Leadership styles, mentoring functions received, and job-related stress

Leadership styles, mentoring functions received, and job-related stress Leadership styles, mentoring functions received, and job-related stress

son.washington.edu
from son.washington.edu More from this publisher
16.07.2014 Views

Journal of Organizational Behavior J. Organiz. Behav. 21, 365±390 (2000) Leadership styles, mentoring functions received, and job-related stress: a conceptual model and preliminary study JOHN J. SOSIK*{ AND VERONICA M. GODSHALK Department of Management and Organization, Great Valley School of Graduate Professional Studies, The Pennsylvania State University, Malvern, PA 19355, U.S.A. Summary This research examined linkages between mentor leadership behaviors (laissez-faire, transactional contingent reward, and transformational), prote ge perception of mentoring functions received (career development and psychosocial support) and jobrelated stress of 204 mentor±prote ge dyads. Results of Partial Least Squares analysis revealed that mentor transformational behavior was more positively related to mentoring functions received than transactional contingent reward behavior, while mentor laissez-faire behavior was negatively related to mentoring functions received. Both mentor transformational behavior and mentoring functions received were negatively related to prote ge job-related stress. The relationship between mentor transformational behavior and prote ge job-related stress was moderated by the level of mentoring functions received. Results are discussed as they relate to researchers and practitioners who are becoming interested in ®nding ways to develop organizational members and allay job-related stress. Copyright # 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Introduction Increased complexity in organizational work environments has given rise to higher levels of job-related stress experienced by organizational members (Brockner et al., 1992; Champy, 1995). Job-related stress is de®ned as an uncomfortable and undesirable feeling experienced by an individual `who is required to deviate from normal or self-desired functioning in the work place as the result of opportunities, constraints, or demands relating to potentially important workrelated outcomes' (Parker and DeCotiis, 1983, p. 165). Job-related stress has been linked to failing individual health and illness (Kram and Hall, 1989), decreased individual performance (Jamal, 1990; Motowidlo, Packard and Manning, 1986), decreased organizational e€ectiveness (Beehr and Newman, 1978; Motowidlo et al., 1986), and increased organizational health care costs (Manning, Jackson and Fusilier, 1996). In fact, job-related stress has been estimated to cost corporate America $200 billion annually in absenteeism, lost productivity, accidents, and medical * Correspondence to: John J. Sosik, Department of Management and Organization, Great Valley School of Graduate Professional Studies, The Pennsylvania State University, Malvern, PA 19355, U.S.A. Tel.: (610) 648 3254. E-mail: jjs20@psu.edu { Author note: An earlier version of this paper was presented at the 1997 Academy of Management Conference in Boston, Massachusetts. Copyright # 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Revised 20 June 1997 Accepted 18 August 1998

Journal of Organizational Behavior<br />

J. Organiz. Behav. 21, 365±390 (2000)<br />

<strong>Leadership</strong> <strong>styles</strong>, <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong><br />

<strong>received</strong>, <strong>and</strong> <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong>:<br />

a conceptual model <strong>and</strong> preliminary study<br />

JOHN J. SOSIK*{ AND VERONICA M. GODSHALK<br />

Department of Management <strong>and</strong> Organization, Great Valley School of Graduate Professional Studies,<br />

The Pennsylvania State University, Malvern, PA 19355, U.S.A.<br />

Summary<br />

This research examined linkages between mentor leadership behaviors (laissez-faire,<br />

transactional contingent reward, <strong>and</strong> transformational), prote ge perception of<br />

<strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong> (career development <strong>and</strong> psychosocial support) <strong>and</strong> <strong>job</strong><strong>related</strong><br />

<strong>stress</strong> of 204 mentor±prote ge dyads. Results of Partial Least Squares analysis<br />

revealed that mentor transformational behavior was more positively <strong>related</strong> to <strong>mentoring</strong><br />

<strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong> than transactional contingent reward behavior, while mentor<br />

laissez-faire behavior was negatively <strong>related</strong> to <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong>. Both<br />

mentor transformational behavior <strong>and</strong> <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong> were negatively<br />

<strong>related</strong> to prote ge <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong>. The relationship between mentor transformational<br />

behavior <strong>and</strong> prote ge <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong> was moderated by the level of <strong>mentoring</strong><br />

<strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong>. Results are discussed as they relate to researchers <strong>and</strong> practitioners<br />

who are becoming interested in ®nding ways to develop organizational members <strong>and</strong><br />

allay <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong>. Copyright # 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.<br />

Introduction<br />

Increased complexity in organizational work environments has given rise to higher levels of<br />

<strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong> experienced by organizational members (Brockner et al., 1992; Champy, 1995).<br />

Job-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong> is de®ned as an uncomfortable <strong>and</strong> undesirable feeling experienced by an<br />

individual `who is required to deviate from normal or self-desired functioning in the work place<br />

as the result of opportunities, constraints, or dem<strong>and</strong>s relating to potentially important work<strong>related</strong><br />

outcomes' (Parker <strong>and</strong> DeCotiis, 1983, p. 165). Job-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong> has been linked to<br />

failing individual health <strong>and</strong> illness (Kram <strong>and</strong> Hall, 1989), decreased individual performance<br />

(Jamal, 1990; Motowidlo, Packard <strong>and</strong> Manning, 1986), decreased organizational e€ectiveness<br />

(Beehr <strong>and</strong> Newman, 1978; Motowidlo et al., 1986), <strong>and</strong> increased organizational health care<br />

costs (Manning, Jackson <strong>and</strong> Fusilier, 1996). In fact, <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong> has been estimated to cost<br />

corporate America $200 billion annually in absenteeism, lost productivity, accidents, <strong>and</strong> medical<br />

* Correspondence to: John J. Sosik, Department of Management <strong>and</strong> Organization, Great Valley School of<br />

Graduate Professional Studies, The Pennsylvania State University, Malvern, PA 19355, U.S.A. Tel.: (610) 648 3254.<br />

E-mail: jjs20@psu.edu<br />

{ Author note: An earlier version of this paper was presented at the 1997 Academy of Management Conference in<br />

Boston, Massachusetts.<br />

Copyright # 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Revised 20 June 1997<br />

Accepted 18 August 1998


366 J. J. SOSIK AND V. M. GODSHALK<br />

insurance (Gibson, 1993; Laws, 1996). Moreover, <strong>stress</strong>-<strong>related</strong> costs may approximate 10 per<br />

cent of the U.S. Gross National Product (Sullivan <strong>and</strong> Bhagat, 1992). At issue for organizations<br />

is how to reduce <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong> <strong>and</strong> its detrimental consequences.<br />

Mentoring is a form of social support which may allay <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong> of organizational<br />

members (House, 1981; Kram <strong>and</strong> Hall, 1989). Mentoring is de®ned as `a deliberate pairing of a<br />

more skilled or experienced person with a lesser skilled or experienced one, with the agreed-upon<br />

goal of having the lesser skilled person grow <strong>and</strong> develop speci®c competencies' (Murray, 1991,<br />

p. xiv). Mentors provide both career development <strong>and</strong> psychosocial support <strong>functions</strong> to prote ge s<br />

(Kram, 1985; Noe, 1988). Psychosocial support <strong>functions</strong> include acceptance, role modelling,<br />

coaching, <strong>and</strong> counselling. These <strong>functions</strong> parallel leadership behaviors identi®ed by Yukl (1990)<br />

such as supporting, motivating <strong>and</strong> inspiring, <strong>and</strong> developing. Career development <strong>functions</strong><br />

include sponsorship, protection, challenging assignments, exposure, <strong>and</strong> visibility. These <strong>functions</strong><br />

parallel leadership behaviors identi®ed by Yukl (1990) such as clarifying roles <strong>and</strong> objectives,<br />

monitoring, <strong>and</strong> networking. Prior research (e.g., Bass, 1998; House, 1996; Sc<strong>and</strong>ura <strong>and</strong><br />

Schriesheim, 1994; Yukl, 1994) suggests that mentors may exhibit a variety of leadership behaviors<br />

or <strong>styles</strong> while interacting with prote ge s. <strong>Leadership</strong> style is de®ned here as acts or behaviors<br />

exhibited by the mentor which in¯uence prote ge s (Bass, 1990). A key question, however, is what<br />

leadership <strong>styles</strong>/behaviors distinguish mentors who are inclined to foster <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong><br />

e€ective in allaying prote ge <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong> from those who are less inclined to do so. In<br />

answering this question, direct implications for recruitment, selection, <strong>and</strong> training of mentors can<br />

be found. For example, if key leadership behaviors which allay <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong> via <strong>mentoring</strong> can<br />

be identi®ed, then individuals who exhibit these behaviors can be selected or trained as mentors.<br />

Scholars in organizational behavior (e.g., Bass, 1990, 1998; Yukl, 1994) have encouraged<br />

researchers to explore relationships between mentor leadership <strong>styles</strong>, <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong>, <strong>and</strong><br />

work-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong>. Yet no research has focused on how mentor leadership style(s) <strong>and</strong> the<br />

development of mentor±prote ge relationships may a€ect <strong>stress</strong> as experienced by the prote ge .<br />

Given that <strong>mentoring</strong> programmes have gained increased importance in today's competitive<br />

business environment (Burke, McKenna <strong>and</strong> McKeen, 1991; Sc<strong>and</strong>ura, 1992), it appears<br />

necessary to investigate how mentor leadership style(s) may in¯uence the e€ectiveness of <strong>mentoring</strong><br />

<strong>functions</strong> in allaying perceived <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong>. Accordingly, this paper focuses on adding to<br />

our underst<strong>and</strong>ing of <strong>mentoring</strong> behavior by identifying from the leadership literature behaviors<br />

that mentors could use to be more e€ective in their <strong>mentoring</strong> roles.<br />

This paper extends prior work in three ways. First, it examines the conceptual similarities <strong>and</strong><br />

di€erences between leadership <strong>and</strong> <strong>mentoring</strong>. Second, it presents a theoretical model which<br />

integrates aspects of the leadership, <strong>mentoring</strong>, <strong>and</strong> occupational <strong>stress</strong> literatures. Third, it<br />

examines whether transformational leadership (cf. Bass, 1985; Burns, 1978) has a more favorable<br />

e€ect on <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong>, directly <strong>and</strong> via <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong>, as compared to other leadership<br />

<strong>styles</strong> such as laissez-faire <strong>and</strong> transactional contingent reward. No previous published work has<br />

explored these linkages.<br />

<strong>Leadership</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>mentoring</strong><br />

The literatures on leadership (e.g., Bass, 1990; Yukl, 1990, 1994) <strong>and</strong> <strong>mentoring</strong> (e.g., Kram,<br />

1985; Noe, 1988) have established similarities between leadership <strong>and</strong> <strong>mentoring</strong>. For example,<br />

Schein (1978) described leaders as creators <strong>and</strong> manipulators of culture, while Wilson <strong>and</strong> Elman<br />

Copyright # 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 21, 365±390 (2000)


LEADERSHIP, MENTORING AND STRESS 367<br />

(1990) described mentors as transfer agents of culture. Gladstone (1988) argued that mentors<br />

behave as leaders when they shape values, act as an example, <strong>and</strong> de®ne meanings for prote ge s.<br />

Thibodeaux <strong>and</strong> Lowe (1996) found convergence of in-group Leader±Member Exchange (LMX)<br />

relations <strong>and</strong> <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong>.<br />

At the same time, the leadership <strong>and</strong> <strong>mentoring</strong> literatures have established several di€erences<br />

between the two constructs. First, leadership involves a performance-oriented in¯uence process,<br />

while <strong>mentoring</strong> involves a long-term role-model relationship which is primarily career <strong>and</strong><br />

development-oriented (Burke et al., 1991). Second, leadership involves one leader <strong>and</strong> one or<br />

more followers, whereas <strong>mentoring</strong> usually involves one mentor <strong>and</strong> one prote ge . <strong>Leadership</strong><br />

may be a more formal, overt, <strong>and</strong> direct in¯uence process, while <strong>mentoring</strong> may be a more<br />

informal, subtle, <strong>and</strong> indirect in¯uence process (Appelbaum, Ritchie <strong>and</strong> Shapiro, 1994). Graen<br />

<strong>and</strong> Sc<strong>and</strong>ura (1986) suggested that leadership is distinct from <strong>mentoring</strong> but e€ective LMX<br />

relations may be a function of being mentored by a leader. Third, not all experienced leaders<br />

become e€ective mentors (Ragins <strong>and</strong> Cotton, 1993). Fourth, empirical distinction between<br />

leadership <strong>and</strong> <strong>mentoring</strong> has been found by Sc<strong>and</strong>ura <strong>and</strong> Schriesheim (1994), Eisenbach (1992,<br />

`An exploration of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>and</strong> leadership as inter<strong>related</strong> concepts'. Unpublished doctoral<br />

dissertation, University of Miami), <strong>and</strong> Morgan (1989).<br />

Given the ambiguity regarding the distinctiveness of leadership <strong>and</strong> <strong>mentoring</strong>, we relied upon<br />

the work of Yukl (1990, 1994) <strong>and</strong> Noe (1988) to propose approximate conceptual similarities<br />

<strong>and</strong> distinctions between speci®c leadership behaviors <strong>and</strong> <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> shown in<br />

