25.10.2012 Views

Phoneme and Allophone - Speech Resource Pages

Phoneme and Allophone - Speech Resource Pages

Phoneme and Allophone - Speech Resource Pages

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>Phoneme</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Allophone</strong><br />

The nexus between<br />

phonetics <strong>and</strong> phonology<br />

Robert Mannell


Discriminative Elements (1)<br />

� Trubetzkoy (1939) wrote:-<br />

"It is the task of phonology to study which<br />

differences in sound are related to<br />

differences in meaning in a given language,<br />

in which way the discriminative elements ...<br />

are related to each other, <strong>and</strong> the rules<br />

according to which they may be combined<br />

into words <strong>and</strong> sentences."


Discriminative Elements (2)<br />

� Linguistic units which cannot be substituted<br />

for each other without a change in meaning<br />

can be referred to as linguistically contrastive<br />

or significant units.<br />

� Such units may be phonological,<br />

morphological, syntactic, semantic etc.


Discriminative Elements (3)<br />

Logically, this takes the form:-<br />

IF unit X in context A GIVES meaning 1<br />

AND IF unit Y in context A GIVES meaning 2<br />

THEN unit X AND unit Y belong to separate linguistic units<br />

e.g.<br />

IF sound [k] in context [ _ æt] GIVES meaning “cat”<br />

AND IF sound [m] in context [ _ æt] GIVES meaning “mat”<br />

THEN sound [k] <strong>and</strong> sound [m] belong to separate linguistic units


What is a phoneme? (1)<br />

� Its not true to say that a phoneme is a sound,<br />

or even that it’s a class of sounds<br />

� <strong>Phoneme</strong>s exist in human brains<br />

� They are abstract cognitive (linguistic) entities<br />

� They are conventions shared by a speech<br />

community but vary, sometimes very<br />

significantly, between speech communities


What is a phoneme? (2)<br />

� You might well ask … but surely they have<br />

something to do with speech sounds?<br />

� Well, yes, but indirectly …<br />

� When we speak we intend our listeners to<br />

underst<strong>and</strong> what words we have uttered.<br />

� A word in the brain is represented as a<br />

sequence of phonemes (or graphemes for the<br />

written word)


What is a phoneme? (3)<br />

� To communicate a sequence of words we must<br />

utter a sequence of sounds (or write the words<br />

or use sign language …)<br />

� A spoken word results from the production of a<br />

sequence of vocal tract gestures<br />

� These gestures result in a sequence of sounds.<br />

� We interpret this sequence of sounds as a<br />

sequence of phonemes


What is a phoneme? (4)<br />

� When we learn a language we learn to<br />

associate sequences of sounds in the<br />

physical world with sequences of phonemes<br />

(<strong>and</strong> therefore words) in the mental world.<br />

� The same sound may belong to a different<br />

phoneme in a different speech community or<br />

even in a different phonetic context.


What is a phoneme? (5)<br />

� OK … doesn’t that mean that a phoneme is<br />

realised as a class (or set) of physical sounds?<br />

� Yes, well almost … as some sounds can, in<br />

different contexts, belong to (or represent)<br />

different phonemes<br />

� This means that the phonemes are represented<br />

in the physical world by potentially overlapping<br />

sets of sounds


What is a phoneme? (6)<br />

� The set of sounds (in the external acoustic<br />

world) that represent a phoneme can be<br />

referred to as a set of allophones.<br />

� “allo-” indicates “difference, alternation or<br />

divergence” (Macquarie Dictionary)


What is a phoneme? (7)<br />

� An allophone is a sound that can represent a<br />

particular phoneme.<br />

� A phoneme can be said to have a huge<br />

number of slightly different allophones.<br />

� This is too complex to work with so we break<br />

this down into a much smaller set of discrete<br />

sounds that we can transcribe phonetically or<br />

measure in some other way.


What is a phoneme? (8)<br />

� One of the reasons for a phoneme having<br />

different allophones is coarticulation.<br />

That is, in different contexts the effect of<br />

adjacent phonemes can affect a phoneme’s<br />

physical realisation.<br />

� Another reason is convention. A speech<br />

community has an implicit (unconscious)<br />

agreement that certain allophones be used in<br />

certain contexts.


