09.09.2014 Views

Lightness and Brightness and Other Confusions

Lightness and Brightness and Other Confusions

Lightness and Brightness and Other Confusions

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

each apple. We see that the farthest apple is the same colour as the other two,<br />

even though it is in shadow while the other two are in stronger light. We see<br />

that all the apples are (approximately) spherical, although one apple is occluded<br />

by another so that only a part of it is visible. We also see that the apples<br />

are all the same size, although the retinal image of the farthest apple is<br />

smaller than that of the nearest one. However, due to the visual phenomenon<br />

of constancy scaling, they are seen to be more similar in size than they would<br />

be in a photograph or classical perspective image of the same scene.<br />

Figure 4. Two interpretations of the same three apples.<br />

(Images: Harald Arnkil)<br />

The above scene could be translated into a flat image in several ways 40 , revealing<br />

the multiple levels of seeing. One artist might concentrate on the<br />

apples’ chromatic differences in the lights, highlights <strong>and</strong> shadows, another<br />

one might ignore them <strong>and</strong> depict them as uniform green. A photorealist<br />

would make a careful note of the optic size difference. Another artist might<br />

paint all three apples the same size in accordance with constancy scaling <strong>and</strong><br />

knowledge of the apples’ physical similarity. And so on. All depictions would<br />

be correct <strong>and</strong> accurate in their own way, but if displayed side by side in an<br />

exhibition, they would appear strikingly different. Viewers, who were familiar<br />

with the original scene, might be surprised (or delighted) by the different<br />

‘interpretations’. They might find that they have missed out on something<br />

that one of the artists has emphasized or made apparent. Like artists of a<br />

given school, we are all – to a certain degree – ‘trained’ to notice <strong>and</strong> to see<br />

particular aspects of our visual environment. This occurs mainly through<br />

shifts of focus <strong>and</strong> attention that are directed by our intentions. A botanist<br />

might be trained to notice minute variations of colour in greenery or a fisherman<br />

to forecast the weather from a combination of colours <strong>and</strong> forms in<br />

sky, clouds <strong>and</strong> water. But it is highly unlikely that the Ukiyo-e -artists of 18 th<br />

– 19 th Century Japan did not see shadows or the converging optical effects of<br />

40 The idea, developed during the Renaissance <strong>and</strong> after, that the perspective image is<br />

the ultimate scientific interpretation of space, is of course based on the idea of a ‘retinal<br />

image’. Translating space experience into a flat image by using perspective was then a<br />

task of figuring out how that retinal image is created – a matter of optical science. (See<br />

Kemp 1990).<br />

37

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!