14.01.2014 Views

2000 HSS/PSA Program 1 - History of Science Society

2000 HSS/PSA Program 1 - History of Science Society

2000 HSS/PSA Program 1 - History of Science Society

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>HSS</strong> Abstracts<br />

charlatans, arguing that they misappropriated and attenuated what had been a limited<br />

and useful concept. Even popularizers, however, used the standardized and formal<br />

measurement <strong>of</strong> efficiency, the percentage, a standard mathematically precise and<br />

yet devoid <strong>of</strong> content. This measurement, the efficiency percentage, retained the<br />

technical features <strong>of</strong> the concept. This paper discusses the role <strong>of</strong> the efficiency<br />

percentage in two different and widely circulating journals <strong>of</strong> the early twentieth<br />

century: Engineering Magazine and The Independent. It argues that efficiency<br />

experts adopted the technical standardized measurement <strong>of</strong> efficiency and<br />

developed a precise and parallel social equivalent, which was neither a metaphor<br />

nor simply a rhetorical device. It takes exception to the prevalent emphasis on<br />

efficiency as a cultural artifact more expressive <strong>of</strong> the progressive social ethos<br />

than <strong>of</strong> its own scientific and technological heritage.<br />

46<br />

Stephen␣ G. Alter University <strong>of</strong> Notre Dame<br />

Unconscious Selection and Darwin’s Distribution Thinking<br />

This paper explores the nexus between Charles Darwin’s concept <strong>of</strong><br />

unconscious selection and what I call his “distribution thinking,” a subset <strong>of</strong><br />

Darwin’s population thinking as described by Ernst Mayr. Darwin never fully<br />

articulated the linkage between these two concepts, yet he increasingly<br />

connected them in his correspondence, in revised editions <strong>of</strong> the Origin <strong>of</strong><br />

Species, and in The Descent <strong>of</strong> Man. He did this, I argue, in an effort to<br />

compensate, by analogy, for his imperfect grasp <strong>of</strong> the normal distribution <strong>of</strong><br />

any given variation existing at a particular point in time. Although Darwin<br />

built his natural selection theory implicitly on the idea <strong>of</strong> a normal range <strong>of</strong><br />

variations, he undercut this theme by suggesting (Origin, ch. 4) that nature<br />

produced “favorable” variations only rarely. He then addressed this perceived<br />

obstacle by suggesting that the presence <strong>of</strong> a large population (analogous to<br />

the conditions producing unconscious selection) increased the chance <strong>of</strong> useful<br />

variations appearing at a given time. The real issue, however, was not the<br />

chance <strong>of</strong> useful variations arising at a given time—what Darwin misleadingly<br />

suggested—but the chance <strong>of</strong> their being included in a given population sample.<br />

Ideally, a full range <strong>of</strong> variations always exists, according to the distribution<br />

principle, yet it is not necessarily represented in every actual sample. Darwin<br />

was therefore right to emphasize large populations as a condition favorable to<br />

selection. Yet he was logically inconsistent in saying that the chance <strong>of</strong> their<br />

appearing could be increased even while affirming their inherent rarity. Darwin<br />

<strong>of</strong>ten responded to critics on this and related issues (A. R. Wallace, Fleeming<br />

Jenkin, and Moritz Wagner) with appeals to the unconscious selection analogy,<br />

for this, he felt, provided a gestalt picture <strong>of</strong> selection working on a large<br />

range <strong>of</strong> variations, even without the aid <strong>of</strong> geographic isolation. My paper<br />

builds upon yet differs from histories <strong>of</strong> these controversies written by Peter<br />

Bowler, Frank Sulloway, and Susan Morris.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!