Table 1. Yukl (1990) developed a taxonomy of leader behavior which integrates several earlier<br />

taxonomies (e.g., Mintzberg, 1973; Stogdill, 1963) of initiating structure (i.e., task-oriented) <strong>and</strong><br />

consideration (i.e., relationship-oriented) leader behaviors. Some leader behaviors are primarily<br />

task-oriented (e.g., planning <strong>and</strong> organizing, problem solving, clarifying roles <strong>and</strong> objectives,<br />

monitoring), while others are primarily relationship oriented (i.e., supporting, developing,<br />

networking, recognizing). Yukl (1994) argued that certain leader behaviors include some<br />

Table 1. Approximate conceptual similarities <strong>and</strong> distinctions between leadership behaviors <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong><br />

Leader behavior Primary behavior Mentoring function (Noe, 1988)<br />

(Yukl, 1990) orientation Career development Psychosocial support<br />

Planning <strong>and</strong> organizing Task Coaching<br />

Problem solving<br />

Task<br />

Clarifying roles <strong>and</strong> objectives Task Challenging assignments Role modelling<br />

Informing<br />

Task<br />

Monitoring Task Protection<br />

Motivating <strong>and</strong> inspiring Mixed Role modelling<br />

Consulting<br />

Mixed<br />

Delegating<br />

Mixed<br />

Supporting Relationship Counselling<br />

Developing <strong>and</strong> <strong>mentoring</strong> Relationship Sponsorship<br />

Coaching<br />

Challenging assignments<br />

Managing con¯ict <strong>and</strong><br />

Relationship<br />

team building<br />

Networking Relationship Exposure/visibility<br />

Sponsorship<br />

Recognizing Relationship Acceptance/con®rmation<br />

Rewarding Relationship Acceptance/con®rmation<br />

Note. Primary behavior orientation based on Yukl (1994). Speci®c <strong>mentoring</strong> function indicates area of overlap with<br />

leadership behavior.<br />

Copyright # 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 21, 365±390 (2000)


368 J. J. SOSIK AND V. M. GODSHALK<br />

component behaviors that are concerned with both task <strong>and</strong> people. For instance, <strong>mentoring</strong><br />

may involve both task-oriented clarifying behaviors <strong>and</strong> people-oriented developing behaviors<br />

(Yukl, 1994). However, path±goal theory (House, 1996) suggests that people-oriented behaviors<br />

are more appropriate than task-oriented behaviors for enhancing the development <strong>and</strong> <strong>job</strong><br />

satisfaction of lower echelon employees (e.g., prote ge s).<br />

Mintzberg (1973) identi®ed participating in developmental activities (e.g., <strong>mentoring</strong>) as a key<br />

leader role. Prior research (e.g., Kram, 1985; Noe, 1988) indicates that mentors provide both<br />

psychosocial support <strong>and</strong> career development <strong>functions</strong> to prote ge s. As noted above, psychosocial<br />

support <strong>functions</strong> parallel leadership behaviors identi®ed by Yukl (1990) such as supporting,<br />

motivating <strong>and</strong> inspiring, <strong>and</strong> developing. Career development <strong>functions</strong> parallel leadership<br />

behaviors identi®ed by Yukl (1990) such as clarifying roles <strong>and</strong> objectives, developing, <strong>and</strong><br />

networking. While some <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> (e.g., protection) may not parallel leadership<br />

behavior <strong>and</strong> some leadership behaviors (e.g., team building) may not parallel <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong>,<br />

Table 1 suggests some conceptual overlap between leadership <strong>and</strong> <strong>mentoring</strong>. However,<br />

conceptual distinctions between leader behaviors <strong>and</strong> <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> shown in Table 1 are<br />

in line with evidence of empirical distinctiveness between the constructs found in research cited<br />

above. Given that Table 1 highlights both similarities <strong>and</strong> di€erences between leader behaviors<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> <strong>and</strong> mentors may display leader behaviors (Gladstone, 1988), we<br />

concluded that leadership behaviors displayed by a mentor may in¯uence prote ge perceptions of<br />

<strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong>.<br />

Theoretical background<br />

Manning et al.'s (1996) simpli®ed model of work <strong>stress</strong>, adapted from Matteson <strong>and</strong> Ivancevich<br />

(1982), provides the general theoretical framework for this study. According to Manning et al.<br />

(1996), personal (e.g., prote ge age) <strong>and</strong> external (e.g., mentor's leadership behavior) sources of<br />

<strong>stress</strong> in¯uence <strong>stress</strong> as experienced, which in turn can a€ect potential outcomes (e.g., physical,<br />

psychological, organizational factors). In addition, having social support (e.g., <strong>mentoring</strong><br />

<strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong>) can reduce <strong>stress</strong> as experienced <strong>and</strong> moderate the in¯uence of external<br />

sources of <strong>stress</strong> on <strong>stress</strong> as experienced (Manning et al., 1996; Parker <strong>and</strong> DeCotiis, 1983). The<br />

present study focuses on examining in¯uences of external sources of <strong>stress</strong> (mentor's leadership<br />

behavior) <strong>and</strong> social support (prote ge perception of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong>) on <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong><br />

<strong>stress</strong> as experienced by the prote ge .<br />

Based upon Manning et al.'s (1996) model of work <strong>stress</strong>, we proposed the general framework<br />

shown in Figure 1, to predict the relationships of mentor leadership style with prote ge <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong><br />

<strong>stress</strong>, directly <strong>and</strong> via <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong> by prote ge . Figure 1 illustrates several key<br />

relationships. First, mentor leadership style is seen as being associated with both prote ge receipt of<br />

<strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong>. Second, prote ge receipt of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> will<br />

in¯uence the level of prote ge <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong>. Third, as suggested by Manning et al. (1996),<br />

prote ge receipt of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> will moderate the relationship between mentor transformational<br />

leadership behavior <strong>and</strong> prote ge <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong>. These relationships are described in<br />

detail below.<br />

To link leadership <strong>styles</strong> to <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong>, we draw<br />

upon literature on the transformational±transactional leadership paradigm which has <strong>received</strong><br />

extensive theoretical <strong>and</strong> empirical attention (e.g., Bass, 1985; Bennis, 1989; Bryman, 1992;<br />

Copyright # 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 21, 365±390 (2000)


LEADERSHIP, MENTORING AND STRESS 369<br />

Figure 1. A model of leadership style, <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong>, <strong>and</strong> <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong><br />

Burns, 1978; Conger <strong>and</strong> Kanungo, 1987; Dubinsky, Yammarino <strong>and</strong> Jolson, 1995; Howell <strong>and</strong><br />

Avolio, 1993; Podsako€ et al., 1990; Sashkin <strong>and</strong> Rosenbach, 1993; Shamir, House <strong>and</strong> Arthur,<br />

1993; Yammarino et al., 1997). Several reviews (e.g., Bass, 1990, 1998; Kirkpatrick <strong>and</strong> Locke,<br />

1996) <strong>and</strong> meta-analyses (Gaspar, 1992Ð`Transformational leadership: an integrative review of<br />

the literature'. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Western Michigan University; Lowe, Kroeck<br />

<strong>and</strong> Sivasubramaniam, 1996; Patterson et al., 1995) have indicated that transactional leadership<br />

can result in expected performance levels, while transformational leadership can result in<br />

individual, group, <strong>and</strong> unit performance beyond expectations. In fact, transformational leadership<br />

is seen as a particularly powerful source of e€ective leadership in Army, Navy, <strong>and</strong> Air Force<br />

settings (e.g., Curphy, 1992; Yammarino <strong>and</strong> Bass, 1990), computer-mediated group settings<br />

(e.g., Sosik, Avolio <strong>and</strong> Kahai, 1997), innovative research <strong>and</strong> development contexts (e.g., Howell<br />

<strong>and</strong> Higgins, 1992; Howell <strong>and</strong> Avolio, 1993), total quality management programmes (Sosik <strong>and</strong><br />

Dionne, 1997), <strong>and</strong> <strong>stress</strong> reduction contexts (Seltzer, Numero€ <strong>and</strong> Bass, 1989).<br />

The transactional±transformational leadership paradigm is grounded upon seminal work<br />

by Burns (1978) <strong>and</strong> Bass (1985). Bass <strong>and</strong> Avolio's (1994) Full Range of <strong>Leadership</strong> (FRL) model<br />

represents a re®nement of Bass (1985) <strong>and</strong> is the foundation of extensive training of individuals<br />

from industry, education, military, religious, <strong>and</strong> non-pro®t sectors. The FRL model proposes<br />

that every leader may display some amount of laissez-faire, transactional, <strong>and</strong> transformational<br />

leadership <strong>styles</strong>. Subsumed within each style are particular behaviors. The most ine€ective <strong>and</strong><br />

passive style is laissez-faire, characterized by delays of action, absence, <strong>and</strong> indi€erence. More<br />

e€ective <strong>styles</strong> include transactional leadership, which involves `leaders approaching followers with<br />

an eye to exchanging one thing for another' (Burns, 1978, p. 4). The most e€ective form of<br />

transactional leadership is contingent reward leadership where one sets goals, clari®es desired<br />

outcomes, provides both positive <strong>and</strong> negative feedback, <strong>and</strong> exchanges rewards <strong>and</strong> recognition<br />

for accomplishments when they are deserved. Transformational leadership involves forming `a<br />

relationship of mutual stimulation <strong>and</strong> elevation that converts followers into leaders <strong>and</strong> may<br />

Copyright # 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 21, 365±390 (2000)


370 J. J. SOSIK AND V. M. GODSHALK<br />

convert leaders into moral agents' (Burns, 1978, p. 4). Bass <strong>and</strong> Avolio (1994) identi®ed four<br />

behaviors associated with transformational leaders: individualized considerationÐgiving personal<br />

attention to followers to promote their development <strong>and</strong> achievement; intellectual stimulationÐ<br />

enabling followers to think of old problems in new ways; inspirational motivationÐcommunicating<br />

high performance expectations through the projection of a powerful, con®dent, dynamic<br />

presence; <strong>and</strong> idealized in¯uenceÐdisplaying role model behaviors for followers through<br />

exemplary personal achievements, character, <strong>and</strong>/or behavior.<br />

The FRL model of leadership was chosen, as opposed to other leadership models, to link<br />

leadership style to <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong> for several reasons. First,<br />

the FRL model leadership <strong>styles</strong> subsume speci®c leader behaviors found in Yukl's (1990)<br />

taxonomy of leader behaviors. Second, while other leadership models depict two-dimensional<br />

models of leadership behavior, such as directive versus participative (House, 1996) or task versus<br />

relations-oriented (Fiedler, 1967), the FRL model covers leadership <strong>styles</strong> which may subsume<br />

prior models of leader behavior. For example, Bass (1998) argued that transformational <strong>and</strong><br />

transactional leadership can be either directive or participative. Thus, by considering transformational,<br />

transactional, <strong>and</strong> laissez-faire leadership, the FRL model provides a rich array of<br />

leader behaviors that other leadership models may lack. Third, the FRL model has been widely<br />

researched in a variety of evaluative investigations (see Bass, 1998 for a comprehensive review).<br />

Fourth, the FRL model describes speci®c behaviors which may facilitate mentor training in terms<br />

of transactional <strong>and</strong> transformational relationships <strong>and</strong> their impacts on prote ge s.<br />

Hypotheses<br />

<strong>Leadership</strong> <strong>styles</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong><br />

Sc<strong>and</strong>ura <strong>and</strong> Schriesheim (1994) argued that transformational leadership is consistent with<br />

requirements for e€ective <strong>mentoring</strong>. Mentor transformational leadership behavior may be more<br />

congruent with prote ge receipt of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> than transactional or laissez-faire<br />

leadership <strong>styles</strong> for several reasons. First, transformational leadership builds follower trust<br />

(Podsako€ et al., 1990). By exhibiting idealized in¯uence, transformational leaders may be<br />

viewed by their followers as a trustworthy symbol of success <strong>and</strong> accomplishment. These<br />

idealized in¯uence behaviors are similar to role modelling behaviors identi®ed by Noe (1988) as<br />

being associated with the psychosocial support function of <strong>mentoring</strong>. Perceived as trustworthy,<br />

respected <strong>and</strong> admirable role models, mentors who exhibit idealized in¯uence may enhance their<br />

prote ge 's ability to undertake calculated risks to advance their careers.<br />

Second, transformational leadership involves (a) spending time teaching <strong>and</strong> coaching others,<br />

(b) treating others as individuals with unique needs, abilities <strong>and</strong> aspirations, (c) helping others<br />

develop strengths, <strong>and</strong> (d) listening attentively to concerns of others (Bass <strong>and</strong> Avolio, 1994).<br />

These individually considerate behaviors are likely to facilitate counselling <strong>and</strong> individualized<br />

coaching of prote ge s, identi®ed by Noe (1988) as important aspects of <strong>mentoring</strong>.<br />

Third, transformational leadership encourages others to reformulate assumptions through<br />

considering the absurd, fantasizing, <strong>and</strong> focusing on the context rather than the task (Bass <strong>and</strong><br />

Avolio, 1994). These methods of intellectual stimulation are useful in fostering prote ge creativity<br />

<strong>and</strong> developing prote ge cognitive abilities (Torrance, 1983). In addition, intellectual stimulation<br />

develops analytical skills through reexamining assumptions, seeking di€erent perspectives,<br />

Copyright # 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 21, 365±390 (2000)


LEADERSHIP, MENTORING AND STRESS 371<br />

suggesting new ways of performing tasks, <strong>and</strong> rethinking what has never been questioned before.<br />

According to Torrance (1983), these e€ects also may foster prote ge intellectual development.<br />