What is a phoneme? (9)<br />

Classical phonology took a simple view of the<br />

relationship between phonemes <strong>and</strong> allophones:-<br />

� phonemes are contrastive, allophones are not<br />

� an allophone belongs to a single phoneme<br />

� allophones of the same phoneme are in<br />

complementary distribution<br />

� allophones are phonetically similar<br />

We will examine the last three points in more detail<br />

later, but first we will examine phonemic analysis.


Phonemic Analysis<br />

� Phonemic analysis uses a narrow transcription<br />

of the speech of a language to determine what<br />

are the phonemes (the their allophones) for<br />

that language.<br />

� The phonemic analysis relies on the<br />

assumption that (a) the transcribed words have<br />

different meanings <strong>and</strong> (b) the transcription<br />

reliably captures the language’s sound system.


Minimal pairs (1)<br />

� <strong>Phoneme</strong>s are the linguistically contrastive or<br />

significant sounds (or sets of sounds) of a<br />

language.<br />

� Such a contrast is usually demonstrated by<br />

the existence of minimal pairs or contrast in<br />

identical environment (CIE).<br />

� The search for minimal pairs is the most<br />

important strategy in phonemic analysis.


Minimal pairs (2)<br />

� Minimal pairs are pairs of words which vary<br />

only by the identity of the segment 1 at a single<br />

location in the word (eg. [mæt] <strong>and</strong> [kæt]).<br />

� If two segments contrast in identical<br />

environment then they must belong to different<br />

phonemes. That is, if we change one sound to<br />

another <strong>and</strong> it changes the meaning then the<br />

sounds belong to different phonemes.<br />

(1) “segment” is another word for “single speech sound”


Minimal pairs (3)<br />

� A paradigm of minimal phonological<br />

contrasts is a set of words differing only by<br />

one speech sound.<br />

� In most languages it is rare to find a<br />

paradigm that contrasts a complete class of<br />

phonemes (eg. all vowels OR all consonants<br />

OR all stops, etc.).


Minimal pairs (4)<br />

e.g. English oral stops<br />

� the English oral stop consonants could be defined by the<br />

following set (paradigm) of minimally contrasting words:i)<br />

/pIn/ vs /bIn/ vs /tIn/ vs /dIn/ vs /kIn/<br />

� Only /ɡ/ does not occur in this paradigm <strong>and</strong> at least one<br />

minimal pair must be found with each of the other 5 stops to<br />

prove conclusively that it is not a variant form of one of them.<br />

ii) /ɡɐn/ vs /pɐn/ vs /bɐn/ vs /tɐn/ vs /dɐn/<br />

� Again, only five stops belong to this paradigm. A single minimal<br />

pair contrasting /ɡ/ <strong>and</strong> /k/ is required now to fully demonstrate<br />

the set of English stop consonants.<br />

iii) /ɡ{In/ vs /k{In/


Contrast in Analogous Environment (1)<br />

� Sometimes it is not possible to find a minimal<br />

pair which would support the contrastiveness<br />

of two phonemes <strong>and</strong> it is necessary to resort<br />

to examples of contrast in analogous<br />

environment (CAE).<br />

� CAE is almost a minimal pair, however the<br />

pair of words differs by more than just the<br />

pair of sounds in question.


Contrast in Analogous Environment (2)<br />

� Preferably, in CAE, the other points of<br />

variation in the pair of words are as remote<br />

as possible (i.e. not adjacent <strong>and</strong> preferably<br />

not in the same syllable) from the pair of<br />

sounds being tested.<br />

� The further away the other contrast is, the<br />

more unlikely it is to have any conditioning<br />

effect on the selection of pair phones of<br />

interest.


Contrast in Analogous Environment (3)<br />

� eg. /ʃ/ vs /ʒ/ in English are usually supported<br />

by examples of CAE pairs such as<br />

"pressure" [preʃə] vs "treasure" [treʒə].<br />

� The only true minimal pairs for these two<br />

sounds in English involve at least one word<br />

(often a proper noun) that has been borrowed<br />

from another language (eg. "Confucian"<br />

[kənfjʉːʃən] vs "confusion" [kənfjʉːʒən], <strong>and</strong><br />

"Aleutian" [əlʉːʃən] vs "allusion" [əlʉːʒən]).


