The ontogeny of prosocial behavior: foraging and food sharing ...

The ontogeny of prosocial behavior: foraging and food sharing ... The ontogeny of prosocial behavior: foraging and food sharing ...

sscnet.ucla.edu
from sscnet.ucla.edu More from this publisher

<strong>The</strong> <strong>ontogeny</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>prosocial</strong> <strong>behavior</strong>: <br />

<strong>foraging</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>food</strong> <strong>sharing</strong> among Hadza <br />

hunter-gatherer children<br />

Alyssa N. Crittenden<br />

<br />

Visiting Assistant Pr<strong>of</strong>essor<br />

Department <strong>of</strong> Anthropology<br />

University <strong>of</strong> Nevada, Las Vegas


Routine <strong>food</strong> <strong>sharing</strong> is argued to<br />

influence the evolution <strong>of</strong>:<br />

• Life history<br />

• Family formation<br />

• Cooperation<br />

• Economies <strong>of</strong> scale<br />

Food transfers can have multiple functions<br />

• Develop <strong>and</strong> maintain social bonds<br />

• Reduce variance <strong>of</strong> <strong>food</strong> consumption –<br />

provision others


Evolutionary Models<br />

Kin Selection<br />

An individual should be willing to share as long as the<br />

cost is less than the benefit, times the coefficient <strong>of</strong><br />

relatedness between individuals C < B x r<br />

(Hamilton 1964)<br />

Reciprocal Altruism<br />

Givers <strong>and</strong> receivers reverse positions on a systematic basis,<br />

such that amounts received <strong>and</strong> given are correlated<br />

(Axelrod 1984; Trivers 1971)


Very little is known the development <strong>of</strong> <strong>food</strong> <strong>sharing</strong> in<br />

children <strong>and</strong> how it may influence the development<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>prosocial</strong> <strong>behavior</strong><br />

Hadza juveniles are active <strong>food</strong> collectors <strong>and</strong> can produce<br />

up to 50% <strong>of</strong> their daily caloric intake by the age <strong>of</strong> 5 years<br />

(Blurton Jones et al. 1989, 1994)<br />

<strong>The</strong>y also share substantial portions <strong>of</strong> their collection with others<br />

(Crittenden et al. 2010)


Current Study:<br />

(1) How much do Hadza children forage <strong>and</strong> can they<br />

<strong>of</strong>fset the cost <strong>of</strong> their own care?<br />

(2) Are Hadza children selecting <strong>sharing</strong> partners<br />

based on kinship <strong>and</strong>/or reciprocity?<br />

(3) Is there a significant caloric difference between the<br />

amount shared with kin vs. non-kin?


~ Hadza Foragers ~<br />

Approximately 1000 individuals (300 hunt <strong>and</strong> gather)<br />

Northern Tanzania ~ Lake Eyasi<br />

Camp membership is fluid ~ 30 residents<br />

Residence pattern is variable ~ wifes kin OR husb<strong>and</strong>s kin


Juvenile <strong>foraging</strong><br />

Previous research suggests that <strong>food</strong> collection by children is<br />

constrained by ecological parameters <strong>and</strong> costs <strong>of</strong> collection<br />

<strong>and</strong> processing<br />

• Low predator pressure<br />

• Access to drinking water<br />

• Variability in terrain


In environments that are conducive to juvenile <strong>foraging</strong>,<br />

young foragers can be avid <strong>and</strong> productive <strong>food</strong> collectors<br />

Hadza <strong>of</strong> Tanzania<br />

- Hawkes, OConnell & Blurton Jones (1995)<br />

Current Anthropology 36:4, 688-700.<br />

- Blurton Jones, Hawkes & OConnell (1997)<br />

Uniting Psychology <strong>and</strong> Biology: Integrative Perspectives on Human<br />