Fourth, in their e€orts to motivate others, transformational leaders articulate inspirational<br />

long-term visions which attach meaning <strong>and</strong> importance to human development. By linking the<br />

signi®cance of human development (e.g., developing new skills <strong>and</strong> higher levels of creative<br />

thinking, trust, <strong>and</strong> responsibility) to the successful attainment of the broader organizational<br />

mission, these leaders enhance others' belief that they can be e€ective contributors to a high<br />

achieving organization (Shamir et al., 1993). These inspirationally motivating behaviors are<br />

similar to ecacy <strong>and</strong> con®dence building behaviors required by mentors (Kram, 1985). On the<br />

basis of these arguments, we expected a strong positive relationship between mentor transformational<br />

leadership behavior <strong>and</strong> prote ge receipt of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong>.<br />

Mentor transactional contingent reward leadership behavior also may be associated with<br />

prote ge receipt of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong>. When a leader sets goals, clari®es outcomes <strong>and</strong> exchanges<br />

rewards <strong>and</strong> recognition for follower accomplishments, followers generally achieve expected performance<br />

(Bass, 1985). A contingent reward leader may lay the foundation for follower development<br />

by clarifying desired developmental outcomes, discussing in speci®c terms learning<br />

objectives which outline what must be accomplished, <strong>and</strong> rewarding the follower when she/he<br />

identi®es <strong>and</strong> participates in developmental activities. Similarly, a <strong>mentoring</strong> relationship, in<br />

which contingent reward behavior is displayed by the mentor, is based upon the assumption that<br />

by clarifying what the mentor expects <strong>and</strong> then rewarding the prote ge for developmental behavior,<br />

the mentor directs the prote ge to the desired developmental outcome. In fact, Thibodeaux <strong>and</strong><br />

Lowe (1996) found mentored individuals to report greater supervisory use of rewards than nonmentored<br />

individuals. In addition, prote ge trust may be enhanced to the extent that the mentor<br />

(a) negotiates, agrees, exchanges, <strong>and</strong> bargains with the prote ge by establishing a `learning<br />

contract'; (b) communicates a clear underst<strong>and</strong>ing to the prote ge about what the mentor <strong>and</strong><br />

prote ge will do for one another in order to adhere to the learning contract; <strong>and</strong> (c) rewards<br />

developmental behavior. These behaviors parallel contingent reward leadership <strong>and</strong> are characteristic<br />

of what Covey (1997) <strong>and</strong> Podsako€ et al. (1990) describe as behaviors that promote<br />

trustÐa key aspect of e€ective mentor±prote ge relationships (Murray, 1991; Yukl, 1994).<br />

E€ective <strong>mentoring</strong> provides career development <strong>and</strong> psychosocial support for the prote geÂ<br />

(Kram, 1985). Given that contingent reward behavior involves goal setting (Bass, 1985) <strong>and</strong> setting<br />

career goals promotes career development (Kram, 1985), mentors who display contingent reward<br />

behavior may provide career development to prote ge s by setting career goals. Since contingent<br />

reward behavior is generally associated with increased <strong>job</strong> satisfaction (Sims <strong>and</strong> Lorenzi, 1992)<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>job</strong> satisfaction is associated with receiving psychosocial support (Bahniuk, Dobos <strong>and</strong> Hill,<br />

1990; Parasuraman, Greenhaus <strong>and</strong> Granrose, 1992), mentors who display contingent reward<br />

behaviors may provide psychosocial support for prote ge s by increasing prote ge <strong>job</strong> satisfaction.<br />

However, contingent reward behavior focuses on getting the task done <strong>and</strong> is less likely to be<br />

associated with stimulating greater development of others than transformational behavior (Bass,<br />

1998). Contingent reward behavior attends less to relational aspects of interactions <strong>and</strong> more to<br />

de®ning the task <strong>and</strong> level of expected performance. Given the importance of relational aspects in<br />

mentor±prote ge relationships (Kram <strong>and</strong> Bragar, 1991) <strong>and</strong> the task orientation of contingent<br />

reward behavior, we expected mentor contingent reward behavior to be less positively associated<br />

with prote ge receipt of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> than transformational behavior.<br />

Mentor laissez-faire behavior may be totally inconsistent with prote ge receipt of <strong>mentoring</strong><br />

<strong>functions</strong>. Laissez-faire leadership results in less concentration on work, poor quality of work,<br />

<strong>and</strong> low levels of productivity, cohesiveness <strong>and</strong> satisfaction (Bass, 1990). Applied to <strong>mentoring</strong><br />

relationships, these e€ects may be detrimental to the skill <strong>and</strong> career development of the prote ge .<br />

Copyright # 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 21, 365±390 (2000)


372 J. J. SOSIK AND V. M. GODSHALK<br />

In addition, laissez-faire behavior strives to maintain the status quo through delay, absence,<br />

<strong>and</strong> indi€erence. However, <strong>mentoring</strong> requires a mentor to actively look for <strong>and</strong> ®nd opportunities<br />

for prote ge development (Yukl, 1994), provide both psychosocial <strong>and</strong> career facilitation<br />

support (Noe, 1988), serve as a role model, <strong>and</strong> spend time developing the prote ge (Kram,<br />

1985). Laissez-faire behavior on the part of the mentor is not likely to ful®l these requirements.<br />

Thus, we expected a negative relationship between mentor laissez-faire behavior <strong>and</strong> prote geÂ<br />

receipt of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong>. Based on the above discussion, we proposed the following<br />

hypotheses:<br />

Hypothesis 1a: Mentor transformational leadership behavior will be positively <strong>related</strong> to<br />

prote ge receipt of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong>.<br />

Hypothesis 1b: Mentor transactional contingent reward leadership behavior will have a lower<br />

positive relationship with prote ge receipt of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> than mentor transformational<br />

leadership behavior.<br />

Hypothesis 1c: Mentor laissez-faire leadership behavior will be negatively <strong>related</strong> to prote geÂ<br />

receipt of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong>.<br />

<strong>Leadership</strong> <strong>styles</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong><br />

Job-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong> is often a function of an individual's perception of organizational <strong>and</strong><br />

environmental events <strong>and</strong> meaning attached to these events (McCauley, 1987; Schuler, 1980). The<br />

meaning which individuals associate with these events is often `managed' or in¯uenced by<br />

signi®cant others, such as leaders <strong>and</strong> mentors (Kram <strong>and</strong> Hall, 1989; Smircich <strong>and</strong> Morgan,<br />

1982). For example, when a mentor manages the meaning of organizational events for a prote ge ,<br />

he or she in¯uences the manner in which the prote ge perceives, interprets, <strong>and</strong> acts upon <strong>job</strong><strong>related</strong><br />

events (e.g., <strong>stress</strong>ful events). In this role, the mentor engages in numerous leadership<br />

behaviors (e.g., motivating, communicating) which provide underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>and</strong> reduce uncertainty<br />

associated with perceived hostile events. In fact, leadership behavior has been linked to<br />

e€ectiveness of individuals dealing with combat conditions (Kalay, 1983), con¯ict (Katz, 1977),<br />

role <strong>stress</strong> (LaRocco <strong>and</strong> Jones, 1978), panic (Kugihara <strong>and</strong> Misumi, 1984), <strong>and</strong> disaster (Watson,<br />

1984). Thus, a prote ge 's <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong> may be in¯uenced by the mentor's leadership style.<br />

Laissez-faire style, involving avoidance or absence of leadership, provides no meaning or clari-<br />

®cation of events for followers. Lack of leader communication undermines follower trust in the<br />

leader (Bass, 1998; Podsako€ et al., 1990) <strong>and</strong> may serve to intensify follower fear of the unknown<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong>. Given the importance of communication in both <strong>mentoring</strong> relationships<br />

(Murray, 1991) <strong>and</strong> <strong>stress</strong> reduction (McCauley, 1987), mentors who fail to communicate may<br />

be like laissez-faire leaders in both behavior <strong>and</strong> in impact of their behavior on prote ge s. Thus,<br />

we expected that mentor laissez-faire behavior would be positively <strong>related</strong> to prote ge <strong>job</strong><strong>related</strong><br />

<strong>stress</strong>.<br />

Stress may be allayed by reducing uncertainty through e€ective communication (McCauley,<br />

1987), raising ecacy expectations (B<strong>and</strong>ura, 1986), <strong>and</strong> developing supportive group relationships<br />

(Schuler, 1980). Contingent reward behavior may reduce uncertainty, raise ecacy expectations,<br />

<strong>and</strong> get agreement on what needs to be done by clarifying performance expectations (Sims<br />

<strong>and</strong> Lorenzi, 1992). As such, contingent reward behavior may provide mentors with mechanisms<br />

for allaying <strong>job</strong>-<strong>stress</strong> experienced by prote ge s. Thus, we expected mentor contingent reward<br />

leadership to be negatively <strong>related</strong> to prote ge <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong>.<br />

Copyright # 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 21, 365±390 (2000)


LEADERSHIP, MENTORING AND STRESS 373<br />

While these transactional methods to allay <strong>stress</strong> may satisfy immediate satisfaction needs of<br />

prote ge s, they are unlikely to establish long-term positive e€ectiveness in coping with <strong>stress</strong> (Bass,<br />

1998). Prior research (e.g., Mulder, van Eck <strong>and</strong> deJong, 1971; Seltzer, Numero€ <strong>and</strong> Bass, 1989)<br />

suggests that transformational behavior displayed by mentors may provide the most e€ective<br />

means to allay prote ge <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong>. For example, Seltzer et al. (1989) found that transformational<br />

leadership based on charismatic <strong>and</strong> individually considerate behaviors was negatively<br />

<strong>related</strong> to follower <strong>stress</strong>. Transformational leadership arouses team spirit, reframes <strong>stress</strong>ful<br />

events into developmental opportunities, <strong>and</strong> inspires others to perceive dicult situations as<br />

meaningful challenges necessary for developing one's professional <strong>and</strong> personal skills (Shamir<br />

et al., 1993). By emphasizing the importance of collective action, transformational leadership<br />

evokes higher-order needs (e.g., sense of belonging to a larger entity) <strong>and</strong> moves followers into a<br />

vigilant search for long-term readiness. As such, follower <strong>stress</strong> may be reduced as `the<br />

insecurity of feeling isolated is replaced by the security of a sense of belonging' (Bass, 1998, p. 46).<br />

These <strong>stress</strong>-reducing behaviors parallel acceptance <strong>and</strong> con®rmation behaviors associated with<br />

the psychosocial support function of <strong>mentoring</strong> (Kram <strong>and</strong> Hall, 1989). Thus, we expected that<br />

mentor transformational leadership behavior would be more negatively <strong>related</strong> to prote ge <strong>job</strong><strong>related</strong><br />

<strong>stress</strong> than transactional contingent reward behavior. Taken together, the above<br />

arguments suggest:<br />

Hypothesis 2a: Mentor transformational leadership behavior will be negatively <strong>related</strong> to<br />

prote ge <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong>.<br />

Hypothesis 2b: Mentor transactional contingent reward leadership behavior will have a lower<br />

negative relationship with prote ge <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong> than mentor transformational leadership<br />

behavior.<br />

Hypothesis 2c: Mentor laissez-faire behavior will be positively <strong>related</strong> to prote ge <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong><br />

<strong>stress</strong>.<br />

Mentoring <strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong><br />

The relationship between <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong> <strong>and</strong> prote ge <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong> may be a<br />

function of the positive outcomes <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> provide. Mentoring has been positively<br />

associated with e€ective socialization of young employees (Schein, 1978), promotions <strong>and</strong><br />

compensation (Dreher <strong>and</strong> Ash, 1990), career mobility (Sc<strong>and</strong>ura, 1992), career satisfaction<br />

(Fagenson, 1989), career commitment (Colarelli <strong>and</strong> Bishop, 1990), <strong>and</strong> <strong>job</strong> satisfaction<br />

(Bahniuk et al., 1990); <strong>and</strong> negatively associated with turnover intentions (Viator <strong>and</strong> Sc<strong>and</strong>ura,<br />

1991). These ®ndings suggest that <strong>mentoring</strong> may provide the prote ge with career development<br />

opportunities <strong>and</strong> career goals.<br />

Individuals who perceive low career development opportunities <strong>and</strong> discrepancies with career<br />

goals resulting from a lack of social support experience high levels of <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong>, while<br />

those who receive social support <strong>and</strong> opportunities for development report lower <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong><br />

<strong>stress</strong> (Parker <strong>and</strong> DeCotiis, 1983). In addition, prior research cited above suggests that<br />

individuals who participate in <strong>mentoring</strong> relationships exhibit positive/functional organizational<br />

attitudes <strong>and</strong> behaviors. For example, Mowday, Porter <strong>and</strong> Steers (1982) argued that individuals<br />

a€ectively respond to aspects of the work environment <strong>and</strong> these reactions in¯uence their<br />

attitudes towards their <strong>job</strong>s. Job-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong> may cause individuals to deviate from normal<br />

functioning (Jamal, 1990). Mentoring appears to help individuals cope with <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong> by<br />

providing both career development <strong>and</strong> psychosocial assistance <strong>and</strong>, therefore, may allow these<br />

Copyright # 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 21, 365±390 (2000)


374 J. J. SOSIK AND V. M. GODSHALK<br />

individuals to function normally in the organization. On the basis of this literature, we propose<br />

the following hypothesis:<br />

Hypothesis 3: Prote ge receipt of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> will be negatively <strong>related</strong> to prote geÂ<br />

<strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong>.<br />

As noted above, Manning et al. (1996) argued that social support may moderate e€ects of<br />

external sources of <strong>stress</strong> on perceived <strong>stress</strong>. A mentor's leadership style may be an external<br />

source of <strong>stress</strong> (Matteson <strong>and</strong> Ivancevich, 1982; Seltzer et al., 1989). Mentoring <strong>functions</strong> are a<br />

form of social support (House, 1981) <strong>and</strong> may provide an antidote to <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong> (Kram<br />