Minimal pairs versus CAE<br />

� Even one example of a minimal pair might be<br />

considered good evidence that two sounds<br />

are allophones of different phonemes.<br />

� Minimal pairs are reliable evidence in<br />

phonemic analysis.<br />

� CAE is poorer evidence in phonemic analysis<br />

<strong>and</strong> ideally requires other supporting<br />

evidence (eg. similar patterns for similar<br />

pairs of sounds confirmed to contrast by CIE)


Syntagmatic analysis (1)<br />

� A syntagmatic analysis of a speech sound<br />

identifies all of the locations or contexts within<br />

the words of a particular language where the<br />

sound can be found.<br />

� Note that in the following examples (next<br />

page), "#" is used to represent a word or<br />

syllable boundary, "V" represents any vowel,<br />

<strong>and</strong> "C" represents any other consonant.


Syntagmatic analysis (2)<br />

For example, English [n] <strong>and</strong> [ŋ] :-<br />

� a syntagm of the phone [n] in English could be<br />

in the form:-<br />

( #CnV..., #nV..., ...Vn#, ...VnC#, ...VnV..., etc.)<br />

� whilst [ŋ] in English would be:-<br />

(...Vŋ#, ...VŋC#, ...VŋV..., etc)<br />

but would not include the word initial forms.


Syntagmatic analysis (3)<br />

� For example, sequences of the type<br />

"#CnV..." would include "snow" [snəʉ],<br />

"snort" [sno:t] <strong>and</strong> "snooker" [snʉ:kə].<br />

� In this case, the only consonant (for English)<br />

that can occupy the initial "C" slot is the<br />

phoneme /s/, <strong>and</strong> so the generalised pattern<br />

could be rewritten as "#snV...".


Syntagmatic analysis (4)<br />

� A syntagmatic analysis can provide information<br />

about different restricted distributions of two<br />

allophones of the same phoneme (always found in<br />

different locations).<br />

� A syntagmatic analysis might help to strengthen a<br />

case based on a CAE analysis. For example, we<br />

might find CAE for [p, b] <strong>and</strong> we find that all other<br />

oral stop pairs (e.g. [t, d] <strong>and</strong> [k, g]) have minimal<br />

pairs in the same syllable location. So this would<br />

strengthen the case for [p, b] being separate<br />

phonemes.


Complementary Distribution (1)<br />

� A phoneme may be realised by more than<br />

one speech sound <strong>and</strong> the selection of each<br />

variant is usually conditioned by the phonetic<br />

environment of the phoneme.<br />

� This is known as mutually exclusive or<br />

complementary distribution (CD)


Complementary Distribution (2)<br />

� The CD of two phonemes means that the two<br />

phonemes can never be found in the same<br />

environment (ie. the same environment in the<br />

senses of position in the word <strong>and</strong> the<br />

identity of adjacent phonemes).<br />

� If two sounds are phonetically similar <strong>and</strong><br />

they are in CD then they can be assumed to<br />

be allophones of the same phoneme.


Complementary Distribution (3)<br />

� eg. in many languages voiced <strong>and</strong> voiceless stops<br />

with the same place of articulation do not contrast<br />

linguistically but are rather two phonetic realisations<br />

of a single phoneme (ie. /p/=[p,b], /t/=[t,d], <strong>and</strong><br />

/k/=[k,ɡ]).<br />

� Whether the voiced or voiceless allophone is chosen<br />

depends upon syntagmatic distribution (e.g. where in<br />

the word) or phonetic context (e.g. whether the<br />

adjacent sounds are voiced or voiceless)


Complementary Distribution (4)<br />

� In some Australian Aboriginal languages word medial<br />

oral stops are voiced if both adjacent phonemes are<br />

voiced (e.g. between two vowels) <strong>and</strong> are voiceless if<br />

at least one of the adjacent sounds is voiceless.<br />

� For initial stops the patterns varies from language to<br />

language <strong>and</strong> even between dialects within a single<br />

language. In some dialects of a language the<br />

voiceless allophone is preferred, in others the voiced<br />

allophone is preferred, <strong>and</strong> in others the choice of<br />

allophone is a matter of individual choice.


Contrastive Distribution<br />

� Contrastive distribution is the opposite of<br />

complementary distribution.<br />

� Sounds in contrastive distribution can occur<br />

in the same location <strong>and</strong> when exchanged<br />

change the meaning of the word.<br />

� Sounds in contrastive distribution belong to<br />

different phonemes.