Development<br />

Martu <strong>of</strong> Australia<br />

- Bird & Bliege-Bird (2005)<br />

Hunter-Gatherer Childhoods<br />

Mikea <strong>of</strong> Madagascar<br />

- Tucker & Young (2005)<br />

Hunter-Gatherer Childhoods


Gangidape ~ April - May 2005 (wet season)<br />

Siponga ~ October 2005 (dry season)<br />

Total Sample: 65 days<br />

• Children (1 - 17 years) n = 43<br />

• Children who foraged (3 - 17 years) n = 34<br />

Foraging Returns:<br />

481 instances <strong>of</strong> <strong>food</strong> back to camp<br />

(n 1 = 14 males, n 2 = 20 females)<br />

Focal Follows:<br />

- 13 follows <strong>of</strong> individual foragers<br />

(n 1 = 6 males, n 2 = 7 females)<br />

- Age range 6 - 14 years<br />

- Ranged from 50 minutes to<br />

5 hours in length, depending<br />

on distance traveled


Ekwa - tuber<br />

(Vigna frutescens)<br />

Kongolobe - berries<br />

Hornbill - bird<br />

Embilipe - berries<br />

Nguilabe - berries<br />

Baalako - honey<br />

Ogoyo - wild fig<br />

Mangwala- legume


Weaver bird seasonal breeding <strong>and</strong><br />

Hadza exploitation <strong>of</strong> nestling birds<br />

Genus Ploceus


Greg Downing<br />

Hadza children are in direct <strong>foraging</strong> competition with baboons<br />

- baobab pods & seeds<br />

- figs<br />

- drinking water


Calories Total Brought Foods Kilocalories Back Collected to Back Camp by to Children by Camp Children<br />

(collapsed across all forays)


15%<br />

Wet Season Calories Collected<br />

19%<br />

19%<br />

15%<br />

Baobab<br />

Berries<br />

Birds<br />

Honey<br />

Tubers<br />

Fruit = 34%<br />

Honey = 15%<br />

Tubers = 19%<br />

Birds = 32%<br />

32%<br />

Dry Season Calories Collected<br />

10%<br />

Fruit = 84%<br />

Legumes = 1%<br />

Birds = 5%<br />

Small Mammals = 10%<br />

1%<br />

11%<br />

8%<br />

5%<br />

4%<br />

61%<br />

Baobab<br />

Berries<br />

Birds<br />

Drupes<br />

Figs<br />

Legumes<br />

Small Mammals


⇒ Does age affect <strong>foraging</strong> return?<br />

Age groups:<br />

(1) 1 - 4 years (n = 2) = 67 kcal<br />

(2) 5 - 9 years (n = 14) = 724 kcal<br />

(3) 10 - 13 years (n = 11) = 634 kcal<br />

(4) 14 - 17 years (n = 7) = 618 kcal<br />

<strong>The</strong> mean values <strong>of</strong> calorie returns per <strong>foraging</strong> trip for<br />

each age group were compared…<br />

Age is NOT a significant predictor <strong>of</strong> <strong>foraging</strong> return<br />

Wilcoxon rank sum p = .055, n 1 = 199 forays for age group 2, n 2 = 191 forays for<br />

age group 3; p = .706, n 1 = 191 forays for age group 3, n 2 = 86 forays for group 4;<br />

p = .143, n 1 = 199 forays for age group 2, n 2 = 86 forays for group 4


6000<br />

5000<br />

Kilocalories Collected<br />

4000<br />

3000<br />

2000<br />

1000<br />

0<br />

0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 18 <br />

Age


Calorie Returns per Individual Foraging Trip<br />

⇒<br />

Does age affect <strong>food</strong> type?<br />

Age IS a significant predictor <strong>of</strong><br />

the type <strong>of</strong> <strong>food</strong> collected<br />

chi squared = 135.72, df = 24, p < .001


Kilocalories From Food Collected<br />

140000<br />

120000<br />

100000<br />

80000<br />

60000<br />

40000<br />

20000<br />

Total Kilocalories Collected, Split by Age<br />

4 (14-17 yrs)<br />

3 (10-13 yrs)<br />

2 (5-9 yrs)<br />

1 (1-4 yrs)<br />

0<br />

Baobab<br />

Birds<br />

Figs<br />

Tubers<br />

Honey<br />

Berries<br />

Small<br />

Mammal<br />

Drupes<br />

Legumes


⇒ Does sex affect <strong>foraging</strong> return?<br />

Mean return per trip =<br />

males (n = 14) = 201 kcal<br />

females (n = 20) = 281 kcal<br />

Sex IS a significant predictor <strong>of</strong> <strong>foraging</strong> return ~<br />