<strong>and</strong> Hall, 1989). However, <strong>stress</strong> reduction e€orts are more e€ective when perceptions of <strong>stress</strong><br />

are changed via development of long-term readiness <strong>and</strong> coping mechanisms (McCauley, 1987).<br />

Such changes in perceptions of <strong>stress</strong> may be facilitated by the developmental nature of<br />

transformational leadership (Sc<strong>and</strong>ura <strong>and</strong> Schriesheim, 1994) <strong>and</strong> its focus on promoting<br />

fundamental change in moving individuals beyond immediate concerns toward concern for an<br />

attractive future (Bennis, 1989; Burns, 1978). Similarly, <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> promote fundamental<br />

changes in prote ge s in terms of attitudes (e.g., perception of <strong>stress</strong>) <strong>and</strong> career <strong>and</strong> selfdevelopment<br />

(Kram <strong>and</strong> Hall, 1989).<br />

These arguments suggest that a mentor's e€orts to allay prote ge <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong> via<br />

transformational behavior will be moderated by prote ge receipt of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong>. In<br />

essence, when <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong> are high, the negative impact of mentor<br />

transformational behavior on prote ge <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong> is stronger. Conversely, when <strong>mentoring</strong><br />

<strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong> are low, the negative impact of mentor transformational behavior on prote geÂ<br />

<strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong> is lower. Our expectation is based on a cognitive consistency argument where<br />

the underlying rule is that when <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong> are low, transformational<br />

behaviors such as individualized consideration may be perceived as inauthentic or insincere <strong>and</strong><br />

therefore ought not promote development of <strong>stress</strong>-reducing mechanisms. When <strong>mentoring</strong><br />

<strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong> are high, transformational behaviors may be perceived as being consistent with<br />

<strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> in terms of developmental orientation <strong>and</strong> therefore promote development<br />

of <strong>stress</strong>-reducing mechanisms. Thus, all else being equal, if we had two groups of prote ge s where<br />

the ®rst group <strong>received</strong> low levels of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> <strong>and</strong> the second group the opposite, we<br />

would expect a low to possibly negative association between mentor transformational behavior<br />

<strong>and</strong> prote ge <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong> for the ®rst group <strong>and</strong> a high negative association for the second<br />

group.<br />

Unlike transformational behavior, transactional contingent reward behavior displayed<br />

by mentors may promote a cost-bene®t exchange relationship with prote ge s (Sc<strong>and</strong>ura<br />

<strong>and</strong> Schriescheim, 1994). Such behavior is not likely to develop prote ge long-term positive<br />

e€ectiveness in coping with <strong>stress</strong>ful conditions or reducing perceptions of <strong>stress</strong> (Bass, 1998).<br />

Similarly, absence or avoidance on the part of a laissez-faire mentor is inconsistent with<br />

<strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> <strong>and</strong> therefore is not likely to promote <strong>stress</strong> reduction e€orts. Thus, we<br />

hypothesized:<br />

Hypothesis 4: Prote ge receipt of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> will moderate the negative relationship<br />

between mentor transformational leadership behavior <strong>and</strong> prote ge <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong>.<br />

Speci®cally, mentor transformational leadership behavior will be more negatively <strong>related</strong><br />

to prote ge <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong> when prote ge receipt of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> is high rather than<br />

low.<br />

Copyright # 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 21, 365±390 (2000)


LEADERSHIP, MENTORING AND STRESS 375<br />

Method<br />

Sample <strong>and</strong> procedure<br />

Two-hundred <strong>and</strong> thirty adult students enrolled in a masters of management programme in a<br />

large public university in the Northeast participated in the study for course credit. Participants<br />

were full-time corporate employees from various industries who were involved in either formal<br />

or informal <strong>mentoring</strong> relationships. The industries represented include: services (22 per cent),<br />

manufacturing (17 per cent), ®nancial/insurance (16 per cent), pharmaceuticals (7 per cent),<br />

transportation/utilities (6 per cent), telecommunications (6 per cent), public administration (1 per<br />

cent), <strong>and</strong> other unidenti®ed industries (25 per cent). Mentoring relationships ranged in length<br />

from 1 year to 12 years, with the average being 2.7 years. Participants ranged in age from 20 years<br />

to 57 years, with the average age being 31. They had worked, on average, 4.8 years with their<br />

companies <strong>and</strong> had a range of company tenure from 2 months to 40 years. Fifty-six per cent of<br />

the participants were male, <strong>and</strong> the vast majority (82 per cent) were Caucasian. The remaining<br />

18 per cent of the sample consisted of African American (6 per cent), Hispanic (2 per cent), Asian<br />

(7 per cent), Native American (1 per cent), <strong>and</strong> non-responding (2 per cent) participants.<br />

Data were collected through two questionnaires, which were distributed to participants in<br />

class, completed outside of class, <strong>and</strong> returned directly to the researchers. The ®rst questionnaire<br />

was completed by the proteÂge <strong>and</strong> included items measuring <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong>,<br />

perceived <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong>, <strong>and</strong> demographic information. This questionnaire contained the<br />

following instructions to de®ne <strong>mentoring</strong> relationships for participants.<br />

`Please provide information regarding your experiences with <strong>mentoring</strong> relationships.<br />

Mentoring relationships are characterized by a close, professional relationship between two<br />

individualsÐone usually more senior in some regard. The mentor <strong>and</strong> prote ge may or may not<br />

be with the same company.'<br />

In addition, the following information was read to participants prior to distribution of the<br />

questionnaires.<br />

`Mentoring is de®ned as a deliberate pairing of a more skilled or experienced person with a<br />

lesser skilled or experienced one, with the agreed-upon goal of having the lesser skilled person<br />

grow <strong>and</strong> develop speci®c competencies. Your mentor may or may not be your manager.'<br />

The second questionnaire included items measuring leadership behaviors <strong>and</strong> was completed by<br />

the prote ge 's mentor. This questionnaire was mailed by each mentor directly to the researchers<br />

using a pre-addressed, stamped return envelope. A total of 204 usable responses, representing<br />

88 per cent of all participant cases, were used in the data analysis. Ninety-one per cent<br />

(186 participants) were in informal <strong>mentoring</strong> relationships while nine per cent (18 participants)<br />

were in formal <strong>mentoring</strong> relationships. Eighty-®ve per cent of mentors were managers/supervisors<br />

of the prote ge s. Of the 15 per cent which were not, 10 per cent were mentored by peers <strong>and</strong><br />

5 per cent were mentored by other individuals.<br />

Data analysis<br />

The hypotheses were tested using Partial Least Squares (PLS; Wold, 1985), a structural equation<br />

modeling technique. PLS has been used by a growing number of researchers in organizational<br />

behavior (e.g., Duxbury <strong>and</strong> Higgins, 1991; House, Spangler <strong>and</strong> Woycke, 1991; Kahai, Sosik<br />

Copyright # 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 21, 365±390 (2000)


376 J. J. SOSIK AND V. M. GODSHALK<br />

<strong>and</strong> Avolio, 1997). PLS o€ers several bene®ts over traditional techniques such as ANOVA or<br />

regression. These bene®ts are discussed in Appendix A.<br />

We used PLS-Graph (version 2.91.02.08), a graphics-based program for performing PLS<br />

analysis in this study. PLS generates estimates of st<strong>and</strong>ardized regression coecients (i.e., path<br />

coecients) for the model paths, which can then be used to measure the relationships between<br />

latent variables. A jackkni®ng procedure called blindfolding was used to compute st<strong>and</strong>ard errors<br />

<strong>and</strong> assess signi®cance of estimates of path coecients (Wold, 1985). The blindfolding procedure<br />

omits a part of the data matrix for a particular construct <strong>and</strong> then estimates the model parameters<br />

(e.g., path coecients) associated with that construct. This process is repeated as often as the<br />

omission distance, which refers to how many data points in the data matrix are skipped before<br />

omitting one data point. An omission distance of 9 was employed (Sambamurthy <strong>and</strong> Chin, 1994).<br />

A conservative hurdle rate of p 5 0.001 was used to indicate signi®cance.<br />

Full sample data (n ˆ 204) was utilized to test Hypothesis 1a through Hypothesis 3, in terms of<br />

the unmoderated model. To test Hypothesis 4 in terms of the moderating e€ect of <strong>mentoring</strong><br />

<strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong>, we split the sample into two sub-samples (i.e., low <strong>and</strong> high levels of<br />

<strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong>). Partitioning of the data into these sub-samples is discussed below.<br />

This approach parallels more traditional moderated regression by testing a model separately for<br />

each sub-sample (Duxbury <strong>and</strong> Higgins, 1991). Unpaired t-tests were used to test for di€erences<br />

in st<strong>and</strong>ardized regression coecients across the sub-samples (see Duxbury <strong>and</strong> Higgins, 1991).<br />

In PLS, constructs may be modelled using re¯ective or formative indicators. Re¯ective<br />

indicators are suggested when they arise from the construct, because of which they tend to covary<br />

(e.g., components of transformational leadership). Formative indicators are suggested when they<br />

combine to form a construct (e.g., aspects of socio-economic status) (Falk <strong>and</strong> Miller, 1992).<br />

Modelling of constructs examined in the present study is discussed below.<br />

Measurement of constructs<br />

Information was obtained from participants <strong>and</strong> from their mentors. Multiple-item measures<br />

were used to assess mentor leadership behaviors <strong>and</strong> prote ge perceptions of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong><br />

<strong>received</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong>. Items for the measures are listed in Appendix B.<br />

Mentor's leadership behaviors<br />

Mentor leadership style/behavior was measured using items from the Multifactor <strong>Leadership</strong><br />

Questionnaire (MLQ-5X; Bass <strong>and</strong> Avolio, 1997). While previous versions of the MLQ have been<br />

criticized for failure to empirically generate the factor structure proposed by Bass <strong>and</strong> Avolio<br />

(1994) to underlie transformational leadership (e.g., Bycio, Hackett <strong>and</strong> Allen, 1995; Yukl, 1994),<br />

research on the MLQ-Form 5X (e.g., Avolio, Bass <strong>and</strong> Jung, 1997; Bass <strong>and</strong> Avolio, 1997; Bass,<br />

1998) has been shown it to be a psychometrically sound instrument. Mentors were asked to judge<br />

how frequently they exhibited speci®c behaviors measured by the MLQ-5X. Each behavior was<br />

measured on a ®ve-point frequency scale ranging from not at all (0) to frequently, if not always<br />

(4). Laissez-faire leadership was measured using two items from the MLQ-5X. Contingent reward<br />

leadership was measured using three items from the MLQ-5X. The following four-item scales was<br />

used as indicators of transformational leadership: (a) idealized in¯uenceÐbehavior (a ˆ 0.75),<br />

(b) inspirational motivation (a ˆ 0.72), (c) intellectual stimulation (a ˆ 0.72), <strong>and</strong> (d)<br />

individualized consideration (a ˆ 0.64).<br />

Copyright # 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 21, 365±390 (2000)


LEADERSHIP, MENTORING AND STRESS 377<br />

Mentoring <strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong><br />

To assess the degree of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>received</strong> by the prote ge , we used two 10-item scales from Noe<br />

(1988) as indicators of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong>: (a) career development (a ˆ 0.86), <strong>and</strong> (b) psychosocial<br />

support (a ˆ 0.87). Prote ge s were asked to indicate their extent of agreement with each item<br />

using a ®ve-point scale ranging from disagree strongly (1) to agree strongly (5). Hypothesis 4,<br />

which pertained to the moderating e€ect of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong>, was tested by creating<br />

two sub-samples based on a median split: low <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong> sub-sample (scale<br />

score 53.85; n ˆ 103) <strong>and</strong> high <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong> sub-sample (scale score 43.85,<br />

n ˆ 101).<br />

Job-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong><br />

Job-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong> perceived by the prote ge was measured using six items from Parasuraman<br />

(1977, `Sources <strong>and</strong> outcomes of organizational <strong>stress</strong>: a multidimensional study of the<br />

antecedents, <strong>and</strong> attitudinal <strong>and</strong> behavioral indices of <strong>job</strong> <strong>stress</strong>'. Unpublished doctoral<br />

dissertation, State University of New York at Bu€alo). Prote ge s were asked to judge how often<br />

they experienced situations described in each item. Each situation was measured on a ®ve-point<br />

frequency scale ranging from never occurs (1) to constantly occurs (5).<br />

The indicators of laissez-faire, contingent reward, <strong>and</strong> transformational leadership were<br />

expected to covary (see Bass, 1998), as were the indicators of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong>, thereby indicating that they arise from their respective constructs. Therefore,<br />

these indicators were modelled as re¯ective.<br />

Control variables<br />

Theoretical work on <strong>mentoring</strong> (e.g., Kram, 1985; Murray, 1991) suggests that prote geÂ<br />

experience (i.e., age, <strong>job</strong> level, <strong>job</strong> tenure, education level), industry, <strong>and</strong> mentor gender can a€ect<br />

mentor behavior, <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong>, <strong>and</strong> prote ge <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong>. To control for<br />

these potential e€ects, age, <strong>job</strong> level, <strong>job</strong> tenure, education level, industry, <strong>and</strong> mentor gender<br />

were entered into the PLS model as covariates. Each covariate was modelled as a single-item<br />

indicator. PLS is insensitive to how the indicators of single-indicator constructs are modelled.<br />