Free Variation<br />

� Occasionally speakers of a language are free<br />

to choose whether they use one or another of<br />

two possible allophones. The choice may be<br />

word-specific (often a sign of language change<br />

in progress). The choice may be pragmatic<br />

(discourse context) or sociolinguistic<br />

(e.g. Some French speakers choose to use the<br />

alveolar trill [r] when in the village <strong>and</strong> the more<br />

prestigious uvular trill [ʀ] when in Paris.)


Phonetic similarity (1)<br />

� <strong>Allophone</strong>s must be phonetically similar to<br />

each other.<br />

� In analysis, this means you can assume that<br />

highly dissimilar sounds are separate<br />

phonemes (even if they are in complementary<br />

distribution).<br />

� For this reason no attempt is made to find<br />

minimal pairs which contrast vowels with<br />

consonants. BUT…


Phonetic similarity (2)<br />

� Even the distinction between vowel <strong>and</strong><br />

consonants isn’t totally unproblematic.<br />

� For example, a sequence in one language<br />

which is perceived as a diphthong might in<br />

another language be perceived as a semivowel<br />

(approximant) <strong>and</strong> a vowel<br />

e.g. /ja/ ~ /ia/ OR /aj/ ~ /aI/<br />

� What happens when the speakers of one of<br />

these languages learns the other language?<br />

What is the interlanguage phonology?


Phonetic similarity (3)<br />

� Exactly what can be considered phonetically<br />

similar may vary somewhat from language to<br />

language <strong>and</strong> so the notion of phonetic<br />

similarity can seem to be quite unclear at times.<br />

� Sounds can be phonetically similar from both<br />

articulatory <strong>and</strong> auditory points of view. One<br />

finds pairs of sounds that vary greatly in their<br />

place of articulation but are sufficiently similar<br />

auditorily to be considered phonetically similar.


Phonetic similarity (4)<br />

� According to Hockett (1942), "...if a <strong>and</strong> b are<br />

members of one phoneme, they share one or<br />

more features".<br />

� Phonetic similarity is therefore based on the<br />

notion of shared features.<br />

� Such judgments of similarity will vary from<br />

language to language <strong>and</strong> there are no<br />

universal criteria for similarity.


Phonetic similarity (5)<br />

� Example 1. Glottal [h] <strong>and</strong> palatal [ç] are<br />

voiceless fricatives which are distant in terms<br />

of places of articulation, but they share<br />

features <strong>and</strong> are sufficiently similar auditorily<br />

(both weak sounding, voiceless, non-tonguetip,<br />

non-labial, fricatives) to be allophones of<br />

a single phoneme in some languages such<br />

as Japanese.


Phonetic similarity (6)<br />

� Example 2. In English, /h/ <strong>and</strong> /ŋ/ are in<br />

complementary distribution. /h/ only ever occurs<br />

at the beginning of a syllable (head, heart,<br />

enhance, perhaps) whilst /ŋ/ only ever occurs at<br />

the end of a syllable (sing, singer, finger).<br />

� The differ in place, manner <strong>and</strong> voicing <strong>and</strong> in<br />

tongue body, velum <strong>and</strong> laryngeal gestures.<br />

They are so different that no one regards them<br />

as allophones of the one phoneme.


Phonetic similarity (7)<br />

� So, if phonetic similarity is so difficult to pin<br />

down, then is it a useful tool in phonemic<br />

analysis.<br />

� Yes, it is. What we need to be aware of are<br />

the common patterns of phonetic similarity<br />

across languages. What follows are some of<br />

the common (but in no case universal)<br />

patterns…


Phonetic similarity (8)<br />

� i) two sounds differing only in voicing:<br />

[pb] [td] [kɡ] [ɸβ] [θð] [sz] [ʃʒ] [xɣ] etc...<br />

� ii) two sounds differing in manner of<br />

articulation only as oral stop vs fricative. The<br />

sibilant or grooved fricatives [s,z,ʃ,ʒ] are<br />

excluded from this category as they are quite<br />

different auditorily from the other fricatives.<br />

[pɸ] [kx] [bβ] [ɡɣ] etc...


