29.04.2014 Views

The ethics of research involving animals - Nuffield Council on ...

The ethics of research involving animals - Nuffield Council on ...

The ethics of research involving animals - Nuffield Council on ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

T h e e t h i c s o f r e s e a r c h i n v o l v i n g a n i m a l s<br />

Box 3.2: Cognitive capacities <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>animals</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

Communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

Chimpanzees communicate through vocal sounds, facial<br />

expressi<strong>on</strong>s, postures and touch. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y have an elaborate<br />

hierarchal social structure and use a complex<br />

communicati<strong>on</strong> system. For example, they alert other<br />

chimpanzees to the whereabouts <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> food sources with<br />

grunts and barks.*<br />

Tool use<br />

Tufted capuchin m<strong>on</strong>keys have been observed in the<br />

wild using st<strong>on</strong>es to dig in the ground to forage for food<br />

and to crack seeds.† In captivity they have been<br />

observed carrying probing tools to a fixed apparatus<br />

baited with syrup in order to obtain the syrup.‡ Great<br />

apes are competent tool users in the wild as well as in<br />

captivity. For example, captive chimpanzee mothers<br />

have been observed showing their infants how to<br />

selectively use tools for tasks such as obtaining h<strong>on</strong>ey<br />

from a c<strong>on</strong>tainer.∫ Other m<strong>on</strong>keys have been observed<br />

washing food, such as potatoes, in the sea, in order to<br />

make them more palatable.**<br />

Intelligence<br />

Dogs have been shown to know the names <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> many<br />

objects by retrieving them as instructed. For example, a<br />

border collie called Rico was shown to be able to<br />

associate words with over 200 different items and make<br />

hypotheses about the meanings <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> words. Rico could<br />

correctly retrieve a new item from am<strong>on</strong>g a selecti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

eleven items already known, by inferring that the word<br />

menti<strong>on</strong>ed did not refer to any <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the ten items already<br />

known.†† Rats and mice perform tasks that make<br />

sensory, motor, motivati<strong>on</strong>al and informati<strong>on</strong> processing<br />

demands. Rodents are able to navigate in mazes or find<br />

platforms hidden in coloured water.<br />

Social behaviour<br />

Reciprocity is comm<strong>on</strong>ly seen within groups <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> capuchin<br />

m<strong>on</strong>keys and chimpanzees, <str<strong>on</strong>g>involving</str<strong>on</strong>g> behaviours such as<br />

food sharing, grooming and cooperati<strong>on</strong>.‡‡ <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>se<br />

activities are not always restricted to family members,<br />

but also extend to unrelated <str<strong>on</strong>g>animals</str<strong>on</strong>g> (n<strong>on</strong>-kin<br />

reciprocity), as has been shown in <str<strong>on</strong>g>research</str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> bats.∫∫<br />

In a study which observed capuchin m<strong>on</strong>keys, m<strong>on</strong>keys<br />

were shown to have a sense <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> ‘justice’. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y reacted<br />

badly if they saw another m<strong>on</strong>key receiving more<br />

preferred food than they did. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> reacti<strong>on</strong> took the<br />

form <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> n<strong>on</strong>-cooperati<strong>on</strong> with the <str<strong>on</strong>g>research</str<strong>on</strong>g> task, or a<br />

refusal to eat the less-preferred food that they were<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>fered (which was otherwise acceptable if another<br />

m<strong>on</strong>key was also given this food item). <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> m<strong>on</strong>keys,<br />

however, did not react against the other m<strong>on</strong>key that<br />

was given the preferred food, but rather against the<br />

task that they would usually complete.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>re are also examples <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> situati<strong>on</strong>s when animal<br />

behaviour has been interpreted as altruistic towards<br />

humans. For example, in 2004 a group <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> swimmers<br />

reported that a pod <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> dolphins protected them from a<br />

great white shark <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f the coast <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> New Zealand. In<br />