Results<br />

Table 2 presents the scale means, st<strong>and</strong>ard deviations, <strong>and</strong> product moment correlations among<br />

the measures. PLS generates statistics to test the reliability <strong>and</strong> validity of latent constructs with<br />

two or more re¯ective indicators. Reliability was assessed by ®rst examining the factor loadings of<br />

indicators: a common rule of thumb is that the factor loadings should exceed 0.7 since this implies<br />

that less than half of the indicator's variance is due to error (Fornell <strong>and</strong> Larcker, 1981). Next, we<br />

computed each construct's composite scale reliability (Fornell <strong>and</strong> Larcker, 1981), a measure of<br />

internal consistency similar to Cronbach's alpha. Fornell <strong>and</strong> Larcker (1981) recommended using<br />

a criterion cut-o€ of 0.7 or more. Also, the average variance extracted by the construct<br />

from indicators was examined. For this criterion, Fornell <strong>and</strong> Larcker (1981) recommended using<br />

a cut-o€ of 0.5 or more.<br />

Copyright # 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 21, 365±390 (2000)


378 J. J. SOSIK AND V. M. GODSHALK<br />

Table 2. Means, st<strong>and</strong>ard deviations, <strong>and</strong> intercorrelations<br />

Construct Indicators<br />

Mentoring level Correlations between constructs<br />

Low High 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11<br />

M S.D. M S.D.<br />

1. Laissez-faire 0.76 0.77 0.57 0.59 (0.80)<br />

2. Transactional contingent reward 3.02 0.67 3.24 0.60 0.11 (0.75)<br />

3. Transformational leadership Idealized in¯uence 2.89 0.68 3.08 0.72 0.05 0.59 (0.78)<br />

Inspirational motivation 3.05 0.62 3.13 0.55<br />

Intellectual stimulation 3.12 0.61 3.20 0.56<br />

Individualized consideration 3.31 0.48 3.47 0.50<br />

4. Mentoring <strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong> Psychosocial support 3.74 0.54 4.59 0.97 0.15 0.02 0.06 (0.84)<br />

Career development 2.84 0.70 4.34 1.05<br />

5. Job <strong>stress</strong> 2.73 0.82 2.78 1.39 0.06 0.07 0.09 0.14 (0.80)<br />

6. Age 31.41 6.91 29.90 7.39 0.08 0.12 0.09 0.16 0.07 ±<br />

7. Mentor gender 1.81 0.84 1.94 1.52 0.01 0.03 0.06 0.03 0.02 0.40 ±<br />

8. Industry 6.81 3.55 6.28 3.46 0.01 0.07 0.05 0.09 0.03 0.02 0.18 ±<br />

9. Education 5.98 0.72 5.79 1.03 0.05 0.04 0.04 0.17 0.17 0.08 0.07 0.04 ±<br />

10. Job tenure 3.15 3.07 3.29 3.13 0.04 0.05 0.06 0.05 0.12 0.07 0.03 0.05 0.04 ±<br />

11. Job level 2.03 1.44 2.32 2.17 0.14 0.08 0.02 0.02 0.20 0.03 0.06 0.04 0.16 0.17 ±<br />

Note. Gender coded: 1 ˆ female, 2 ˆ male. Industry coded: 1 ˆ accounting, 2 ˆ consulting, 3 ˆ ®nancial/insurance, 4 ˆ manufacturing, 5 ˆ pharmaceuticals,<br />

6 ˆ public administration, 7 ˆ retailing/hospitality, 8 ˆ services (health, legal), 9 ˆ telecommunications, 10 ˆ utilities/transportation, 11 ˆ other. Education level<br />

ranged from 1 ˆ high school to 7 ˆ graduate degree. Job level coded: 1 ˆ non-supervisory, 2 ˆ ®rst line manager, 3 ˆ middle management, 4 ˆ upper management,<br />

5 ˆ executive. Bold-faced elements on the diagonal represent the square root of the average variance extracted. O€ diagonal elements are correlations between constructs.<br />

Signi®cant correlations (p 5 0.05) are underlined.<br />

Copyright # 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 21, 365±390 (2000)


Table 3. Factor loadings, composite scale reliability, <strong>and</strong> average variance extracted to assess reliability of<br />

measures<br />

Construct<br />

Measures<br />

LEADERSHIP, MENTORING AND STRESS 379<br />

Factor<br />

loading<br />

Weights of<br />

measures<br />

Composite<br />

scale<br />

reliability<br />

Average<br />

variance<br />

extracted<br />

1. Laissez-faire LF 1 0.92 0.79 0.78 0.64<br />

LF 2 0.68 0.45<br />

2. Transactional contingent reward CR1 0.74 0.47 0.79 0.56<br />

CR2 0.82 0.54<br />

CR3 0.67 0.35<br />

3. Transformational leadership II 0.81 0.36 0.86 0.62<br />

IM 0.68 0.15<br />

IS 0.76 0.20<br />

IC 0.89 0.54<br />

4. Mentoring <strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong> PSYCH 0.85 0.61 0.83 0.71<br />

CARDEV 0.84 0.58<br />

5. Job <strong>stress</strong> STRESS1 0.76 0.31 0.91 0.63<br />

STRESS2 0.81 0.19<br />

STRESS3 0.74 0.12<br />

STRESS4 0.89 0.27<br />

STRESS5 0.71 0.18<br />

STRESS6 0.83 0.19<br />

Note. LFˆ laissez-faire; CR ˆ contingent reward; II ˆ idealized in¯uence-behavior; IM ˆ inspirational motivation;<br />

IS ˆ intellectual stimulation; IC ˆ individualized consideration; PSYCH ˆ psychosocial support; CARDEV ˆ career<br />

development; STRESS ˆ <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong>.<br />

Table 3 shows the factor loadings, weights, composite scale reliabilities, <strong>and</strong> average variance<br />

extracted based on PLS analysis of full sample data. With the exception of factor loadings of one<br />

indicator of laissez-faire, contingent reward, <strong>and</strong> transformational leadership constructs, which<br />

were slightly below the recommended criterion cut-o€, all reliability criteria were met by the<br />

study's constructs. In PLS, convergent <strong>and</strong> discriminant validity of indicators of re¯ective<br />

constructs is assessed using criteria similar to a multi-trait/multi-method analysis (Falk <strong>and</strong><br />

Miller, 1992; Kahai et al., 1997). One criterion is that the construct representing the items should<br />

share more variance with its items than with other constructs in the model (Carmines <strong>and</strong> Zeller,<br />

1979). A matrix is shown in Table 2, in which the diagonal elements show the square root of<br />

the average variance shared by a construct with its indicators. For adequate convergent <strong>and</strong><br />

discriminant validity, the diagonal elements should be greater than entries in the corresponding<br />

rows <strong>and</strong> columns. Results summarized in Table 2 indicate this criterion was met.<br />

Results of PLS analysis<br />

Results of PLS analysis to test the hypotheses are presented in Table 4. As predicted by<br />

Hypothesis 1a, there was a positive relationship between mentor transformational leadership<br />

behavior <strong>and</strong> prote ge receipt of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong>. As predicted by Hypothesis 1b, there was a<br />

positive relationship between mentor transactional contingent reward leadership behavior <strong>and</strong><br />

prote ge receipt of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong>. As expected, the positive in¯uence of mentor transactional<br />

contingent reward leadership behavior on prote ge receipt of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> was<br />

not as strong as that of mentor transformational leadership behavior. As predicted by Hypothesis<br />

Copyright # 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 21, 365±390 (2000)


380 J. J. SOSIK AND V. M. GODSHALK<br />

Table 4. Results of Partial Least Squares analysis<br />

Hypothesis <strong>and</strong> proposed relation<br />

St<strong>and</strong>ardized<br />

path coecient t(8) a<br />

H1a: Transformational leadership ! <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong> 0.15 28.08*<br />

H1b: Transactional contingent reward ! <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong> 0.06 5.38*<br />

H1c: Laissez-faire ! <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong> 0.15 23.26*<br />

H2a: Transformational leadership ! <strong>job</strong> <strong>stress</strong> 0.08 15.58*<br />

H2b: Transactional contingent reward ! <strong>job</strong> <strong>stress</strong> 0.01 1.74<br />

H2c: Laissez-faire ! <strong>job</strong> <strong>stress</strong> 0.01 1.61<br />

H3: Mentoring <strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong> ! <strong>job</strong> <strong>stress</strong> 0.11 29.22*<br />

H4: Transformational leadership ! <strong>job</strong> <strong>stress</strong><br />

0.01 1.59<br />

(low <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong> sub-sample)<br />

Transformational leadership ! <strong>job</strong> <strong>stress</strong><br />

(high <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong> sub-sample)<br />

0.31 44.49*<br />

Note. Hˆ hypothesis. The variance explained in <strong>job</strong> <strong>stress</strong> by all measures <strong>and</strong> covariates was 11 per cent. The variance<br />

explained in <strong>mentoring</strong> by all measures <strong>and</strong> covariates was 12 per cent.<br />

a Degrees of freedom for t-test based on omission distance minus 1 (Sambamurthy <strong>and</strong> Chin, 1994).<br />

* p 5 0.001, two-tailed.<br />

1c, mentor laissez-faire leadership behavior was negatively <strong>related</strong> to prote ge receipt of <strong>mentoring</strong><br />

<strong>functions</strong>. Thus, Hypothesis 1 was fully supported.<br />

Hypothesis 2a also was supported. Mentor transformational leadership behavior was<br />

negatively <strong>related</strong> to prote ge <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong>. However, both Hypotheses 2b <strong>and</strong> 2c were not<br />

supported. Mentor transactional contingent reward <strong>and</strong> laissez-faire leadership behaviors<br />

were not <strong>related</strong> to prote ge <strong>job</strong> <strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong>. Hypothesis 3 was supported. Prote ge receipt<br />

of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> was negatively associated with prote ge <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong>. Hypothesis 4<br />

also was supported. Mentor transformational leadership behavior was more negatively<br />

<strong>related</strong> to prote ge <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong> in the high <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong> subsample<br />

(path coecient ˆ0.31) than in the low <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong> subsample (path coecient<br />

ˆ 0.01) (t(8) ˆ43.03, p 5 0.001).<br />

Several covariates had signi®cant e€ects (p


LEADERSHIP, MENTORING AND STRESS 381<br />

coecient ˆ 0.08, t(8) ˆ 12.35). In addition, prote ge <strong>job</strong> tenure (path coecient ˆ 0.10,<br />

t(8) ˆ 25.08), prote ge <strong>job</strong> level (path coecient ˆ 0.17, t(8) ˆ 49.31), <strong>and</strong> prote ge education<br />

level (path coecient ˆ 0.14, t(8) ˆ 41.4) were each positively <strong>related</strong> to prote ge <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong><br />

<strong>stress</strong>. Male mentors were associated with more prote ge <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong> than female mentors<br />

(path coecient ˆ 0.07, t(8) ˆ 8.82).<br />

Discussion<br />

An important ®nding of the present study is that various leadership <strong>styles</strong>/behaviors displayed by<br />

mentors can have di€erential associations with prote ge perceptions of both <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong><br />

<strong>received</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>job</strong> <strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong>. Speci®cally, ®ndings revealed that mentor transformational<br />

leadership was associated with increased prote ge receipt of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> <strong>and</strong> reduced<br />

prote ge <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong>. While mentor transactional contingent reward leadership also was<br />

found to be associated with increased prote ge receipt of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong>, the association was<br />

not as strong as that of transformational leadership <strong>and</strong> prote ge receipt of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong>.<br />

In addition, mentor laissez-faire leadership was found to be negatively <strong>related</strong> to prote ge receipt<br />

of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong>.<br />

This pattern of results supports prior research which highlights the ecacy of transformational<br />

leadership in developing subordinates (e.g., Bass, 1998; Sc<strong>and</strong>ura <strong>and</strong> Schreisheim, 1994; Sosik<br />

<strong>and</strong> Dionne, 1997; Yukl, 1994) <strong>and</strong> allaying subordinate <strong>stress</strong> (e.g., Seltzer et al., 1989). These<br />

results also are in line with results of Aryee, Chay, <strong>and</strong> Chew (1996), Burke et al. (1991), Burke<br />

(1984), <strong>and</strong> Mackey (1996) which suggested that e€ective <strong>mentoring</strong> relationships may be predicted<br />

by development-linked leadership qualities of the mentor. For example, Mackey (1996)<br />

identi®ed planning <strong>and</strong> goal setting, building prote ge self-ecacy, explaining the importance<br />

of the task in relation to the `big picture', <strong>and</strong> giving individualized attention as aspects of e€ective<br />

<strong>mentoring</strong>. These behaviors parallel those exhibited by transformational leaders who augment<br />

contingent reward behavior with inspiring, charismatic, <strong>and</strong> individually considerate behaviors<br />

(Bass, 1998).<br />

Why is transformational leadership displayed by a mentor e€ective in promoting prote ge receipt<br />

of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong>? One possible explanation involves emotional <strong>and</strong> self-concept development<br />

support. Speci®cally, Thibodeaux <strong>and</strong> Lowe (1996) found that in-group LMX individuals<br />

reported higher levels of emotional involvement <strong>and</strong> self-concept development <strong>mentoring</strong> outcomes<br />

than out-group LMX individuals. Given that transformational leadership promotes a<br />

strong emotional attachment of followers to the leader (Bass, 1985) <strong>and</strong> motivates individuals by<br />

developing <strong>and</strong> linking their self-concept to a collective mission (Shamir et al., 1993), results of the<br />

present study suggest that transformational leadership behavior displayed by mentors may<br />

facilitate the construction of perceptions of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> by prote ge s via emotional <strong>and</strong><br />

self-concept based mechanisms. As such, future research should explore the role of emotions <strong>and</strong><br />

the self-concept in <strong>mentoring</strong> relationships.<br />

An alternate explanation is provided by the <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>and</strong> leadership literatures. A<br />

common theme running through the <strong>mentoring</strong> literature (e.g., Burke, 1984; Kram, 1985;<br />