Phonetic similarity (9)<br />

� iii) Any pairs of consonants close in place of<br />

articulation <strong>and</strong> differing in no other<br />

contrastive feature:<br />

[sʃ] [zʒ] [nɲŋ] [lɭ] [lʎ] [mɱ], etc...<br />

� iv) Any other pairs of consonants which are<br />

close in articulation <strong>and</strong> differ by one other<br />

feature but are nevertheless frequently<br />

members of the same phoneme<br />

[lɹ] [cɡ] [tθ] [dð]


Phonetic similarity (10)<br />

� In languages where voicing is noncontrastive,<br />

two phones differing in voicing<br />

<strong>and</strong> only slightly in place of articulation might<br />

be considered similar e.g. [cɡ] etc.<br />

� Further, for the purposes of this type of<br />

analysis, the place of articulation of the<br />

apicodental fricatives [θ,ð] is considered to<br />

be close enough to that of the alveolar stops<br />

[t,d] to be considered phonetically similar.


Phonetic similarity (11)<br />

� v) Any two vowels differing in only one<br />

feature or articulated with adjacent tongue<br />

positions<br />

[æ ɐ] [i I] [ɐ: ɐ] [i y] [ɑ ɑ̃]


Phonetic similarity (12)<br />

� Although it is implied above that the notion of<br />

"phonetic similarity" is in some way less<br />

linguistically abstract (more phonetic?) than<br />

the notion of complementary distribution, it is,<br />

nevertheless, a quite abstract concept.<br />

� The are no obvious <strong>and</strong> consistent acoustic,<br />

auditory or articulatory criteria for phonetic<br />

similarity.


Phonetic similarity (13)<br />

� There are many examples of very similar phones<br />

which are perceived by native speakers to belong to<br />

separate phonemes.<br />

� In English, for example, a word terminal voiceless stop<br />

may be either released <strong>and</strong> aspirated or unreleased.<br />

The homorganic (same place of articulation) voiced<br />

stop may also be released or unreleased.<br />

� Often the unreleased voiced <strong>and</strong> voiceless stops may<br />

actually be identical in every way except that the<br />

preceding vowel is lengthened before the<br />

phonologically voiced stop.


Phonetic similarity (14)<br />

� In terms of phonetic similarity, the two<br />

unreleased stops may actually be identical<br />

<strong>and</strong> yet be perceived by native speakers to<br />

belong to different phonemes because of<br />

their different effect on the preceding vowel.<br />

� /kɐp/→[kɐpʰ] ... [kɐp̚ ]<br />

/kɐb/→[kɐˑb] ... [kɐˑb̚ ] ... [kɐˑp̚ ]<br />

(nb. " ̚ " means unreleased stop <strong>and</strong> " ˑ "<br />

means partially lengthened vowel)


Phonetic similarity (15)<br />

� Conversely, phones which are very dissimilar<br />

(at least from certain perspectives) may be<br />

felt by native speakers to belong to a single<br />

phoneme.<br />

� eg. Japanese (1)<br />

/h/ → [ɸ] before /u/ eg.[ɸuku] "luck“<br />

/h/ → [ç] before /i/ eg.[çito] "man“<br />

/h/ → [h] before /e,a,o/ eg.[hana]<br />

(1) Japanese in the mid 20 th century. This pattern has undergone recent change.


Phonemic Pattern<br />

� A pair of phones in complementary distribution may<br />

sometimes be classified into separate phonemes on<br />

the basis of phonemic pattern.<br />

� In other words, is there a group of phonemes which<br />

exhibit a similar pattern of distribution (eg. clustering<br />

behaviour, morphology, etc.) to one of the phones<br />

being examined?<br />

� In the case of the pair [h], [ŋ] there are some<br />

similarities in patterning between [h] <strong>and</strong> certain<br />

fricatives, <strong>and</strong> between [ŋ] <strong>and</strong> the other nasal stops.


Phonological Space (1)<br />

� The greater the distance between a<br />

phoneme <strong>and</strong> its nearest neighbours, the<br />

greater the scope for allophonic variation.<br />

� In other words, the larger the number of<br />

redundant features (ie. features which when<br />

changed will not create another phoneme)<br />

the greater the number of allophones which<br />

can actually occur.


Phonological Space (2)<br />

� eg. English<br />

/p/ = [-voice] [+bilabial] [+stop] [-nasal] [+/-aspirated]<br />

(nb. + present, - absent, +/- optional)<br />

� Changing the feature [-voice] to [+voice] will create /b/,<br />

changing the feature [bilabial] may create /t,k/,<br />

changing the feature [stop] may create /w,f/,<br />

changing the feature [nasal] will create /m/.<br />

� The only feature with complete freedom of movement<br />

is aspiration, <strong>and</strong> variation of this feature does create<br />

the main pair of allophones of this phoneme in English.