Female foragers bring back more <strong>food</strong> to camp<br />

Wilcoxon rank sum p = .003, n 1 = 214 forays for males,<br />

n 2 = 266 forays for females


Calorie Returns per Individual Foraging Trip<br />

⇒<br />

Does sex affect <strong>food</strong> type?<br />

Sex IS also a significant predictor<br />

<strong>of</strong> the type <strong>of</strong> <strong>food</strong> collected<br />

chi squared =223.945, df = 8, p < .001


Kilocalories From Food Collected<br />

140000<br />

120000<br />

100000<br />

80000<br />

60000<br />

40000<br />

20000<br />

0<br />

Total Kilocalories Collected, Split by Sex - Across Seasons<br />

Female<br />

Male<br />

Baobab<br />

Birds<br />

Figs<br />

Tubers<br />

Honey<br />

Berries<br />

Small Mammal<br />

Legumes


Focal Follows<br />

Age DOES affect distance traveled ~<br />

younger foragers stayed closer to camp<br />

Wilcoxon rank sum test p = .005, n1 = 4 foragers < 10 years, n2 = 9 foragers > 10 years).<br />

Average distance for males (n = 6 ) = 2.3 km<br />

Average distance for females (n = 7 ) = 1.3 km<br />

Sex <strong>of</strong> the child does NOT affect distance traveled<br />

Wilcoxon rank sum test: p = .295


Average consumption by males (n = 6) = 2267 kcals<br />

Average consumption by females (n = 7) = 667 kcals<br />

Sex <strong>of</strong> the child DOES affect consumption while <strong>foraging</strong> ~<br />

male foragers consume significantly more<br />

Wilcoxon rank sum test: p = .014


Hadza juveniles are active foragers <strong>and</strong> routinely collect<br />

a large portion <strong>of</strong> their daily requirements<br />

Sex differences in collection <strong>and</strong> consumption may be linked<br />

to differential <strong>foraging</strong> strategies <strong>and</strong>/or sex differences<br />

in household responsibilities


Young male foragers target small mammals, birds, <strong>and</strong> honey.<br />

This type <strong>of</strong> <strong>foraging</strong> is energetically dem<strong>and</strong>ing –<br />

young male foragers may be feeding their own excess<br />

energetic requirements


Young female foragers not only collect <strong>and</strong> prepare<br />

<strong>food</strong> for other children, but also spend large amounts<br />

<strong>of</strong> time caring for younger children in camp<br />

<strong>The</strong>re are greater dem<strong>and</strong>s on the time <strong>of</strong> young<br />

female foragers


• Age does not appear to<br />

significantly affect kilocalories back<br />

to camp<br />

• Age <strong>and</strong> sex differences in type <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>food</strong> collected – follows adult<br />

pattern <strong>of</strong> the sexual division <strong>of</strong><br />

labor among the Hadza<br />

• Young female foragers bring back<br />

more calories to camp<br />

• Although there is no sex<br />

difference in <strong>foraging</strong> distance,<br />

male foragers consume more while<br />

out <strong>foraging</strong>


Hadza juvenile foragers are able to self-provision <strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong>fset<br />

the cost <strong>of</strong> their own care…<br />

How do they distribute their <strong>foraging</strong> yield?


