Stony Brook University - SUNY Digital Repository

Stony Brook University - SUNY Digital Repository Stony Brook University - SUNY Digital Repository

dspace.sunyconnect.suny.edu
from dspace.sunyconnect.suny.edu More from this publisher
25.12.2013 Views

presented here supports that hypothesis” (15). To Shapiro, however, this “evidence” raises more questions than it answers, the most begging of these being, “Do students at higher levels of intellectual maturity write better because they think better, or is it possible that the act of writing itself generates the disequilibrium which leads to cognitive development” (18)? While she frames her question as “a classic ‘chicken or egg’ quandary,” with her reports’ contention that “Rhetorical Maturity” is greatly determined by “contextual relativism” (3), “the writer’s ability to accommodate to his readers” (6) and “assess […] readers’ needs more accurately” (15), her evidence would seem to assert, implicitly, the necessity of those student writers’ working, writing and thinking, within a “community,” an audience and, thusly, a context for which and in which to write, as well as its heavy influence upon that relationship between their “intellectual maturity” and their “rhetorical maturity,” the two very likely existing within a dialectic, the one pushing and pulling the other. Again, “reconstructive collaboration.” This heavy influence would be defined much more explicitly in the early ‘90s with two other research essays: Sue Dinitz and Jean Kiedaisch’s “Persuasion from an Eighteen-Year-Old’s Perspective” from 1990 and Toni-Lee Capossela’s “Using William Perry’s Scheme to Encourage Critical Writing.” With them both, that “community” of student writers, as well as the writing 212

instructor’s standing within such a community, became more and more of a prominent thing within Perry’s scheme, its crucial presence evinced through the further observations and subsequent conclusions offered by those two essays. Dinitz and Kiedaisch published their findings in the Journal of Teaching Writing in 1990. They had observed, seemingly, their own freshman writing course, the students in which had been asked to “‘persuade the other members of your workshop group to change their minds and/or behavior on any topic of your choice’”(209). For Dinitz and Kiedaisch, the purpose of this assignment was “to teach students why and how to make writing choices based on their audience” (209). What they saw left them scratching their heads: [T]his assignment turned out to be puzzlingly difficult; many first-year students seemed unable or unwilling to make writing choices based on what would influence their audience. […] They treated their audience analyses as mechanical exercises: they had few questions, spent little time, and wrote a composite essay, as if all the students were exactly the same. When they shared drafts, some of them ignored suggestions, seemingly not caring whether their peers were persuaded. (210) After sorting through different possibilities for why this had happened, they finally turned to William Perry (and Jean Piaget) to “explain choices our students made in writing [their] persuasive essays” (209) and there they would seem to have found what they were looking for. The crux of the meaning they had made of this situation with their students’ assignment, as read 213

presented here supports that hypothesis” (15). To Shapiro,<br />

however, this “evidence” raises more questions than it answers,<br />

the most begging of these being, “Do students at higher levels<br />

of intellectual maturity write better because they think better,<br />

or is it possible that the act of writing itself generates the<br />

disequilibrium which leads to cognitive development” (18)?<br />

While she frames her question as “a classic ‘chicken or egg’<br />

quandary,” with her reports’ contention that “Rhetorical<br />

Maturity” is greatly determined by “contextual relativism” (3),<br />

“the writer’s ability to accommodate to his readers” (6) and<br />

“assess […] readers’ needs more accurately” (15), her evidence<br />

would seem to assert, implicitly, the necessity of those student<br />

writers’ working, writing and thinking, within a “community,” an<br />

audience and, thusly, a context for which and in which to write,<br />

as well as its heavy influence upon that relationship between<br />

their “intellectual maturity” and their “rhetorical maturity,”<br />

the two very likely existing within a dialectic, the one pushing<br />

and pulling the other. Again, “reconstructive collaboration.”<br />

This heavy influence would be defined much more explicitly<br />

in the early ‘90s with two other research essays: Sue Dinitz<br />

and Jean Kiedaisch’s “Persuasion from an Eighteen-Year-Old’s<br />

Perspective” from 1990 and Toni-Lee Capossela’s “Using William<br />

Perry’s Scheme to Encourage Critical Writing.” With them both,<br />

that “community” of student writers, as well as the writing<br />

212

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!