Phonological Space (3)<br />

� eg. English<br />

� /r/ → [ɹ] alveolar approximant<br />

� /r/ → [R&] voiceless alveolar approximant (e.g. after /t/)<br />

� /r/ → [ɻ ] retroflex approximant (West Engl<strong>and</strong>)<br />

� /r/ → [ɾ] alveolar flap (Scottish) eg. [ɡɾIn]<br />

� /r/ → [ʁ] uvular fricative (Tyneside, UK)<br />

� The possible varieties of /r/ seem to include variations<br />

of manner, place <strong>and</strong> voicing. The only restrictions are<br />

that its allophones may not overlap with those of /l/<br />

<strong>and</strong> /w/.


<strong>Allophone</strong>s & Narrow Transcription (1)<br />

� It should now be obvious that broad<br />

(phonemic) transcription is only possible<br />

following phonemic analysis. We can’t<br />

transcribe the phonemes until we know what<br />

they are.<br />

� To what extent do narrow transcriptions<br />

match the result of the identification of<br />

allophones in phonemic analysis?


<strong>Allophone</strong>s & Narrow Transcription (2)<br />

� No narrow transcription captures all<br />

allophones of each phoneme. As we have<br />

already seen, each phoneme has an<br />

extremely large number of slightly varying<br />

allophones.<br />

� In this course we greatly simplify narrow<br />

transcription <strong>and</strong> ignore a lot of significant<br />

Australian English allophones.


<strong>Allophone</strong>s & Narrow Transcription (3)<br />

� What might be a maximal narrow transcription<br />

of a language (e.g. Australian English)?<br />

� The maximal set of possible distinctions that<br />

we can transcribe is limited by the availability<br />

of IPA symbols, including the diacritics <strong>and</strong> by<br />

our ability to hear (or otherwise measure)<br />

these differences.


<strong>Allophone</strong>s & Narrow Transcription (4)<br />

� For example we can use, not only the IPA<br />

vowel symbols but also the raising, lowering,<br />

fronting <strong>and</strong> backing diacritics to indicate a<br />

large variety of vowel qualities.<br />

� We can also provide diacritics for vowel<br />

nasalisation, breathy voice <strong>and</strong> creaky voice<br />

as well as voiceless (for whispered vowels).<br />

� For Arabic vowels we could add a velarised<br />

diacritic (for velarised vowels).


<strong>Allophone</strong>s & Narrow Transcription (5)<br />

� Traditionally, phonemic analysis was limited<br />

by the accuracy <strong>and</strong> degree of detail of a<br />

narrow transcription. If a feature was missed<br />

in a narrow transcription it wasn’t part of the<br />

phonemic analysis.<br />

� Today we can add detail from acoustic <strong>and</strong><br />

physiological measurement when judging<br />

allophonic variation.


<strong>Phoneme</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Allophone</strong><br />

� This is a complex topic. You will not become<br />

sufficiently familiar with it without practice in actual<br />

phonemic analysis.<br />

� You should review the unit web site. The web site<br />

has some additional material not covered here<br />

(especially “The Premises of Phonemic Analysis”).<br />

� Read <strong>and</strong> try all of the phonemic analysis tutorial<br />

exercises, including the complex ones. Solutions will<br />

be available after the relevant tutorial.


Readings<br />

� The unit web site:http://www.ling.mq.edu.au/speech/phonetics/phonology/phoneme/index.html<br />

� Clark, J., Yallop, C., & Fletcher, J. (2007), An introduction to<br />

phonetics <strong>and</strong> phonology, Blackwell (3rd edition).


References<br />

Referred to in preparation of these notes, but not required reading<br />

� Hockett, C.F. (1942) “A System of Descriptive Phonology”, Language,<br />

18(1), 3-21<br />

� Pike, K.L. (1947) Phonemics, U.Michigan<br />

� Trubetzkoy, N.S. (1939) Grundzüge der Phonologie. Travaux du<br />

Cercle Linguistique de Prague 7, Reprinted 1958, Göttingen:<br />

V<strong>and</strong>enhoek & Ruprecht. Translated into English by C.A.M.Baltaxe<br />

1969 as Principles of Phonology, Berkeley: University of California<br />

Press.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!