FOOD SHARING<br />

2 camps (Gangidape = April 2005; Siponga = October 2005)<br />

36 days<br />

128 <strong>food</strong> transfers<br />

n = 36 children in camp<br />

n = 35 children participating in a <strong>food</strong> <strong>sharing</strong> event<br />

(age 2 – 17 years; n 1 = 13 males <strong>and</strong> n 2 = 22 females)<br />

n = 106 <strong>sharing</strong> dyads (n 1 = 54 kin <strong>and</strong> n 2 = 52 non-kin)<br />

Food transfers range from 50kcal – 6,000kcal


Statistical analyses<br />

Due to the unknown correlation structure among dyads<br />

<strong>and</strong> because <strong>of</strong> possible non-Gaussian (non-linear)<br />

responses, we used two non-parametric tests <strong>of</strong> statistical<br />

significance:<br />

(1) QAP (quadratic assignment procedure):<br />

we constructed adjacency matrices, created null responses,<br />

<strong>and</strong> iterated the process ~50,000 times<br />

(2) Permutation testing:<br />

simulations <strong>of</strong> the null distributions <strong>of</strong> the test statistic <strong>of</strong><br />

interest


Sex Differences in Food Sharing<br />

NO difference between the amount <strong>of</strong> <strong>food</strong> that male <strong>and</strong><br />

female foragers share<br />

(r = 0.0014, p = 0.503, two-sided permutation test on correlation)


Age Differences in Food Sharing<br />

Age DOES predict total amount shared <strong>and</strong> frequency <strong>of</strong> <strong>sharing</strong><br />

correlation between increase in age <strong>and</strong> total amount shared<br />

(r = 0.531, p = 0.0024, one-sided permutation test on correlation)<br />

correlation between increase in age <strong>and</strong> frequency <strong>of</strong> <strong>sharing</strong><br />

(r = 0.608, p = 0.00006, one-sided permutation test on correlation)


Food <strong>sharing</strong> as a function <strong>of</strong> age


Partner Selection - kin selection or reciprocity?<br />

Children who are genetically related tend to share<br />

MORE <strong>food</strong> in HIGHER frequency<br />

greater quantity (p = 0.00014, one-sided QAP on gr<strong>and</strong> sum)<br />

higher frequency (p ≈ 0; one-sided QAP on gr<strong>and</strong> sum)<br />

A higher degree <strong>of</strong> genetic relatedness<br />

correlates with MORE <strong>food</strong> shared in<br />

HIGHER frequency<br />

(r = 0.141, p = 0.00016, one-sided QAP on correlation)


Partner Selection - kin selection or reciprocity?<br />

Among genetically unrelated children, the<br />

amount <strong>of</strong> <strong>food</strong> shared IS reciprocal<br />

(r = 0.272, p = 0.036, one-sided QAP on correlation)<br />

<strong>The</strong> frequency <strong>of</strong> <strong>sharing</strong> is NOT<br />

reciprocal<br />

(r = 0.226, p = 0.1101, one-sided QAP on<br />

correlation)


Network graph showing observed meal <strong>sharing</strong> (arrows)<br />

between children (circle = female, triangle = male)


Network graph showing observed meal <strong>sharing</strong> (arrows)<br />

between children (circle = female, triangle = male)


Are Hadza juveniles selecting <strong>sharing</strong> partners based on<br />

kinship or reciprocity?<br />

Kinship first, reciprocity second<br />

Is there a significant caloric difference between the<br />

amounts shared with kin vs. non-kin?<br />

More <strong>food</strong>, in greater frequency, is shared with kin


Young foragers not only self-provision, but also share<br />

large portions <strong>of</strong> their yield with related <strong>and</strong> unrelated children.<br />

<strong>The</strong>se data support the hypothesis that other-regarding<br />

preferences begin to strongly develop in middle childhood.<br />

Middle childhood is the time when children begin to<br />

incorporate the normative rules <strong>of</strong> the society in which they<br />

live.


Acknowledgements<br />

This research was funded by NSF, UC San Diego & Harvard University<br />

Thank you to Frank Marlowe, my Tanzanian research assistants (Happy,<br />

Golden, Ephraim, <strong>and</strong> Holiness), Jackie Benjamin (UCSD), <strong>and</strong> Dave Zes<br />

(UCLA). Many many thanks to the Hadza for making this work so<br />

enjoyable <strong>and</strong> for welcoming me into their lives…

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!