03.07.2014 Views

Report on methods and classification of stress, inattention - sensation

Report on methods and classification of stress, inattention - sensation

Report on methods and classification of stress, inattention - sensation

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

D1.1.2<br />

C<strong>on</strong>tract N. IST-507231<br />

One problem is that there is no definiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> chr<strong>on</strong>ic <strong>stress</strong>. For example, should a <strong>stress</strong>ful<br />

m<strong>on</strong>th be labeled as a chr<strong>on</strong>ic <strong>stress</strong> situati<strong>on</strong>? Most experts would probably refer to l<strong>on</strong>ger<br />

time periods with <strong>stress</strong> exposure in order to define it as a chr<strong>on</strong>ic <strong>stress</strong> situati<strong>on</strong>. With<br />

respect to the physiological resp<strong>on</strong>ses <strong>on</strong>e could expect that chr<strong>on</strong>ic <strong>stress</strong> should result in<br />

hyperarousal, e.g. elevated levels <strong>of</strong> heart rate, blood pressure, cortisol etc, which is observed<br />

in depressive <strong>and</strong> anxiety disorders (Grings & Daws<strong>on</strong>, 1978). There is some evidence<br />

supporting this hypothesis. Some studies <strong>on</strong> prol<strong>on</strong>ged occupati<strong>on</strong>al <strong>stress</strong> show that high job<br />

strain (characterized by high work dem<strong>and</strong>s <strong>and</strong> effort, <strong>and</strong> low c<strong>on</strong>trol <strong>and</strong> reward) is<br />

associated with elevated blood pressure (Theorell, Ahlberg-Hulten, Jodko, Sigala & de la<br />

Torre, 1993; Pickering, 2000; Steptoe, Siegrist, Kirschbaum & Marmot, 2004). However,<br />

what is perhaps more interesting is that the elevated levels not <strong>on</strong>ly occur during work, but<br />

also during free time <strong>and</strong> sleep. Thus, chr<strong>on</strong>ic <strong>stress</strong> seems to result in elevated levels<br />

throughout the 24h period <strong>and</strong> not <strong>on</strong>ly during work. The reas<strong>on</strong> for this could be difficulties<br />

to shut <strong>of</strong>f the <strong>stress</strong> activati<strong>on</strong> systems. Similar findings have been obtained in studies <strong>of</strong><br />

extended periods with overtime work. Rissler (1977) observed that during a period <strong>of</strong> intense<br />

overtime, epinephrine increased in the evening after work.<br />

However, the physiological <strong>stress</strong> marker that has been mostly studied in c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong> with<br />

chr<strong>on</strong>ic <strong>stress</strong> is cortisol (Melamed, Ugarten, Shirom, Kahana, Lerman & Froom, 1999). The<br />

results for this marker are somewhat inc<strong>on</strong>sistent, <strong>and</strong> some studies suggest that chr<strong>on</strong>ic <strong>stress</strong><br />

may be associated with decreased cortisol levels (a state <strong>of</strong> hypoarousal) instead <strong>of</strong> the<br />

expected increase (Yehuda, 1997; Zarkovic et al., 2003). For example, subjects that score<br />

high <strong>on</strong> burnout have showed decreased morning cortisol (Pruessner, Hellhammer &<br />

Kirschbaum, 1999). It has been hypothesized that the decrease in cortisol may be related to<br />

subjective feelings <strong>of</strong> exhausti<strong>on</strong>. There are also studies showing that the temporal pattern <strong>of</strong><br />

cortisol changes during chr<strong>on</strong>ic <strong>stress</strong> situati<strong>on</strong>s (Rosm<strong>on</strong>d, Dallman, & Björntorp, 1998).<br />

Chr<strong>on</strong>ic <strong>stress</strong> may also be reflected in markers <strong>of</strong> the immune system. For example a study<br />

by Grossi <strong>and</strong> coworkers showed enhanced inflammatory resp<strong>on</strong>ses (higher levels <strong>of</strong> TNFalpha)<br />

in a high burnout group (Grossi, Perski, Evengård, Blomkvist & Orth-Gomér, 2003).<br />

It has also been suggested that the analyses <strong>of</strong> temporal changes in the balance between SAM<br />

system <strong>and</strong> the HPA axis may enhance the characterizati<strong>on</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>stress</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>ses <strong>and</strong> <strong>stress</strong><br />

related illnesses (Nicolaidis, 2002). Another interesting issue is whether chr<strong>on</strong>ic <strong>stress</strong> affects<br />

the acute <strong>stress</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se. It may be possible that the acute <strong>stress</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se becomes weaker<br />

after chr<strong>on</strong>ic <strong>stress</strong> exposure (Kristens<strong>on</strong> et al., 1998).<br />

3.5 Sleep <strong>and</strong> <strong>stress</strong><br />

Disturbed sleep is probably <strong>on</strong>e <strong>of</strong> the most sensitive markers <strong>of</strong> <strong>stress</strong>. Stress is also regarded<br />

as the most frequent cause <strong>of</strong> short-term insomnia, although it has seldom been documented<br />

with polysomnographic recordings (Roehrs, Zorick & Roth, 1989). For example, occupati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

<strong>stress</strong> is a comm<strong>on</strong> predictor <strong>of</strong> disturbed sleep (Kalimo, Tenkanen, Härmä, Poppius &<br />

Heinsalmi, 2000; Åkerstedt, Knutss<strong>on</strong>, Westerholm, Theorell, Alfredss<strong>on</strong> & Kecklund, 2002)<br />

<strong>and</strong> even some <strong>of</strong> the questi<strong>on</strong>naires <strong>on</strong> occupati<strong>on</strong>al <strong>stress</strong> include questi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>of</strong> sleep<br />

complaints. Also, studies <strong>on</strong> anticipatory <strong>stress</strong> have shown associati<strong>on</strong>s with disturbed sleep,<br />

e.g. reduced amounts <strong>of</strong> slow wave sleep (Torsvall & Åkerstedt, 1988a; Kecklund &<br />

Åkerstedt, 2004). The mechanism seems to involve elevated physiological activati<strong>on</strong> at<br />

bedtime <strong>and</strong> during the night. Hyperarousal is also a comm<strong>on</strong> observati<strong>on</strong> am<strong>on</strong>g many<br />

insomniacs <strong>and</strong> their negative attitude to sleep may act as a chr<strong>on</strong>ic <strong>stress</strong>or for these<br />

individuals (Vg<strong>on</strong>tzas, Bixler, & Kales, 2000). Interestingly, some measures <strong>of</strong> chr<strong>on</strong>ic <strong>stress</strong>,<br />

such as burnout, seem to c<strong>on</strong>sistently be associated with self-reported sleep disturbances<br />

30/08/2004 12<br />

Karolinska Institutet

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!