06.09.2013 Views

CONFLICT MANAGEMENT The Psychology of conflict and conflict ...

CONFLICT MANAGEMENT The Psychology of conflict and conflict ...

CONFLICT MANAGEMENT The Psychology of conflict and conflict ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

32 DE DREU AND GELFAND<br />

well as in a variety <strong>of</strong> field studies, <strong>and</strong> has generally received good support<br />

(for a review, see Tjosvold, 1998). This work has also shown that constructive<br />

controversy yields desirable outcomes to disputants <strong>and</strong> their collective<br />

alike. It promotes learning <strong>and</strong> innovation, team effectiveness, <strong>and</strong> the quality<br />

<strong>of</strong> group decision making, to name but a few.<br />

Another important theory about <strong>conflict</strong> management is Dual Concern<br />

<strong>The</strong>ory (Pruitt & Rubin, 1986; also see Blake & Mouton, 1964; Olekalns et al.,<br />

chapter 3, this volume). In a nutshell, it argues that <strong>conflict</strong> management is<br />

a function <strong>of</strong> high or low concern for self combined with high or low concern<br />

for other. A high concern for self <strong>and</strong> low concern for other results in a<br />

preference for forcing focused on imposing one’s will on the other side (unilateral<br />

action). Forcing involves threats <strong>and</strong> bluffs, persuasive arguments,<br />

<strong>and</strong> positional commitments. Low concern for self <strong>and</strong> high concern for<br />

other results in a preference for yielding, which is oriented toward accepting<br />

<strong>and</strong> incorporating other’s will. It involves unilateral concessions, unconditional<br />

promises, <strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong>fering help (unilateral action). Low concern for self<br />

<strong>and</strong> other results in a preference for avoiding, which involves reducing the<br />

importance <strong>of</strong> the issues <strong>and</strong> attempts to suppress thinking about the issues<br />

(unilateral action). High concern for self <strong>and</strong> other produces a preference for<br />

problem solving, which is oriented toward an agreement that satisfies both<br />

own <strong>and</strong> other’s aspirations as much as possible (joint action). It involves an<br />

exchange <strong>of</strong> information about priorities <strong>and</strong> preferences, showing insights,<br />

<strong>and</strong> making trade<strong>of</strong>fs between important <strong>and</strong> unimportant issues.<br />

Recently, some authors have suggested that intermediate concern for<br />

self paired with intermediate concern for other results in a preference for<br />

compromising. Some see compromising as “half-hearted problem solving”<br />

(e.g., Pruitt & Rubin, 1986). Others, however, see it as a distinct strategy<br />

that involves the matching <strong>of</strong> other’s concessions, the making <strong>of</strong> conditional<br />

promises <strong>and</strong> threats, <strong>and</strong> an active search for a middle ground<br />

(e.g., Van de Vliert, 1997). Empirically, the debate seems to be settled in<br />

favor <strong>of</strong> those viewing compromise as a separate strategy, although more<br />

work needs to be done (De Dreu, Evers, Beersma, Kluwer, & Nauta, 2001).<br />

Ury, Brett, <strong>and</strong> Goldberg (1993) differentiated forcing <strong>and</strong> dominating into<br />

two subcategories—rights <strong>and</strong> power—in their taxonomy <strong>of</strong> approaches to<br />

dispute resolution. When using a rights-based approach, parties attempt to<br />

resolve the dispute by applying some st<strong>and</strong>ard <strong>of</strong> fairness, precedent, contract,<br />

or law. A focus on rights is likely to lead to agreements in which each<br />

party has to give up something in order to reach an agreement, with the possibility<br />

<strong>of</strong> one party giving more than receiving. A power-based approach<br />

results in the dispute being resolved by determining which party is able to<br />

force his or her desired outcome—who is stronger, has higher status, is able<br />

to coerce the other, or can force a concession from the other party. A powerbased<br />

approach usually leads to agreements that have greater potential to<br />

escalate due to feelings <strong>of</strong> resentment <strong>and</strong> a desire for revenge (Brett et al.,<br />

1998; Tinsley, 2001; also see Friedman et al., chapter 12, this volume; Goldman<br />

et al., chapter 10, this volume; Pruitt, chapter 8, this volume). While

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!