24.10.2012 Views

The Role of Male Sexual Arousal in Rape: Six Models

The Role of Male Sexual Arousal in Rape: Six Models

The Role of Male Sexual Arousal in Rape: Six Models

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

622 HOWARD E. BARBAREE AND WILLIAM L. MARSHALL<br />

determ<strong>in</strong>ants (Craig, 1990). Recently, it has been suggested that<br />

rape is a heterogeneous phenomenon, with different <strong>in</strong>stances<br />

<strong>of</strong> the behavior requir<strong>in</strong>g different explanations. Knight and<br />

Prentky (1990) have presented a psychological typology <strong>of</strong> rap-<br />

ists, grounded <strong>in</strong> empirical <strong>in</strong>vestigation, that specifies differ-<br />

ent motives underly<strong>in</strong>g the behavior <strong>in</strong> different subtypes <strong>of</strong><br />

rapists. In addition, Malamuth has presented a predictive<br />

model <strong>of</strong> sexual assault that attributes the behavior to multiple<br />

predispos<strong>in</strong>g characteristics (Malamuth, 1986, 1988). <strong>The</strong>re-<br />

fore, rape is be<strong>in</strong>g seen less and less as a unitary phenomenon.<br />

<strong>Sexual</strong> arousal <strong>in</strong> the male perpetrator is an obvious and<br />

important component <strong>in</strong> most acts <strong>of</strong> rape. Study<strong>in</strong>g sexual<br />

arousal <strong>in</strong> rape directly as it occurs <strong>in</strong> the natural environment<br />

is obviously out <strong>of</strong> the question, and most <strong>of</strong> what we know <strong>of</strong><br />

sexual arousal <strong>in</strong> rape comes from <strong>in</strong>terviews with rapists and<br />

their victims. Rapists are generally regarded as poor sources <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>formation because they tend to lie or distort <strong>in</strong>formation<br />

regard<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>of</strong>fense (Scully & Marola, 1984). <strong>The</strong>re have been<br />

several approaches to deal<strong>in</strong>g with this dishonesty. For exam-<br />

ple, some <strong>in</strong>vestigators have encouraged honesty by mak<strong>in</strong>g<br />

special arrangements to protect the confidentiality <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>forma-<br />

tion (Abel et al., 1988) or the rapist's anonymity (Koss & D<strong>in</strong>ero,<br />

1988). Other authors have written <strong>of</strong> their cl<strong>in</strong>ical impressions<br />

<strong>of</strong> rapists, and their descriptions <strong>of</strong> the motivation for rape have<br />

been <strong>in</strong>valuable <strong>in</strong> reach<strong>in</strong>g our current understand<strong>in</strong>g <strong>of</strong> rape<br />

(Groth, 1979). <strong>The</strong> social psychological approach has depended<br />

almost exclusively on self-report responses to survey question-<br />

naires as a research methodology. <strong>The</strong> cl<strong>in</strong>ical approach has<br />

depended heavily on self-report, <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>terview and <strong>in</strong> responses<br />

to questionnaires, but this approach has also utilized labora-<br />

tory assessments <strong>of</strong> sexual arousal and preferences as an impor-<br />

tant assessment measure and treatment target.<br />

Studies <strong>of</strong> sexual arousal among rapists and nonrapists <strong>in</strong> the<br />

laboratory us<strong>in</strong>g analog assessment techniques have been used<br />

to study the role <strong>of</strong> sexual arousal <strong>in</strong> rape, and the current<br />

article will focus on the results <strong>of</strong> these studies. Most <strong>of</strong> the<br />

studies have been done <strong>in</strong> the follow<strong>in</strong>g way. <strong>Male</strong> subjects are<br />

recruited, either from among <strong>in</strong>carcerated or convicted rapists<br />

or from a community sample <strong>of</strong> men serv<strong>in</strong>g as a comparison<br />

group. <strong>The</strong> community sample is usually designated as a non-<br />

rapist sample, although a m<strong>in</strong>ority <strong>of</strong> these men will have quite<br />

likely engaged <strong>in</strong> some k<strong>in</strong>d <strong>of</strong> sexually aggressive behavior<br />

(Koss & D<strong>in</strong>ero, 1988). In a laboratory sett<strong>in</strong>g, their erectile<br />

response is monitored, usually by circumferential stra<strong>in</strong> gauge<br />

plethysmography, while they are presented with verbal descrip-<br />

tions <strong>of</strong> consent<strong>in</strong>g sexual activity between adults and <strong>of</strong> rape <strong>in</strong><br />

which a male forces <strong>in</strong>tercourse on an adult woman.<br />

<strong>The</strong> present article reviews the literature concerned with sex-<br />

ual arousal to rape cues and describes six different models, each<br />

specify<strong>in</strong>g a different relationship between the aggressive<br />

aspects <strong>of</strong> rape and the perpetrator's sexual response. We do not<br />

argue for the supremacy <strong>of</strong> any particular model. In fact, differ-<br />

ent models may be helpful <strong>in</strong> understand<strong>in</strong>g the role <strong>of</strong> sexual<br />

arousal <strong>in</strong> different rapes or <strong>in</strong> the rapes <strong>of</strong> different rapists.<br />