Noe, 1988) is that a mentor is viewed as a learned <strong>and</strong> trusted advisorÐa source of wisdom.<br />

Several leadership scholars (e.g., DiTomaso, 1993; Etzioni, 1961; Weber, 1947) have used the<br />

concept of wisdom to de®ne charisma, the largest component of variance in transformational<br />

leadership (Bass, 1998). Speci®cally, charisma can be viewed as the accumulation of basic truths,<br />

Copyright # 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 21, 365±390 (2000)


382 J. J. SOSIK AND V. M. GODSHALK<br />

purposes, <strong>and</strong> meanings via experiences <strong>and</strong> self-awareness over time (DiTomaso, 1993). In fact,<br />

Sosik <strong>and</strong> Dworakivsky (1998) found elements of leader wisdom (i.e., private self-awareness <strong>and</strong><br />

purpose-in-life) to be positively <strong>related</strong> to charismatic leadership. Given that a mentor shares<br />

experience, wisdom, knowledge <strong>and</strong> perspective with a prote ge (Kram, 1985), it may be that a<br />

mentor's charisma facilitates receipt of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> by the prote ge .<br />

To test this speculation, we performed a post-hoc test which involved correlating the components<br />

of transformational leadership (i.e., idealized in¯uence, inspirational motivation,<br />

intellectual stimulation, individualized consideration), contingent reward leadership, <strong>and</strong> laissezfaire<br />

leadership with psychosocial support <strong>and</strong> career development <strong>functions</strong> of <strong>mentoring</strong>.<br />

Results indicated that psychosocial support was positively <strong>related</strong> to two aspects of charismatic<br />

leadershipÐidealized in¯uence (r ˆ 0.21, p 5 0.01) <strong>and</strong> inspirational motivation (r ˆ 0.17,<br />

p 5 0.05)Ðas well as individualized consideration (r ˆ 0.18, p 5 0.04) <strong>and</strong> contingent reward<br />

(r ˆ 0.22, p 5 0.01). No leadership behaviors were associated with the career development<br />

function of <strong>mentoring</strong>. These results suggest that mentors who augment contingent reward<br />

behavior with charismatic <strong>and</strong> individually considerate behaviors may provide psychosocial<br />

support to prote ge s. Future research should identify which aspects of leadership or substitutes<br />

for leadership (e.g., prote ge development-linked reward systems <strong>and</strong> aspects of <strong>job</strong>s) may<br />

in¯uence the career development function of <strong>mentoring</strong> (Aryee et al., 1996).<br />

Two additional key ®ndings of the present study were that prote ge receipt of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong><br />

was negatively <strong>related</strong> to prote ge <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong> <strong>and</strong> that prote ge receipt of <strong>mentoring</strong><br />

<strong>functions</strong> strengthened the negative relationship between mentor transformational leadership<br />

behavior <strong>and</strong> prote ge <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong>. The former ®nding provides support for theoretical <strong>and</strong><br />

empirical indications that <strong>mentoring</strong> can allay <strong>stress</strong> experienced by prote ge s (e.g., House, 1981;<br />

Kram <strong>and</strong> Hall, 1989; Parker <strong>and</strong> DeCotiis, 1983). However, given that both <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong><br />

<strong>received</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong> were reported by prote ge s, this result should be interpreted with<br />

caution since common method variance may be a potential alternative explanation for<br />

this ®nding.<br />

The latter ®nding provides support for Bass (1998) <strong>and</strong> House (1981) who suggested that<br />

development-oriented leadership (i.e., transformational leadership) coupled with social support<br />

(i.e., <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> <strong>received</strong>) can reduce <strong>stress</strong> experienced by prote ge s. Thus, it may be<br />

that social support provided by <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>and</strong> the sense of identity with a social network of<br />

support emphasized in transformational leadership may help allay prote ge <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong>.<br />

Future research should explore the mechanisms by which transformational leadership <strong>and</strong><br />

prote ge receipt of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> may interact to allay prote ge <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong>.<br />

Implications<br />

Results of the present study suggest several managerial implications. First, to enhance prote geÂ<br />

receipt of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong>, mentors should be trained to avoid laissez-faire behaviors <strong>and</strong> to<br />

exhibit transformational leadership behaviors, such as idealized in¯uence <strong>and</strong> individualized<br />

consideration. Because contemporary management thought encourages providing autonomy to<br />

one's subordinates, mentors may fall into the trap of confusing empowerment with laissez-faire<br />

behaviors. To avoid such a folly, mentors should be trained to di€erentiate between empowerment<br />

<strong>and</strong> laissez-faire leadership. Empowerment encompasses providing psychological support<br />

<strong>and</strong> tangible resources to bolster autonomy <strong>and</strong> ecacy of prote ge e€orts. In contrast, laissezfaire<br />

represents the absence <strong>and</strong> abdication of mentor involvement in the mentor±prote geÂ<br />

relationship (Bass, 1998).<br />

Copyright # 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 21, 365±390 (2000)


LEADERSHIP, MENTORING AND STRESS 383<br />

Second, Senge (1996) argues that to be competitive in the next millennium, an organization's<br />

intellectual capital (i.e., knowledge resources <strong>and</strong> learning structures) should be mentored by<br />

leaders who possess wisdom. Given that charisma can be de®ned in terms of wisdom (DiTomaso,<br />

1993), our results suggest that mentors should focus on exhibiting idealized in¯uence <strong>and</strong><br />

inspirational motivation to build e€ective <strong>mentoring</strong> relationships. Both idealized in¯uence<br />

<strong>and</strong> inspirational motivation represent charismatic leadership, a relationship in which followers<br />

form a strong emotional attachment to the leader based on common internalized values <strong>and</strong><br />

identi®cation with the leader (Bass, 1985; House, 1996). To the extent that psychosocial support<br />

involves mentor±prote ge value congruence, prote ge respect <strong>and</strong> emulation of the mentor, <strong>and</strong><br />

mentor emotional encouragement of the prote ge (Kram, 1985; Thibodeaux <strong>and</strong> Lowe, 1996),<br />

study ®ndings suggest that idealized in¯uence <strong>and</strong> inspirational motivation may be useful<br />

behaviors for promoting <strong>mentoring</strong> e€ectiveness.<br />

Third, given increased levels of <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong> experienced by organizational members<br />

(Brockner et al., 1992), organizations should implement <strong>and</strong> support <strong>mentoring</strong> programmes to<br />

help allay employee <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong>. This implication is important since employee <strong>stress</strong> has<br />

been associated with signi®cant costs, both ®nancial (Laws, 1996) <strong>and</strong> health-<strong>related</strong> (Manning<br />

et al., 1996). Results of the present study suggest that organizations should couple <strong>mentoring</strong><br />

programmes with transformational leadership training for mentors to maximize reductions in<br />

prote ge <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong> (<strong>and</strong> its associated costs).<br />

Limitations <strong>and</strong> future research paths<br />

Certain limitations of the study, which are suggestive of future research paths, should be noted.<br />

First, the sample consisted of graduate student participants, who collectively represented<br />

employees from a wide variety of ages, backgrounds, <strong>and</strong> industries. Such a sample was judged<br />

preferable to using employees within the same organization due to the potential for data<br />

re¯ecting shared participant pool, organizationally-speci®c values, or <strong>mentoring</strong> relationships<br />

that may or may not be representative of the general population. Nevertheless, the limitations of<br />

generalizations from `convenient' sample data are acknowledged. Subsequent investigations<br />

could employ samples from speci®c organizations <strong>and</strong> industries.<br />

Second, MLQ-5X items employed in the present study to measure the laissez-faire construct<br />

may be subject to socially desirable responses. While prior research (Bass <strong>and</strong> Avolio, 1989) has<br />

indicated that the MLQ leadership measures are not likely to be associated with social desirability<br />

bias, the potential for such bias in self-reports on laissez-faire items cannot be ruled out.<br />

Future independent research should be conducted on potential social desirability bias in self <strong>and</strong><br />

other ratings of MLQ-5X items, especially those measuring laissez-faire leadership.<br />

Third, given that 91 per cent of our sample were involved in informal <strong>mentoring</strong> relationships,<br />

results of the present study are generalizable to informal <strong>mentoring</strong> relationships. Chao, Walz,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Gardner (1992) noted distinctions in process between formal <strong>and</strong> informal <strong>mentoring</strong><br />

relationships. Future research should replicate the present study using a sample comprised of<br />

primarily formal <strong>mentoring</strong> relationships.<br />

Fourth, the present study examined the linkage between mentor transformational leadership<br />

behavior <strong>and</strong> prote ge <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong> under high <strong>and</strong> low levels of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong><br />

<strong>received</strong>. Other important variables may potentially moderate this linkage. For example, future<br />

research could focus on how cross-gender (Burke <strong>and</strong> McKeen, 1990), <strong>and</strong>/or cross-cultural<br />

(Cox, 1993) issues may a€ect transformational leadership <strong>and</strong> <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong>. In addition,<br />

Copyright # 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 21, 365±390 (2000)


384 J. J. SOSIK AND V. M. GODSHALK<br />

subsequent studies could examine how stages of professional careers (Dalton, Thompson <strong>and</strong><br />

Price, 1977) may in¯uence this relationship.<br />

Despite these limitations, the present study o€ers some support for the proposed conceptual<br />

model <strong>and</strong> a preliminary empirical basis for comparison in future research. Given increased<br />

recognition of <strong>mentoring</strong> relationships as an antidote to <strong>stress</strong> (Kram <strong>and</strong> Hall, 1989), it is hoped<br />

that the present study has underscored the importance of appropriate mentor behavior in promoting<br />

e€ective <strong>mentoring</strong> relationships <strong>and</strong> reducing prote ge <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong>. In addition, it is<br />

hoped the present study will arouse researchers to examine mechanisms which can facilitate<br />

<strong>mentoring</strong> e€ectiveness <strong>and</strong> outcomes. Subsequent research should examine <strong>and</strong> re®ne the<br />

proposed model to further our knowledge concerning relationships among mentor leadership<br />

<strong>styles</strong>/behaviors, prote ge receipt of <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong>, <strong>and</strong> outcomes of <strong>mentoring</strong> such as<br />

reduced prote ge <strong>job</strong>-<strong>related</strong> <strong>stress</strong>.<br />

Acknowledgements<br />

The authors gratefully acknowledge the helpful suggestions of Denise Potosky, Eric Stein,<br />

George Watson <strong>and</strong> three anonymous reviewers on earlier versions of this paper, <strong>and</strong> the data<br />

collection <strong>and</strong> analysis support <strong>received</strong> from Georgia Gordon-Martin.<br />

References<br />

Appelbaum, S. H., Ritchie, S. <strong>and</strong> Shapiro, B. T. (1994). `Mentoring revisited: an organizational behaviour<br />

construct', International Journal of Career Management, 6, 3±10.<br />

Aryee, S., Chay, Y. W. <strong>and</strong> Chew, J. (1996). `The motivation to mentor among managerial employees',<br />

Group & Organization Management, 21, 261±277.<br />

Avolio, B. J., Bass, B. M. <strong>and</strong> Jung, D. I. (1997). Replicated Con®rmatory Factor Analyses of the Multifactor<br />

<strong>Leadership</strong> Questionnaire, Center for <strong>Leadership</strong> Studies, Binghamton University, Binghamton, NY.<br />

Bahniuk, M. H., Dobos, J. <strong>and</strong> Hill, S. E. (1990). `The impact of <strong>mentoring</strong>, collegial support, <strong>and</strong> information<br />

adequacy on career success: a replication', Journal of Social Behavior <strong>and</strong> Personality, 5, 431±451.<br />

B<strong>and</strong>ura, A. (1986). Social Foundations of Action <strong>and</strong> Thought: A Social Cognitive View, Prentice Hall,<br />

Englewood Cli€s, NJ.<br />

Bass, B. M. (1985). <strong>Leadership</strong> <strong>and</strong> Performance Beyond Expectations, Free Press, New York.<br />

Bass, B. M. (1990). Bass <strong>and</strong> Stodgill's H<strong>and</strong>book of <strong>Leadership</strong>, Free Press, New York.<br />

Bass, B. M. (1998). Transformational <strong>Leadership</strong>: Industry, Military, <strong>and</strong> Educational Impact, Lawrence<br />

Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ.<br />

Bass, B. M. <strong>and</strong> Avolio, B. J. (1989). `Potential biases in leadership measures: how prototypes, leniency, <strong>and</strong><br />

general satisfaction relate to ratings <strong>and</strong> rankings of transformational <strong>and</strong> transactional leadership<br />

constructs', Educational <strong>and</strong> Psychological Measurement, 49, 509±527.<br />

Bass, B. M. <strong>and</strong> Avolio, B. J. (1994). Improving Organizational E€ectiveness Through Transformational<br />

<strong>Leadership</strong>, Sage, Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks, CA.<br />

Bass, B. M. <strong>and</strong> Avolio, B. J. (1997). Full Range <strong>Leadership</strong> Development: Manual for the Multifactor<br />

