17.05.2014 Views

FRIDAY MORNING, 20 MAY 2005 REGENCY E, 8:30 A.M. TO 12:00 ...

FRIDAY MORNING, 20 MAY 2005 REGENCY E, 8:30 A.M. TO 12:00 ...

FRIDAY MORNING, 20 MAY 2005 REGENCY E, 8:30 A.M. TO 12:00 ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

11:<strong>30</strong><br />

5aPAb5. Measurements of the rate of change of the power spectral<br />

density due to nonlinearity in one-dimensional propagation. Lauren<br />

E. Falco, Kent L. Gee, and Anthony A. Atchley Grad. Program in<br />

Acoust., Penn State Univ., 217 Appl. Sci. Bldg., University Park, PA<br />

16802-5018<br />

The influence of nonlinear effects in the propagation of jet noise is<br />

typically characterized by examining the change in the power spectral<br />

density PSD of the noise as a function of propagation distance. The rate<br />

of change of the PSD is an indicator of the importance of nonlinearity.<br />

Morfey and Howell AIAA J. 19, 986–992 1981 introduced an analysis<br />

technique that has the potential to extract this information from a measurement<br />

at a single location. They develop an ensemble-averaged Burgers<br />

equation that relates the rate of change of the PSD with distance to the<br />

quantity Q p 2 p , which is the imaginary part of the cross-spectral density of<br />

the pressure and the square of the pressure. Despite its potential applicability<br />

to jet noise analysis, the physical significance and utility of Q p 2 p<br />

have not been thoroughly studied. This work examines Q p 2 p for the onedimensional<br />

propagation of plane waves in a shock tube. The use of such<br />

a simple, controlled environment allows for a better understanding of the<br />

significance of Q p 2 p . Work supported by the National Science Foundation,<br />

the Office of Naval Research, and the Strategic Environmental Research<br />

and Development Program.<br />

11:45<br />

5aPAb6. Nonlinear standing waves in shaped resonators driven by a<br />

piston. Cheng Luo, Xiaoyang Huang, and Nam Trung Nguyen School<br />

of Mech. and Production Eng., Nanyang Technolog. Univ., 50 Nanyang<br />

Ave., Singapore 639798<br />

The nonlinear standing waves in a shaped resonator driven by a piston<br />

are investigated analytically and experimentally. In the study, the resonator<br />

is an exponentially expanded horn with a piston oscillating at its large end.<br />

The air pressure waves inside the resonator are obtained by solving a 1-D<br />

nonlinear wave equation with the Galerkins method, taking into account<br />

the moving boundary due to the piston. The simulation results, in terms of<br />

the pressure waveforms, the pressure amplitudes, and the resonance frequency<br />

shift, show that the nonlinear standing waves generated by the<br />

piston have characteristics similar to those in the same resonator but under<br />

entirely shaking condition. The experiment is also conducted on an exponentially<br />

expanded resonator. The small end of the resonator is sealed and<br />

the big end is connected to a 5-inch 40 W loudspeaker, functioned as the<br />

driving piston. The resonance frequency shift is observed, and high amplitude<br />

pressures, up to 0.310 5 Pa, have been detected at the small end,<br />

which is <strong>20</strong> times higher than that at the big end. The experimental results<br />

agree well with the simulated values.<br />

<strong>FRIDAY</strong> <strong>MORNING</strong>, <strong>20</strong> <strong>MAY</strong> <strong>20</strong>05<br />

<strong>REGENCY</strong> C, 8:<strong>00</strong> A.M. <strong>TO</strong> <strong>12</strong>:<strong>00</strong> NOON<br />

Session 5aPP<br />

Psychological and Physiological Acoustics: Auditory Perception and Psychophysics II „Poster Session…<br />

Neal F. Viemeister, Chair<br />

Dept. of Psychology, Univ. of Minnesota, 75 East River Rd., Minneapolis, MN 55455<br />

Contributed Papers<br />

All posters will be on display from 8:<strong>00</strong> a.m. to <strong>12</strong>:<strong>00</strong> noon. To allow contributors an opportunity to see other posters, contributors of<br />

odd-numbered papers will be at their posters from 8:<strong>00</strong> a.m. to 10:<strong>00</strong> a.m. and contributors of even-numbered papers will be at their<br />

posters from 10:<strong>00</strong> a.m. to <strong>12</strong>:<strong>00</strong> noon.<br />

5aPP1. Sensitivity to stimulus distribution characteristics in auditory<br />

categorization. Sarah C. Sullivan Dept. of Psych., Univ. of Texas,<br />

Austin, TX 787<strong>12</strong>-0187, Andrew J. Lotto Boys Town Natl. Res.<br />

Hospital, Omaha, NE 68131, Elizabeth T. Newlin, and Randy L. Diehl<br />

Univ. of Texas, Austin, TX 787<strong>12</strong>-0187<br />

Several experiments were performed to examine the ability of humans<br />

to categorize sounds as a function of training distribution characteristics.<br />

Participants were presented non-speech sounds randomly sampled from<br />

two overlapping distributions. The sounds consisted of 25 narrow-band<br />

noise bursts varying in center frequency from 1<strong>00</strong>0–1360 Hz. Two conditions<br />

were created by varying the ratio of stimuli in each category i.e.,<br />

prior probabilities of each category, resulting in different ideal boundaries<br />

maximizing accuracy. Participants were asked to categorize the sounds<br />

as either A or B and feedback was provided. In one experiment, prior<br />

probabilities were altered mid-session without alerting the subjects. Performance<br />

was tracked by plotting identification functions and noting<br />

boundary placement for each individual block. After only six blocks of<br />

training (35 min), most subjects had established optimal or nearoptimal<br />

boundaries. Identification function slopes were calculated and<br />

found to be steeper than training distribution slopes; suggesting that listeners<br />

established criterion-like boundaries as opposed to performing<br />

probability matching. The fact that listeners adjusted their responses in<br />

accordance to distribution changes within relatively short periods of time<br />

demonstrates the perceptual systems effectiveness at optimizing categorization<br />

based on distribution characteristics. Work supported by NSF and<br />

NIDCD.<br />

5aPP2. Hearing silent shapes: Identifying the shape of a sound<br />

occluding surface. Ryan L. Robart and Lawrence D. Rosenblum Univ.<br />

of California, Riverside, Riverside, CA, 92521, rosenblu@citrus.ucr.edu<br />

While most psychoacoustics is concerned with perception of sound<br />

sources, there is evidence that surfaces which reflect or occlude sound can<br />

also be detected and can guide behavior e.g., M. S. Gordon and L. D.<br />

Rosenblum, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 107, 2851 <strong>20</strong><strong>00</strong>. While there is also<br />

evidence that listeners can hear the shape of sound reflecting surfaces<br />

e.g., C. E. Rice, Science 155, 655–664 1967, it is not known whether<br />

the shape of sound occluding surfaces can be heard. In a series of experiments,<br />

blindfolded listeners were asked to judge the shape of surfaces of<br />

equal area which occluded a set of loudspeakers emitting white noise.<br />

Overall, listeners were successful at this task, with some listeners showing<br />

near perfect performance. Follow-up experiments examined the acoustic<br />

information supportive of this skill. The findings suggest a type of auditory<br />

sensitivity not often considered in the psychoacoustics literature.<br />

2596 J. Acoust. Soc. Am., Vol. 117, No. 4, Pt. 2, April <strong>20</strong>05 149th Meeting: Acoustical Society of America 2596

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!