<br />

the selective power of attention. 41 On first viewing, many people do not notice<br />

the gorilla amidst the basketball game. In fact they report that they did not<br />

see it, <strong>and</strong> when the video is replayed to them many insist that it has been<br />

changed (Chabris <strong>and</strong> Simons 1999). In other words, they had no experience<br />

of the gorilla moving among the players, even though the stimulus must have<br />

created all kinds of neural firing in their brains. One could also say that they<br />

did not register the gorilla. The failure to see the gorilla resulted from the<br />

observers concentrated attention to other features in the video. The message<br />

of the demonstration is: this is happening to us all the time. We fail so see<br />

large parts of the potential visual information reaching our brains.<br />

41<br />

See: www.theinvisiblegorilla.com/gorilla_experiment.html<br />

40

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!