<strong>Leadership</strong> Questionnaire, Mind Garden, Palo Alto, CA.<br />

Beehr, T. A. <strong>and</strong> Newman, J. E. (1978). `Job <strong>stress</strong>, employee health, <strong>and</strong> organizational e€ectiveness: a<br />

facet analysis, model <strong>and</strong> literature review', Personnel Psychology, 31, 665±699.<br />

Bennis, W. (1989). On Becoming a Leader, Simon & Schuster, New York.<br />

Copyright # 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 21, 365±390 (2000)


LEADERSHIP, MENTORING AND STRESS 385<br />

Bookstein, F. (1986). `The elements of latent variable models: a cautionary lecture'. In: Lamb, M., Brown,<br />

A. <strong>and</strong> Rogo€, B. (Eds) Advances in Developmental Psychology, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale,<br />

NJ, pp. 203±230.<br />

Brockner, J., Grover, S., Reed, T. F. <strong>and</strong> DeWitt, R. L. (1992). `Layo€s, <strong>job</strong> insecurity, <strong>and</strong> survivors' work<br />

e€ort: evidence of an inverted-U relationship', Academy of Management Journal, 35, 413±425.<br />

Bryman, A. (1992). Charisma <strong>and</strong> <strong>Leadership</strong> in Organizations, Sage Publications, London.<br />

Burke, R. J. (1984). `Mentors in organizations', Group & Organization Studies, 9, 353±372.<br />

Burke, R. J. <strong>and</strong> McKeen, C. A. (1990). `Mentoring in organizations: implications for women', Journal of<br />

Business Ethics, 9, 317±332.<br />

Burke, R. J., McKenna, C. S. <strong>and</strong> McKeen, C. A. (1991). `How do mentorships di€er from typical<br />

supervisory relationships?', Psychological Reports, 68, 459±466.<br />

Burns, J. M. (1978). <strong>Leadership</strong>, Harper & Row, New York.<br />

Bycio, P., Hackett, R. D. <strong>and</strong> Allen, J. S. (1995). `Further assessments of Bass's (1985) conceptualization of<br />

transactional <strong>and</strong> transformational leadership', Journal of Applied Psychology, 80, 468±478.<br />

Carmines, E. <strong>and</strong> Zeller, R. (1979). Reliability <strong>and</strong> Validity Assessment, Sage University Paper Series on<br />

Quantitative Applications in the Social Sciences, No. 07-017, Sage, Beverly Hills, CA.<br />

Champy, J. (1995). Reengineering Management, Harper Business, New York.<br />

Chao, G. T., Walz, P. M. <strong>and</strong> Gardner, P. (1992). `Formal <strong>and</strong> informal mentorships: a comparison<br />

on <strong>mentoring</strong> <strong>functions</strong> <strong>and</strong> contrast with nonmentored counterparts', Personnel Psychology, 45,<br />

619±636.<br />

Colarelli, S. M. <strong>and</strong> Bishop, R. C. (1990). `Career commitment: <strong>functions</strong>, correlates <strong>and</strong> management',<br />

Group & Organization Studies, 15, 158±176.<br />

Conger, J. A. <strong>and</strong> Kanungo, R. N. (1987). `Toward a behavioral theory of charismatic leadership'.<br />

In: Conger, J. A. <strong>and</strong> Kanungo, R. N. (Eds) Charismatic <strong>Leadership</strong>: The Elusive Factor in Organizational<br />

E€ectiveness, Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, CA, pp. 78±97.<br />

Covey, S. R. (1997). `Modeling <strong>and</strong> <strong>mentoring</strong>', Executive Excellence, 14, 3±4.<br />

Cox, T. (1993). Cultural Diversity in Organizations: Theory, Research & Practice, Berrett-Koehler<br />

Publishers, San Francisco, CA.<br />

Curphy, G. J. (1992). `An empirical investigation of the e€ects of transformational <strong>and</strong> transactional<br />

leadership on organizational climate, attrition <strong>and</strong> performance'. In: Clark, K. E., Clark,<br />

M. B. <strong>and</strong> Campbell, D. R. (Eds) Impact of <strong>Leadership</strong>, The Center for Creative <strong>Leadership</strong>,<br />

Greensboro, NC.<br />

Dalton, G. W., Thompson, P. H. <strong>and</strong> Price, R. L. (1977). `The four stages of professional careers: A new<br />

look at performance by professionals', Organizational Dynamics, 6, 19±42.<br />

DiTomaso, N. (1993). `Weber's social history <strong>and</strong> Etzioni's structural theory of charisma in organizations:<br />

implications for thinking about charismatic leadership', <strong>Leadership</strong> Quarterly, 4, 257±275.<br />

Dreher, G. F. <strong>and</strong> Ash, R. A. (1990). `A comparative study of <strong>mentoring</strong> among men <strong>and</strong> women in<br />

managerial, professional, <strong>and</strong> technical positions', Journal of Applied Psychology, 75, 539±546.<br />

Dubinsky, A. J., Yammarino, F. J. <strong>and</strong> Jolson, M. A. (1995). `An examination of linkages between<br />

personality characteristics <strong>and</strong> dimensions of transformational leadership', Journal of Business <strong>and</strong><br />

Psychology, 9, 315±335.<br />

Duxbury, R. M. <strong>and</strong> Higgins, C. A. (1991). `Gender di€erences in work±family con¯ict', Journal of Applied<br />

Psychology, 76, 60±74.<br />

Etzioni, A. (1961). A Comparative Analysis of Complex Organizations, The Free Press, New York.<br />

Fagenson, E. A. (1989). `The mentor advantage: perceived career/<strong>job</strong> experiences of prote ge s versus<br />

nonprote ge s', Journal of Organizational Behavior, 10, 309±320.<br />

Falk, R. F. <strong>and</strong> Miller, N. B. (1992). A Primer for Soft Modeling, The University of Akron Press,<br />

Akron, OH.<br />

Fiedler, F. E. (1967). A Theory of <strong>Leadership</strong> E€ectiveness, McGraw-Hill, New York.<br />

Fornell, C. <strong>and</strong> Larcker, D. (1981). `Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables <strong>and</strong><br />

measurement error', Journal of Marketing Research, 18, 39±50.<br />

Gibson, V. M. (1993). `Stress in the workplace: a hidden cost factor', HR Focus, 70, 15±16.<br />

Gladstone, M. S. (1988). Mentoring: A Strategy for Learning in a Rapidly Changing Society, CEGEP John<br />

Abbot College, Research <strong>and</strong> Development Secretariat, Montreal.<br />

Graen, G. B. <strong>and</strong> Sc<strong>and</strong>ura, T. A. (1986). `A theory of dyadic career reality', Research in Personnel <strong>and</strong><br />

Human Resource Management, 4, 147±181.<br />

House, J. S. (1981). Work Stress <strong>and</strong> Social Support, Addison-Wesley, Reading, MA.<br />

Copyright # 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 21, 365±390 (2000)


386 J. J. SOSIK AND V. M. GODSHALK<br />

House, R. J. (1996). `Path±goal theory of leadership: lessons, legacy, <strong>and</strong> a reformulated theory', <strong>Leadership</strong><br />

Quarterly, 7, 323±352.<br />

House, R. J., Spangler, W. D. <strong>and</strong> Woycke, J. (1991). `Personality <strong>and</strong> charisma in the U.S. presidency: a<br />

psychological theory of leader e€ectiveness', Administrative Science Quarterly, 36, 364±396.<br />

Howell, J. M. <strong>and</strong> Avolio, B. J. (1993). `Predicting consolidated unit performance: leadership behavior,<br />

locus of control <strong>and</strong> support for innovation', Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 891±902.<br />

Howell, J. M. <strong>and</strong> Higgins, C. A. (1990). `Champions of technological innovations', Administrative Science<br />

Quarterly, 35, 317±341.<br />

Jamal, M. (1990). `Relationship of <strong>job</strong> <strong>stress</strong> <strong>and</strong> Type-A behavior to employees' <strong>job</strong> satisfaction,<br />

organizational commitment, psychosomatic health problems, <strong>and</strong> turnover motivation', Human<br />

Relations, 43, 727±738.<br />

Kahai, S. S., Sosik, J. J. <strong>and</strong> Avolio, B. J. (1997). `E€ects of leadership style <strong>and</strong> problem structure on work<br />

group process <strong>and</strong> outcomes in an electronic meeting system environment', Personnel Psychology, 50,<br />

121±146.<br />

Kalay, E. (1983). The Comm<strong>and</strong>er in Stress Situations in IDF Combat Units During the `Peace for Galilee'<br />

Campaign. Paper, Third International Conference on Psychological Stress <strong>and</strong> Adjustment in Time of<br />

War <strong>and</strong> Peace, Tel Aviv, Israel.<br />

Katz, R. (1977). `The in¯uence of group con¯ict on leadership e€ectiveness', Organizational Behavior <strong>and</strong><br />

Human Performance, 20, 265±286.<br />

Kirkpatrick, S. A. <strong>and</strong> Locke, E. A. (1996). `Direct <strong>and</strong> indirect e€ects of three core charismatic leadership<br />

components on performance <strong>and</strong> attitudes', Journal of Applied Psychology, 19, 71±75.<br />

Kram, K. E. (1985). Mentoring at Work, Scott, Foresman <strong>and</strong> Company, Glenview, IL.<br />

Kram, K. E. <strong>and</strong> Bragar, M. C. (1991). `Development through <strong>mentoring</strong>: a strategic approach for the<br />

1990s'. In: Montross, D. H. <strong>and</strong> Shinkman, C. J. (Eds) Career Development in the 1990s: Theory <strong>and</strong><br />

Practice, Charles C. Thomas, Spring®eld, Ill.<br />

Kram, K. E. <strong>and</strong> Hall, D. T. (1989). `Mentoring as an antidote to <strong>stress</strong> during corporate trauma', Human<br />

Resource Management, 24, 493±510.<br />

Kugihara, N. <strong>and</strong> Misumi, J. (1984). `An experimental study of the e€ect of leadership types on followers'<br />

escaping behaviors in a fearful emergency maze-situation', Japanese Journal of Psychology, 55, 214±220.<br />

LaRocco, J. M. <strong>and</strong> Jones, A. P. (1978). `Co-worker <strong>and</strong> leader support as moderators of <strong>stress</strong>±strain<br />

relationships in work situations', Journal of Applied Psychology, 63, 629±634.<br />

Laws, J. (1996). `The enormous cost of <strong>job</strong> <strong>stress</strong>', Occupational Health & Safety, 65, 4±5.<br />

Lowe, K. B., Kroeck, K. G. <strong>and</strong> Sivasubramaniam, N. (1996). `E€ectiveness correlates of transformational<br />

<strong>and</strong> transactional leadership: a meta-analytic review', <strong>Leadership</strong> Quarterly, 7, 385±425.<br />

Mackey, J. (1996). `The <strong>mentoring</strong> role: instruction by example'. In: Hendricks, W. (Ed.) Coaching,<br />

Mentoring, <strong>and</strong> Managing, Career Press, Franklin Lakes, NJ, pp. 127±156.<br />

Manning, M. R., Jackson, C. N. <strong>and</strong> Fusilier, M. R. (1996). `Occupational <strong>stress</strong>, social support, <strong>and</strong> the<br />

costs of health care', Academy of Management Journal, 39, 738±750.<br />

Matteson, M. T. <strong>and</strong> Ivancevich, J. M. (1982). Managing Job-Stress <strong>and</strong> Health, Free Press, New York.<br />

McCauley, C. D. (1987). `Stress <strong>and</strong> the eye of the beholder', Issues & Observations, 7, 1±16.<br />

Mintzberg, H. (1973). The Nature of Managerial Work, Harper & Row, New York.<br />

Morgan, R. B. (1989). `Reliability <strong>and</strong> validity of a factor analytically derived measure of leadership<br />

behavior <strong>and</strong> characteristics', Educational <strong>and</strong> Psychological Measurement, 49, 911±919.<br />

Motowidlo, S. J., Packard, J. S. <strong>and</strong> Manning, M. R. (1986). `Occupational <strong>stress</strong>: its causes <strong>and</strong><br />

consequences for <strong>job</strong> performance', Journal of Applied Psychology, 71, 618±629.<br />

Mowday, R. T., Porter, L. W. <strong>and</strong> Steers, R. M. (1982). Employee±Organizational Linkages: The<br />

Psychology of Commitment, Absenteeism <strong>and</strong> Turnover, Academic Press, New York.<br />

Mulder, M., van Eck, R. <strong>and</strong> deJong, R. D. (1971). `An organization in crisis <strong>and</strong> noncrisis situations',<br />

Human Relations, 24, 19±41.<br />

Murray, M. (1991). Beyond the Myths <strong>and</strong> Magic of Mentoring, Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, CA.<br />

Noe, R. (1988). `An investigation of the determinants of successful assigned <strong>mentoring</strong> relationships',<br />

Personnel Psychology, 41, 457±479.<br />

Parasuraman, S., Greenhaus, J. H. <strong>and</strong> Granrose, C. S. (1992). `Role <strong>stress</strong>ors, social support, <strong>and</strong> wellbeing<br />

among two-career couples', Journal of Organizational Behavior, 13, 339±356.<br />

Parker, D. F. <strong>and</strong> DeCotiis, T. A. (1983). `Organizational determinants of <strong>job</strong> <strong>stress</strong>', Organizational<br />

Behavior <strong>and</strong> Human Performance, 32, 160±177.<br />

Copyright # 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 21, 365±390 (2000)


LEADERSHIP, MENTORING AND STRESS 387<br />

Patterson, C., Fuller, J. B., Kester, K. <strong>and</strong> Stringer, D. Y. (1995). A Meta-Analytic Examination of<br />

