29.04.2014 Views

The ethics of research involving animals - Nuffield Council on ...

The ethics of research involving animals - Nuffield Council on ...

The ethics of research involving animals - Nuffield Council on ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

T h e e t h i c s o f r e s e a r c h i n v o l v i n g a n i m a l s<br />

vertebrates. Given what we know about how nervous impulses are transported and<br />

processed, it seems highly unlikely that <str<strong>on</strong>g>animals</str<strong>on</strong>g> without a nervous system, such as sp<strong>on</strong>ges,<br />

experience pain or suffering, but highly likely that <str<strong>on</strong>g>animals</str<strong>on</strong>g> with more complex anatomy and<br />

behaviour, including vertebrates, do. 7 Thus, primate species with higher levels <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

physiological, and especially neurophysiological, complexity have the potential to<br />

experience a given disease or procedure in a more similar way to humans.<br />

4.9 Some people also emphasise the large number <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> genes that are shared between species. For<br />

example, humans share 99 percent <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> their DNA with chimpanzees and they argue that<br />

chimpanzees are therefore ‘almost human’. But knowledge about the percentage <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> shared<br />

DNA has limited applicati<strong>on</strong> in helping to decide whether or not an animal experiences pain<br />

and suffering in ways similar to humans. We also share significant amounts <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> DNA with<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>animals</str<strong>on</strong>g> with which we are less closely related, such as mice (96 percent) and fruit flies (70<br />

percent), and indeed with crops such as bananas (50 percent). Furthermore, the same gene<br />

may be expressed in different ways, or for different periods, or interact in different ways<br />

with other genes, which means that having genes in comm<strong>on</strong> is informati<strong>on</strong> that is <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

limited relevance with respect to assessing welfare. 8<br />

4.10 Clearly, however, evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary c<strong>on</strong>tinuities in the form <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> behavioural, anatomical,<br />

physiological, neurological, biochemical and pharmacological similarities provide sufficient<br />

grounds for the hypothesis that those <str<strong>on</strong>g>animals</str<strong>on</strong>g> that possess relevant features are capable <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

experiencing pain, suffering and distress. 9 Evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary c<strong>on</strong>tinuity also means that, <strong>on</strong><br />

scientific grounds, <str<strong>on</strong>g>animals</str<strong>on</strong>g> can, in specific cases, be useful models to study human diseases,<br />

and to examine the effects <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> therapeutic and other interventi<strong>on</strong>s. Nevertheless, the<br />

questi<strong>on</strong> remains as to what exactly evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary c<strong>on</strong>tinuity means with regard to the<br />

quality <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> pain and suffering which <str<strong>on</strong>g>animals</str<strong>on</strong>g> are capable <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> experiencing. If we use <str<strong>on</strong>g>animals</str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

as models for diseases that are painful for humans, such as neuropathy, is it not reas<strong>on</strong>able<br />

to expect that the animal models will experience similar pain? We note that for <str<strong>on</strong>g>animals</str<strong>on</strong>g> to<br />

provide valid models, it is usually <strong>on</strong>ly important that some element <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> their bodily processes<br />

should be similar to that <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> humans (see Chapters 5–9). 10 <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y do not always need to show<br />

all the typical signs <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> a disease, but just those relevant to a specific <str<strong>on</strong>g>research</str<strong>on</strong>g> questi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Arguments claiming that all <str<strong>on</strong>g>animals</str<strong>on</strong>g> used as models for human diseases necessarily suffer<br />

‘…assume that all the systems involved in the detecti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> pain evolved as a unitary package,<br />

which is either present and works in its entirety or is absent and does not work at all… this<br />

assumpti<strong>on</strong> is not merely implausible, it is wr<strong>on</strong>g. Most complex neural functi<strong>on</strong>s can be<br />

dissociated into sub-systems and, even in humans, parts <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the pain system can be intact<br />

while others are deficient. Furthermore, it remains far from obvious that all <str<strong>on</strong>g>animals</str<strong>on</strong>g> that<br />

7 See also Chapter 4, footnote 27.<br />

8 <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> percentage <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> genes that are shared between two species is not very informative. See, for example, Oxnard C (2004)<br />

Brain evoluti<strong>on</strong>: mammals, primates, chimpanzees, and humans Int J Primatol 25: 1127–58. Individual genes can code for<br />

more than <strong>on</strong>e protein through alternative splicing. <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g>y can also be expressed in a variety <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> different ways depending <strong>on</strong><br />

how they are regulated. In additi<strong>on</strong>, a significant proporti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the genome is not in the form <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> genes and is referred to as<br />

‘junk DNA’. Its functi<strong>on</strong>s are thought to be involved in genetic regulati<strong>on</strong>. It is also noteworthy that changes in a single gene<br />

al<strong>on</strong>e can be dramatic. For example, chimpanzees and humans became divided from a comm<strong>on</strong> ancestor at least five milli<strong>on</strong><br />

years ago. About 2.4 milli<strong>on</strong> years ago, an important gene mutati<strong>on</strong> occurred in the line that developed into the human<br />

species. It has been shown that this mutati<strong>on</strong> resulted in a reducti<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the size <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> the jaw muscles, and may have allowed<br />

the brain to expand and develop into its modern human form. See Stedman HH, Kozyak BW, Nels<strong>on</strong> A et al. (2004) Myosin<br />

gene mutati<strong>on</strong> correlates with anatomical changes in the human lineage Nature 428: 415–8.<br />

9 See, for example, Bek<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f M (2002) Minding Animals: Awareness, Emoti<strong>on</strong>s and Heart (Oxford and New York: Oxford<br />

University Press); Goodall J and Bek<str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g>f M (2002) <str<strong>on</strong>g>The</str<strong>on</strong>g> Ten Trusts: What We Must Do to Care For the Animals We Love (San<br />

Francisco: HarperCollins); Panksepp J (2003) ‘Laughing’ rats and the evoluti<strong>on</strong>ary antecedents <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> human joy? Physiol Behav<br />

79: 533–47.<br />

10 For example, although humans and mice clearly differ in their appearance, the functi<strong>on</strong> <str<strong>on</strong>g>of</str<strong>on</strong>g> anatomical structures such as<br />

tend<strong>on</strong>s is the same in both, and results from studies <strong>on</strong> tend<strong>on</strong>s in mice can readily be transferred to humans.<br />

CHAPTER 4 THE CAPACITY OF ANIMALS TO EXPERIENCE PAIN, DISTRESS AND SUFFERING<br />

65

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!