06.09.2013 Views

CONFLICT MANAGEMENT The Psychology of conflict and conflict ...

CONFLICT MANAGEMENT The Psychology of conflict and conflict ...

CONFLICT MANAGEMENT The Psychology of conflict and conflict ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

1. <strong>CONFLICT</strong> IN THE WORKPLACE 21<br />

identity is likely to be a fundamental need that traverses national cultures<br />

(thus producing affective or value-based <strong>conflict</strong>s). Finally, differences in<br />

viewpoints are perhaps inevitable regardless <strong>of</strong> the national culture <strong>conflict</strong>,<br />

suggesting that our third source <strong>of</strong> <strong>conflict</strong>—cognitive <strong>conflict</strong>—is also<br />

a universal antecedent <strong>of</strong> <strong>conflict</strong>.<br />

At the same time, although these are likely broad universal sources <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>conflict</strong>, cross-cultural variation exists in the specific triggers <strong>of</strong> <strong>conflict</strong>s<br />

within each category. For example, what is perceived to be a threat to<br />

one’s values can clearly vary across national cultures. Shteynberg, Gelf<strong>and</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Kim (2005) argued that <strong>conflict</strong> processes are “sparked” or<br />

initiated when core cultural focal concerns are violated, <strong>and</strong> thus, different<br />

events elicit <strong>conflict</strong> in different cultural contexts. Consistent with this,<br />

they found that violations to rights were perceived to be much more<br />

harmful <strong>and</strong> incited anger to a much greater degree in the United States,<br />

whereas violations to duties <strong>and</strong> face caused much more harm <strong>and</strong><br />

incited anger to a greater degree in Korea. Individuals can project their<br />

values onto identical situations <strong>and</strong> perceive different “cultural slights”<br />

that are salient in their own cultural contexts. For example, Gelf<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

colleagues (2001) found that identical <strong>conflict</strong> situations were perceived<br />

differently in the United States <strong>and</strong> Japan: Americans perceived <strong>conflict</strong>s<br />

to be more about winning <strong>and</strong> violations to individual rights, whereas<br />

Japanese perceived the same <strong>conflict</strong>s to be about compromise <strong>and</strong> violations<br />

to duties <strong>and</strong> obligations.<br />

<strong>The</strong> processes that lead to value <strong>conflict</strong>s can also vary across cultures.<br />

For example, self-serving biases <strong>and</strong> self-enhancement—processes we<br />

have argued are facilitating factors in value <strong>conflict</strong>s—have been found to<br />

be more pronounced in individualistic cultures as compared with collectivistic<br />

cultures (Gelf<strong>and</strong> et al., 2002; see also Heine, Lehman, Markus, &<br />

Kitayama, 1999). Other self-processes—such as self-esteem <strong>and</strong> self-concept<br />

clarity, previously discussed—may also play a more powerful role in<br />

the production <strong>of</strong> <strong>conflict</strong> in individualistic cultures. On the other h<strong>and</strong>,<br />

given that individuals in collectivistic cultures focus on group identity,<br />

group-level constructs—group enhancement, group esteem, group concept<br />

clarity—will play a more powerful role in the elicitation <strong>of</strong> <strong>conflict</strong><br />

in collectivistic cultures. This is consistent with research that has shown<br />

that samples from collectivistic cultures are much more competitive in<br />

intergroup or outgroup negotiations (Chen & Li, 2005; Probst, Carnevale,<br />

& Tri<strong>and</strong>is, 1999; Tri<strong>and</strong>is et al., 2001).<br />

We would also argue that although resource <strong>and</strong> cognitive-based <strong>conflict</strong>s<br />

are likely universal, there is likely cultural variation in the nature<br />

<strong>and</strong> extent <strong>of</strong> these antecedents. Given that individuals are socialized to<br />

develop, express, <strong>and</strong> affirm their own ideas <strong>and</strong> seek to maximize their<br />

own outcomes in individualistic cultures, resource <strong>and</strong> cognitive-based<br />

<strong>conflict</strong>s might be more rampant at the individual level in these contexts.<br />

By contrast, in collectivistic cultures, individuals are socialized to subject<br />

their own interests <strong>and</strong> opinions to the group (<strong>and</strong> in high-power distance

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!