17.06.2014 Views

printer-friendly version (PDF) - Reflex Sympathetic Dystrophy ...

printer-friendly version (PDF) - Reflex Sympathetic Dystrophy ...

printer-friendly version (PDF) - Reflex Sympathetic Dystrophy ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Functional (f)MRIs of subjects in a state of mind characterized<br />

by grief reveal activation of the posterior cingulate cortex<br />

(20). Recent observations suggest that a predisposition to<br />

depression is related to abnormal (and variably asymmetrical)<br />

feedback circuits that link the ACC and the amygdala, which is<br />

responsible for the experience of fear (21).<br />

Activation of the ACC is observed in experiments in conflict<br />

resolution (22). In fMRI studies by Joshua Greene and colleagues,<br />

subjects who elect a rational solution to a moral<br />

dilemma also show activation of the ACC as a marker of “conflict,”<br />

along with activation of areas of the brain associated with<br />

cognitive processing (23, 24). My response to Greene’s summary<br />

of data and interpretations from the “crying baby” 8 experiments<br />

supports my theory (25). After reading the “crying baby<br />

dilemma,” I concluded, with difficulty, that I would select the<br />

rational solution: I would smother my own crying baby rather<br />

than expose the hiding place of my fellow villagers to a nearby,<br />

wartime enemy that would surely kill us all. The emotional resolution<br />

to the dilemma (do not smother the baby regardless of<br />

the consequences) is “easier” (Greene observed shorter reaction<br />

times). After reading Greene’s article (25), I was unable to sleep,<br />

distressed by thoughts of my own children and six-month-old<br />

granddaughter and by images of William Styron’s Sophie 9 , mortally<br />

suffering for the choice she had made. I speculated that<br />

the rational resolution to the crying baby dilemma requires<br />

more time and produced my own reaction, in part, because the<br />

somatic markers for decision-making in the crying baby<br />

dilemma include those of suffering. My painful obsessions and<br />

insomnia resulted from fear and grief—the emotional cost of<br />

my rational choice—even though the stimulus was fictional,<br />

just like the one in Styron’s novel and the subsequent movie. To<br />

quote Goethe, “To act is easy, to think is hard, to act according<br />

to our thought is troublesome.” 10 PREMISE 5<br />

The evolution of suffering is essential to the<br />

communication of emotion and to mammalian<br />

nurturing.<br />

THE QUALITIES OF SUFFERING that result in withdrawal, isolation,<br />

and stasis—misery, hopelessness, despair, torment,<br />

anguish, etc.—are grievous and fearsome indeed. In the evolution<br />

of species, such qualities and their resulting inaction would<br />

appear to impart a serious survival and reproductive disadvantage.<br />

It would follow, then, that a capacity for suffering should<br />

be extinguished by natural selection. Why, then, does suffering<br />

exist with such negative effects on the human condition? 11 I<br />

propose that suffering evolved, paradoxically, to impart survival<br />

and reproductive advantage.<br />

The increasing size of the brain relative to body mass (a<br />

development that paleontologists and physical anthropologists<br />

call “encephalization” 12 ) imparts survival and reproductive<br />

advantage (26). Increasing encephalization required that offspring<br />

are born neurologically immature, which is a serious survival<br />

disadvantage. 13 I propose that the resolution to the<br />

apparent paradox is the evolution of nurturing: an altruistic<br />

behavior that permits an individual to cooperate with another<br />

and relinquish its independence for the survival of the offspring.<br />

According to Paul MacLean, the transition from fish,<br />

amphibians, and reptiles—many, but not all, of which abandon<br />

their young—to mammals that protect them, play with them,<br />

have distinctive cries of separation and grieve for their loss,<br />

requires the paleopallium, the midbrain, the limbic system,<br />

specifically the circuit of Papez and the ACC (27). I propose<br />

that the full complement of primary emotions, mediated by the<br />

paleopallium, is the currency of mammalian nurturing and its<br />

invariable sequence: attachment, individuation, and separation.<br />

Suffering and empathy are the means of receipt and distribution<br />

of that currency. 14 I conclude, therefore, that suffering and<br />

8 “It is wartime, and you and some of your fellow villagers are hiding from<br />

enemy soldiers in a basement. Your baby starts to cry, and you cover your<br />

baby’s mouth to block the sound. If you remove your hand, your baby will<br />

cry loudly, the soldiers will hear, and they will find you and the others and<br />

kill everyone they find, including your baby. If you do not remove your<br />

hand, your baby will smother to death. Is it okay to smother your baby to<br />

death in order to save yourself and the other villagers?” (25)<br />

9 Meryl Streep played Sophie in the film adaptation of William Styron’s<br />

novel Sophie’s Choice. Sophie, upon arriving at a Nazi concentration camp,<br />

must choose between her two children. One will be taken from her and,<br />

she assumes, killed. She is allowed to keep the other child with her. Neither<br />

child survives. Sophie, years later, in the United States, commits suicide.<br />

10 From Wilhelm Meister’s Apprenticeship, Book VII, chapter 9.<br />

11 Twentieth-century existentialism simply gave up on rationalizing the role of<br />

suffering in the human condition and concluded that suffering is the human<br />

condition. The question of how suffering survives evolution is the same as<br />

the one that philosophers and scientists ask about altruism in its conflict<br />

with exploitation: how can it resist extinction?<br />

12 The rank order of the “encephalization quotient” (EQ) of mammals (humans<br />

7.4, dolphins 5.3, nonhuman primates 2.1-2.5, elephants 1.9, whales 1.8,<br />

canines 1.2, felines 1.0, equines 0.9, ungulates 0.8, rodents 0.4-0.5) well<br />

approximates the prevalence of animal behaviors that imply empathy,<br />

as we know it.<br />

13 Another troublesome paradox of the human condition is that we are capable<br />

of reproduction at the age of about 13 years, but the parts of the brain that<br />

are responsible for the control of impulsive behavior do not mature until<br />

the age of 21, if ever.<br />

14 A corollary to the hypothesis that a capacity for suffering requires the paleopallium<br />

or limbic system is the proposition that, birds not withstanding,<br />

most pre-mammalian species do not suffer, since they lack the neurological<br />

machinery to do so. The obverse proposition is that all mammals, to one<br />

degree or another, possess the capacity for suffering. It is, therefore, ethically<br />

safe to be very careful in the manner in which we conduct animal research.<br />

T H E PA I N P R A C T I T I O N E R | V O L U M E 16 , N U M B E R 1 | 79

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!