<strong>Leadership</strong> Style <strong>and</strong> Selected Compliance Outcomes. (May) Paper presented at the Society for Industrial<br />

<strong>and</strong> Organizational Psychology, Orl<strong>and</strong>o, FL.<br />

Podsako€, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Morman, R. H. <strong>and</strong> Fetter, R. (1990). `Transformational leadership<br />

behaviors <strong>and</strong> their e€ects on follower's trust in leader, satisfaction, <strong>and</strong> organizational citizenship<br />

behaviors', <strong>Leadership</strong> Quarterly, 1, 107±142.<br />

Ragins, B. R. <strong>and</strong> Cotton, J. L. (1993). `Gender <strong>and</strong> willingness to mentor in organizations', Journal of<br />

Management, 19, 97±111.<br />

Sambamurthy, V. <strong>and</strong> Chin, W. W. (1994). `The e€ects of group attitudes toward alternative GDSS<br />

designs on the decision-making performance of computer-supported groups', Decision Sciences, 25,<br />

215±241.<br />

Sashkin, M. <strong>and</strong> Rosenbach, W. E. (1993). `A new leadership paradigm'. In: Rosenbach, W. E. <strong>and</strong> Taylor,<br />

R. L. (Eds) Contemporary Issues in <strong>Leadership</strong>, Westview Press, Boulder, CO, pp. 87±108.<br />

Sc<strong>and</strong>ura, T. (1992). `Mentorship <strong>and</strong> career mobility: an empirical investigation', Journal of Organizational<br />

Behavior, 13, 169±174.<br />

Sc<strong>and</strong>ura, T. A. <strong>and</strong> Schriesheim, C. A. (1994). `Leader±member exchange <strong>and</strong> supervisor career<br />

<strong>mentoring</strong> as complementary constructs in leadership research', Academy of Management Journal, 37,<br />

1588±1602.<br />

Schein, E. H. (1978). Career Dynamics: Matching Individual <strong>and</strong> Organizational Needs, Addison-Wesley,<br />

Reading, MA.<br />

Schuler, R. S. (1980). `De®nition <strong>and</strong> conceptualization of <strong>stress</strong> in organizations', Organizational Behavior<br />

<strong>and</strong> Human Performance, 25, 184±215.<br />

Seltzer, J., Numero€, R. E. <strong>and</strong> Bass, B. M. (1989). `Transformational leadership: is it a source of more or<br />

less burnout or <strong>stress</strong>?', Journal of Health <strong>and</strong> Human Resource Administration, 12, 174±185.<br />

Senge, P. (1996). `Leading learning organizations', Executive Excellence, 13, 10±11.<br />

Shamir, B., House, R. <strong>and</strong> Arthur, M. (1993). `The motivational e€ects of charismatic leadership: a selfconcept<br />

based theory', Organization Science, 4, 1±17.<br />

Sims, H. <strong>and</strong> Lorenzi, P. (1992). The New <strong>Leadership</strong> Paradigm, Sage Publications, Newberry Park, CA.<br />

Smircich, L. <strong>and</strong> Morgan, G. (1982). `<strong>Leadership</strong>: the Management of Meaning', Journal of Applied<br />

Behavioral Science, 18, 257±273.<br />

Sosik, J. J. <strong>and</strong> Dionne, S. D. (1997). `<strong>Leadership</strong> <strong>styles</strong> <strong>and</strong> Deming's behavior factors', Journal of Business<br />

<strong>and</strong> Psychology, 11, 447±462.<br />

Sosik, J. J. <strong>and</strong> Dworakivsky, A. C. (1998) `Self-concept based aspects of the charismatic leader: more than<br />

meets the eye', <strong>Leadership</strong> Quarterly, 9, 503±526.<br />

Sosik, J. J., Avolio, B. J. <strong>and</strong> Kahai, S. S. (1997). `E€ects of leadership style <strong>and</strong> anonymity on group<br />

potency <strong>and</strong> e€ectiveness in a group decision support system environment', Journal of Applied<br />

Psychology, 82, 89±103.<br />

Stodgill, R. M. (1963). Manual for the Leader Behavior QuestionnaireÐForm XII, Bureau of Business<br />

Research, Columbus: Ohio State University.<br />

Sullivan, S. E. <strong>and</strong> Bhagat, R. S. (1992). `Organizational <strong>stress</strong>, <strong>job</strong> satisfaction <strong>and</strong> <strong>job</strong> performance: where<br />

do we go from here?', Journal of Management, 18, 353±374.<br />

Thibodeaux, H. F. <strong>and</strong> Lowe, R. H. (1996). `Convergence of leader±member exchange <strong>and</strong> <strong>mentoring</strong>: an<br />

investigation of social in¯uence patterns', Journal of Social Behavior <strong>and</strong> Personality, 11, 97±114.<br />

Torrance, E. P. (1983). `Role of mentors in creative achievement', The Creative Child <strong>and</strong> Adult Quarterly,<br />

8, 8±18.<br />

Viator, R. E. <strong>and</strong> Sc<strong>and</strong>ura, T. (1991). `A study of mentor±prote ge relationships in large public accounting<br />

®rms', Accounting Horizons, 5, 20±30.<br />

Watson, B. M. (1984). `Lawrence, KansasÐbefore <strong>and</strong> after `the day after'', Public Management, 66,<br />

13±15.<br />

Weber, M. (1947). In: Parsons, T. (Ed.) The Theory of Social <strong>and</strong> Economic Organization, translated by<br />

Henderson A. M., Parsons T, The Free Press, New York.<br />

Wilson, J. A. <strong>and</strong> Elman, N. S. (1990). `Organizational bene®ts of <strong>mentoring</strong>', Academy of Management<br />

Executive, 4, 88±93.<br />

Wold, H. (1985). `Systems analysis by Partial Least Squares'. In: Nijkamp, P., Leitner, H. <strong>and</strong> Wrigley, N.<br />

(Eds) Measuring the Unmeasurable, Martinus Nijho€, Dordrecht, pp. 221±252.<br />

Yammarino, F. J. <strong>and</strong> Bass, B. M. (1990). `Long-term forecasting of transformational leadership <strong>and</strong> its<br />

e€ects among Naval ocers: some preliminary ®ndings'. In: Clark, K. E. <strong>and</strong> Clark, M. R. (Eds)<br />

Copyright # 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 21, 365±390 (2000)


388 J. J. SOSIK AND V. M. GODSHALK<br />

Measures of <strong>Leadership</strong>, Greensboro, NC: Center for Creative <strong>Leadership</strong>, <strong>Leadership</strong> Library of<br />

America, West Orange, NJ.<br />

Yammarino, F. J., Dubinsky, A. J., Comer, L. B. <strong>and</strong> Jolson, M. A. (1997). `Women <strong>and</strong> transformational<br />

<strong>and</strong> contingent reward leadership: a multiple-levels-of-analysis perspective', Academy of Management<br />

Journal, 40, 205±222.<br />

Yukl, G. (1990). Skills for Managers <strong>and</strong> Leaders, Prentice Hall, Englewood Cli€s, NJ.<br />

Yukl, G. (1994). <strong>Leadership</strong> in Organizations, Prentice Hall, Englewood Cli€s, NJ.<br />

Appendix A<br />

Overview of Partial Least Squares (PLS) analysis<br />

PLS enables analysis of complex nomological networks of constructs, as in the present study,<br />

which would be impossible or dicult in the context of traditional techniques (Falk <strong>and</strong> Miller,<br />

1992). PLS does not make assumptions about (a) data distributions to estimate model<br />

parameters, (b) observation independence, or (c) variable metrics (Fornell <strong>and</strong> Larcker, 1981).<br />

Because of its less restrictive assumptions, PLS is highly suitable for the early stages of theory<br />

building <strong>and</strong> testing <strong>and</strong>/or when sample sizes are small (Falk <strong>and</strong> Miller, 1992; Wold, 1985).<br />

This feature of PLS makes it suitable even over LISREL, another structural modeling technique,<br />

which is suitable for con®rmatory stages of theory building <strong>and</strong> testing (Falk <strong>and</strong> Miller, 1992).<br />

PLS simultaneously assess both the structural component, representing the relationship among<br />

constructs, <strong>and</strong> the measurement component, representing the relationship between constructs<br />

<strong>and</strong> their measures (Fornell <strong>and</strong> Larcker, 1981). The simultaneous analysis of structural <strong>and</strong><br />

measurement components facilitates measurement reliability <strong>and</strong> validity assessments within the<br />

context of the theoretical model being tested. Thus, in accordance with contemporary philosophy<br />

of science views (Falk <strong>and</strong> Miller, 1992), PLS acknowledges that psychometric properties of<br />

measures derive their meaning from the nomological network of relationships in which the<br />

measures are employed. In traditional analysis, assessment of the measurements component is<br />

performed separately from the relationship among constructs (e.g., Cronbach's are estimated for<br />

scales of constructs ®rst to assess their reliability <strong>and</strong> then these scales are used in regression<br />

analysis to examine the relationship among constructs). Results generated by PLS can be<br />

interpreted by considering them in the context of regression <strong>and</strong> principal components factor<br />

analysis (Bookstein, 1986). PLS generates estimates of st<strong>and</strong>ardized regression path coecients,<br />

computes R 2 (i.e., proportion of variance explained) for endogenous constructs, <strong>and</strong> produces<br />

factor loadings <strong>and</strong> weights of indicators of constructs. The weights of indicators are the<br />

regression coecients by which the st<strong>and</strong>ardized scores of the measures of the construct are<br />

multiplied before being summed up to obtain factor scores for the construct.<br />

(Adapted from Sosik, J. J., Avolio, B. J., & Kahai, S. S. (1998) Inspiring group creativity. Small<br />

Group Research, 29(1), 3±31. Reprinted by permission of Sage Publications, Inc.)<br />

Copyright # 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 21, 365±390 (2000)


LEADERSHIP, MENTORING AND STRESS 389<br />

Appendix B<br />

Questionnaire items<br />

Laissez-faire<br />

I am absent when needed.<br />

I delay responding to urgent questions.<br />

Transactional contingent reward<br />

I discuss in speci®c terms who is responsible for achieving performance targets.<br />

I make it clear what one can expect to receive when performance goals are achieved.<br />

I express satisfaction when others meet expectations.<br />

Transformational leadership<br />

Inspirational motivation<br />

I talk optimistically about the future.<br />

I talk enthusiastically about what needs to be accomplished.<br />

I articulate a compelling vision of the future.<br />

I express con®dence that goals will be achieved.<br />

Idealized in¯uence<br />

I talk about my most important values <strong>and</strong> beliefs.<br />

I specify the importance of having a strong sense of purpose.<br />

I consider the moral <strong>and</strong> ethical consequences of decisions.<br />

I emphasize the importance of having a collective sense of mission.<br />

Intellectual stimulation<br />

I re-examine critical assumptions to question whether they are appropriate.<br />

I seek di€ering perspectives when solving problems.<br />

I get others to look at problems from many di€erent angles.<br />

I suggest new ways of looking at how to complete assignments.<br />

Individualized consideration<br />

I spend time teaching <strong>and</strong> coaching.<br />

I treat others as individuals rather than just as a member of a group.<br />

I consider the individual as having di€erent needs, abilities <strong>and</strong> aspirations from others.<br />

I help others develop their strengths.<br />

Mentoring <strong>functions</strong><br />

Psychosocial support<br />

I agree with my mentors' attitudes <strong>and</strong> values regarding my career.<br />

I respect <strong>and</strong> admire my mentor.<br />

I will try to be like my mentor when I reach a similar position in my career.<br />

My mentor has demonstrated good listening skills in our conversations.<br />

My mentor has discussed my questions or concerns regarding feelings of competence,<br />

commitment to advancement, relationships with peers <strong>and</strong> supervisors or work/family<br />

con¯icts.<br />

My mentor has encouraged me to prepare for advancements.<br />

My mentor has encouraged me to talk openly about my anxiety <strong>and</strong> fears that detracts from<br />

my work.<br />

My mentor has conveyed empathy for the concerns <strong>and</strong> feelings that I have discussed with<br />

him/her.<br />

My mentor has conveyed feelings of respect for me as an individual.<br />

My mentor has encouraged me to try new ways of behaving in my <strong>job</strong>.<br />

Copyright # 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 21, 365±390 (2000)


390 J. J. SOSIK AND V. M. GODSHALK<br />

Career development<br />

Mentor helped you to ®nish assignments/tasks to meet deadlines that otherwise would have<br />

been dicult to complete.<br />

Mentor gave you assignments that increased written <strong>and</strong> personal contact with upper<br />

management.<br />

Mentor assigned responsibilities to you that have increased your contact with managers who<br />

may judge your potential for future advancement.<br />

Mentor gave you assignments or tasks in your work that prepare you for an advanced<br />

position.<br />

Mentor gave you assignments that present opportunities to learn new skills.<br />

Mentor reduced unnecessary risk that could be a detriment.<br />

Mentor provided you with feedback regarding your performance.<br />

Job <strong>stress</strong><br />

Your <strong>job</strong> makes you upset.<br />

Your <strong>job</strong> makes you frustrated.<br />

You are under strain on the <strong>job</strong>.<br />

Your <strong>job</strong> makes you tense.<br />

The amount of work you have to do interferes with how well it gets done.<br />

Your <strong>job</strong> places you under a great deal of <strong>stress</strong>.<br />

Your <strong>job</strong> makes you jumpy <strong>and</strong> nervous.<br />

Your <strong>job</strong> puts you under a lot of pressure.<br />

Copyright # 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. J. Organiz. Behav. 21, 365±390 (2000)

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!