1996 an eight-year-old Western lowland gorilla Binti Jua<br />

carried a three-year-old child who had fallen into the<br />

animal’s enclosure at Brookfield Zoo in Chicago, USA, to<br />

zoo keepers and paramedics, warning <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f another<br />

gorilla that was approaching. Other species have been<br />

observed showing signs <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> severe distress following the<br />

loss <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> an infant or parent, such as carrying the body<br />

around for several days, withdrawing from their group<br />

or appetite loss (see paragraphs 4.13 and 4.32).<br />

* See, for example, <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Jane Goodall Institute Chimp Calls,<br />

available at: http://www.janegoodall.org/jane/studycorner/chimpanzees/chimp-calls.asp.<br />

Accessed <strong>on</strong>: 18 Apr 2005.<br />

† Moura ACdeA and Lee PC (2004) Capuchin st<strong>on</strong>e tool use in<br />

Caatinga Dry Forest Science 306: 1909.<br />

‡ Cleveland A, Rocca AM, Wendt EL and Westergaard GC<br />

(2004) Transport <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> tools to food sites in tufted capuchin<br />

m<strong>on</strong>keys (Cebus apella) Anim Cogn 7: 193–8.<br />

∫<br />

Hirata S and Celli ML (2003) Role <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> mothers in the<br />

acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> tool-use behaviours by captive infant<br />

chimpanzees Anim Cogn 6: 235–44.<br />

** See De Waal F (2001) <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Ape and the Sushi Master: Cultural<br />

reflecti<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a primatologist (New York: Basic Books).<br />

†† Kaminski J, Call J and Fischer J (2004) Word learning in a<br />

domestic dog: evidence for ‘fast mapping’ Science 304: 1682–3.<br />

‡‡ See Brosnan SF and de Waal FBM (2002) A proximate<br />

perspective <strong>on</strong> reciprocal altruism Hum Nat 13: 129–52.<br />

∫∫<br />

Wilkins<strong>on</strong> GS (1990) Food sharing in vampire bats Sci Am<br />

262: 76–82.<br />

Brosnan SF and de Waal FBM (2003) M<strong>on</strong>keys reject unequal<br />

pay Nature 425: 297–9.<br />

BBC News (2004) Dolphins prevent NZ shark attack, available<br />

at: http://news.bbc.co.uk/1/hi/world/asia-pacific/4034383.stm.<br />

Accessed <strong>on</strong>: 18 Apr 2005.<br />

CHAPTER 3 ETHICAL ISSUES RAISED BY ANIMAL RESEARCH<br />

3.34 Nevertheless, the degree to which <str<strong>on</strong>g>animals</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> different types are capable <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> expressing<br />

higher cognitive capacities remains highly c<strong>on</strong>tentious. Clearly, though, it seems that in<br />

behavioural terms many <str<strong>on</strong>g>animals</str<strong>on</strong>g> are capable <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> dem<strong>on</strong>strating dissent by attempting to<br />

flee. It can therefore be argued that the implicati<strong>on</strong>s <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> an animal’s inclusi<strong>on</strong> in an<br />

experiment that it seeks to evade is something that should be taken into account. At the<br />

same time, we should hesitate before drawing the opposite c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>: that an animal that<br />

takes part apparently willingly does so freely. Participati<strong>on</strong> can be achieved through<br />

training, which most likely lessens the possible stressfulness <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> <str<strong>on</strong>g>research</str<strong>on</strong>g>, but cannot be<br />

taken to mean the same as c<strong>on</strong>sent given freely from a competent human <str<strong>on</strong>g>research</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

participant. For example, an animal may have realised that cooperati<strong>on</strong> with <str<strong>on</strong>g>research</str<strong>on</strong>g>ers is<br />

the <strong>on</strong>ly means <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> leaving a cage or pen, or gaining access to food, and it may ‘agree’ to<br />

take part for these reas<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

43

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!