Copyright Malvin Porter, Jr. 2010 - acumen - The University of ...

Copyright Malvin Porter, Jr. 2010 - acumen - The University of ... Copyright Malvin Porter, Jr. 2010 - acumen - The University of ...

acumen.lib.ua.edu
from acumen.lib.ua.edu More from this publisher
15.08.2013 Views

individual data points. Even though internal consistencies can take on any value less than or equal to 1, where the positive values are more meaningful, higher values of .70 or more are more desirable (Lomax, 2001). While there were low internal consistencies for each Action and Justification subscale within each of the four stories, within story correlations revealed mixed patterns of significance indicating methodological problems with combining both story character role and story form of victimization for story analysis. One explanation might be that multiple story variables within a single story confused children as the tried to imagine multiple roles and forms of victimization. Future studies should use stories that clearly delineate single variables in order to eliminate confounding variables. Another explanation for low internal consistencies within stories might be the way in which the measure was administered. Children were only given the option of the online format and completing the measure in a group setting among their peers. They did not have the option of using paper and pencil or to participate in an individual setting. Children might have lost concentration and responded by keying the same values such as all 1’s or 5’s, or with a repeated sequence of values in order to quickly complete the task. Upon close examination of the response data, only two of original 165 participants either failed to complete the survey and were eliminated from the data set. Another possibility might have been that children were confused about how to interpret rated items or by the directions such as what was meant by the terms good, bad, best or worst. Even though the researcher addressed all questions that were asked by children and maintained close proximity in order to answer individual questions, the possibility exists that children might have been reluctant to raise questions in the group for fear of calling attention to themselves among their peers. For future studies, the peer effect can be controlled for 140

y administering the measure in a individual setting. Additionally, problems with systematic response errors can be alleviated by use of paper and pencil formats in an interview format and by administering the online measure one-on-one with an interviewer present to clarify confusing items or terms. While the lack of significant reliability and presence of mixed correlations within stories lead to the decision to abandon the general linear model for rated data in favor of a nonparametic analyses of the categorical ranked data, findings within the CBVS Action Choice and Justification Choice variables across all four stories revealed moderate to strong internal consistency magnitudes. These results will be integrated into the discussion of the nonparametric findings. The Relationship Between Action Choices and Justification Choices How do children’s Action Choices relate to their justifications? This study found that there is a dependency between CBVS Action Choice and CBVS Justification Choice. While these findings are not unexpected, these variables were rated by the same source using related methods. These findings may have an inflated association between these variables due to the common source, which was not seen when compared to teacher ratings. First, this study found that children’s Prosocial Action Choices relate directly to their Prosocial/Care Justification Choices and to Justice/Fair Justification Choices. Specifically, children who choose Prosocial/Care Justification Choices were more likely to choose Prosocial Action Choices than Aggressive Action Choices. Not surprisingly, other studies found that prosocial behavior begins at an early age and that personality disposition and prosocial parenting have a significant influence on a child’s development of prosocial dispositions (Eisenberg, Fabes, & Spinrad, 2006; Scourfield, John, Martin, & McGuffin, 2004). The significance of the 141

y administering the measure in a individual setting. Additionally, problems with systematic<br />

response errors can be alleviated by use <strong>of</strong> paper and pencil formats in an interview format and<br />

by administering the online measure one-on-one with an interviewer present to clarify confusing<br />

items or terms.<br />

While the lack <strong>of</strong> significant reliability and presence <strong>of</strong> mixed correlations within stories<br />

lead to the decision to abandon the general linear model for rated data in favor <strong>of</strong> a nonparametic<br />

analyses <strong>of</strong> the categorical ranked data, findings within the CBVS Action Choice and<br />

Justification Choice variables across all four stories revealed moderate to strong internal<br />

consistency magnitudes. <strong>The</strong>se results will be integrated into the discussion <strong>of</strong> the nonparametric<br />

findings.<br />

<strong>The</strong> Relationship Between Action Choices and Justification Choices<br />

How do children’s Action Choices relate to their justifications? This study found that<br />

there is a dependency between CBVS Action Choice and CBVS Justification Choice. While these<br />

findings are not unexpected, these variables were rated by the same source using related<br />

methods. <strong>The</strong>se findings may have an inflated association between these variables due to the<br />

common source, which was not seen when compared to teacher ratings.<br />

First, this study found that children’s Prosocial Action Choices relate directly to their<br />

Prosocial/Care Justification Choices and to Justice/Fair Justification Choices. Specifically,<br />

children who choose Prosocial/Care Justification Choices were more likely to choose Prosocial<br />

Action Choices than Aggressive Action Choices. Not surprisingly, other studies found that<br />

prosocial behavior begins at an early age and that personality disposition and prosocial parenting<br />

have a significant influence on a child’s development <strong>of</strong> prosocial dispositions (Eisenberg,<br />

Fabes, & Spinrad, 2006; Scourfield, John, Martin, & McGuffin, 2004). <strong>The</strong> significance <strong>of</strong> the<br />

141

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!