20.12.2013 Views

The Syntax of Givenness Ivona Kucerová

The Syntax of Givenness Ivona Kucerová

The Syntax of Givenness Ivona Kucerová

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

(23) What can be understood as new?<br />

a. (i) What did Marie give to Pavel?<br />

(ii) Marie dala Pavlovi [facku] New ←− slap<br />

b. (i) What did Marie give to whom?<br />

(ii) Marie dala [Pavlovi facku] New ←− Pavel a slap<br />

c. (i) What did Marie do?<br />

(ii) Marie [dala Pavlovi facku] New ←− gave Pavel a slap<br />

d. (i) What happened?<br />

(ii) [Marie dala Pavlovi facku] New ←− Marie gave Pavel a slap<br />

In contrast, if an utterance contains any deviance from the basic order, then such an utterance<br />

is infelicitous in an all-new context. It means that if any reordering takes place,<br />

at least one constituent must be α G , i.e., introduced in the previous discourse. In other<br />

words, any reordering limits the number <strong>of</strong> structural positions in which we can identify<br />

the partition between given and new. For example, in a derived word order, as in (24), there<br />

is only one felicitous interpretation <strong>of</strong> the information structure. More precisely, only the<br />

rightmost constituent can be interpreted as new (non-G). In this particular case, it is the<br />

indirect object Pavel. Thus, in (24) there is only one possible partition between given and<br />

new, in contrast to (22) that is compatible with several partitions, as schematized in (25).<br />

(24) Focus Projection within a derived word order:<br />

a. Marie dala facku [Pavlovi] New ←− S V DO || IO t DO<br />

b. #Marie dala [facku Pavlovi] New<br />

c. #Marie [dala facku Pavlovi] New<br />

d. #[Marie dala facku Pavlovi] New<br />

(25) a. Marie dala facku || Pavlovi.<br />

b. (||) Marie (||) dala (||) Pavlovi (||) facku.<br />

What we can learn from the observed pattern is that in basic word order there is a relative<br />

freedom in what parts <strong>of</strong> the utterance can be interpreted as new and what parts can be interpreted<br />

as given. As I have already anticipated in the previous discussion <strong>of</strong> G-movement,<br />

this pattern can be described in the following manner: whatever is interpreted as given cannot<br />

be linearly preceded by anything interpreted as new.<br />

Let’s now turn to the question <strong>of</strong> how exactly the multiple partition effect follows from<br />

our system. To see that, we will look at a very simple case: a transitive clause that has no<br />

modifiers, only a subject, a verb, and an object. Consider first the case where the subject is<br />

the only given element. As we already know, the resulting word order is SVO, as seen in<br />

(26) and (27). 16<br />

(26) a. Subject-G verb Object ←−<br />

b. #Object verb Subject-G<br />

16 I use here examples with potom ‘then, afterward’ instead <strong>of</strong> a wh-question. <strong>The</strong> reason is that potom<br />

creates a natural context where only the subject is presupposed/given. For reasons that are not clear to me, it<br />

is difficult to obtain the same pragmatic effect with a wh-question.<br />

19

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!