<strong>The</strong> models are be<strong>in</strong>g presented <strong>in</strong> two categories. First, there<br />

are those that focus on the nature <strong>of</strong> the responses <strong>in</strong>volved, and<br />

these are referred to as response control models. Second, there<br />

are those that focus on the way <strong>in</strong> which stimuli associated with<br />

rape behavior evoke sexual arousal. We refer to these as stimu-<br />

lus control models. <strong>The</strong>re are four <strong>of</strong> these presented. Most <strong>of</strong><br />

the models that are presented here are "trait" models <strong>in</strong> the<br />

sense that they postulate an organization <strong>of</strong> behavior <strong>in</strong> the<br />

perpetrators <strong>of</strong> rape that dist<strong>in</strong>guishes them from normal men.<br />

One <strong>of</strong> the models is a "state" model <strong>in</strong> the sense that it postu-<br />

lates the potential for a transitory state that has the potential for<br />

<strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g the risk <strong>of</strong> rape <strong>in</strong> normal men.<br />

<strong>The</strong> models will be presented <strong>in</strong> order <strong>of</strong> <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g complex-<br />

ity. Parsimony demands that we favor the less complex models,<br />

and it may be that some f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs currently understood with<strong>in</strong><br />

the framework <strong>of</strong> a more complex model will eventually be fully<br />

expla<strong>in</strong>ed by us<strong>in</strong>g a more straightforward model.<br />

A number <strong>of</strong> published articles have discussed the method-<br />

ological issues relat<strong>in</strong>g to penile plethysmography and its use as<br />

an assessment <strong>of</strong> sexual <strong>of</strong>fenders (Barbaree, 1990; Earls & Mar-<br />

shall, 1983). <strong>The</strong>se articles have rightly po<strong>in</strong>ted to the factors<br />

that may contribute to error variance <strong>in</strong> these measures, such as<br />

construction <strong>of</strong> the stimuli and the ability <strong>of</strong> subjects to sup-<br />

press arousal or to "fake" responses. We will not attend to these<br />

issues, but rather focus on the conceptual and theoretical issues<br />

perta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g to the measurement <strong>of</strong> rape arousal. Often, method-<br />

ological and conceptual issues will seem to be synonymous. For<br />

example, men may have the ability to suppress their arousal to<br />

avoid the detection <strong>of</strong> a deviant sexual <strong>in</strong>terest by the assessor,<br />

and the problem <strong>of</strong> fak<strong>in</strong>g has been addressed as an important<br />

methodological issue (Qu<strong>in</strong>sey & Chapl<strong>in</strong>, 1988). On the other<br />

hand, the first model presented here highlights men's ability to<br />

suppress as a conceptual issue, by postulat<strong>in</strong>g important differ-<br />

ences among men <strong>in</strong> the ability to suppress and by postulat<strong>in</strong>g a<br />

relationship between this ability and the propensity to commit<br />

sexual aggression (Hall, 1989). Because <strong>of</strong> limited space, we will<br />

not be able to discuss the methodological issues <strong>in</strong> detail.<br />

Ability to Suppress <strong>Arousal</strong><br />

Response Control <strong>Models</strong><br />

<strong>The</strong> first model presented is one <strong>of</strong> the more recently de-<br />

scribed <strong>in</strong> the literature. Hall (1989) suggested this very simple<br />

model after present<strong>in</strong>g data collected from a large sample <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>carcerated sexual <strong>of</strong>fenders. Hall (1989) presented these <strong>of</strong>-<br />

fenders with verbal descriptions <strong>of</strong> consent<strong>in</strong>g sexual <strong>in</strong>terac-<br />

tions, rape, and nonsexual assault while monitor<strong>in</strong>g their erec-<br />

tile responses <strong>in</strong> the laboratory. At the end <strong>of</strong> the session, he<br />

presented the stimulus that had evoked the greatest arousal <strong>in</strong><br />

the session aga<strong>in</strong> to each subject, with <strong>in</strong>structions to <strong>in</strong>hibit<br />

their arousal as best they could. He then divided his subjects<br />

<strong>in</strong>to those who were able to <strong>in</strong>hibit their arousal and those who<br />

were not. Those subjects who were not able to <strong>in</strong>hibit had<br />

shown greater arousal to stimuli depict<strong>in</strong>g sexual assault <strong>in</strong> the<br />

session before be<strong>in</strong>g given <strong>in</strong>structions to <strong>in</strong>hibit. In an earlier<br />

study, Abel, Blanchard, and Barlow (1981) found that some<br />

rapists actually showed <strong>in</strong>creases <strong>in</strong> arousal to rape cues after<br />

they had been <strong>in</strong>structed to suppress arousal.<br />

This model suggests that an explanation <strong>of</strong> sexual arousal to<br />

rape cues <strong>in</strong> the laboratory might reduce to the ability the sub-<br />

ject shows <strong>in</strong> consciously suppress<strong>in</strong>g his sexual arousal. Even<br />

though most <strong>of</strong> the subjects <strong>in</strong> the Hall (1989) study, all <strong>of</strong> whom<br />

were <strong>of</strong>fenders, were able to suppress their arousal, this model

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!