01.08.2013 Views

Improving Literacy Skills in Students with Complex Communication ...

Improving Literacy Skills in Students with Complex Communication ...

Improving Literacy Skills in Students with Complex Communication ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Education and Tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Autism and Developmental Disabilities, 2011, 46(3), 352–368<br />

© Division on Autism and Developmental Disabilities<br />

<strong>Improv<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>Literacy</strong> <strong>Skills</strong> <strong>in</strong> <strong>Students</strong> <strong>with</strong> <strong>Complex</strong><br />

<strong>Communication</strong> Needs Who Use Augmentative/Alternative<br />

<strong>Communication</strong> Systems<br />

Rita L. Bailey, Maureen E. Angell, and Julia B. Stoner<br />

Ill<strong>in</strong>ois State University<br />

Abstract: A structured <strong>in</strong>tervention package <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g direct, scaffolded, <strong>in</strong>structional lessons was implemented<br />

us<strong>in</strong>g an error correction learn<strong>in</strong>g system and a picture book-based phonological and phonemic awareness<br />

activity for four participants <strong>with</strong> complex communication needs, rang<strong>in</strong>g from 12 to 15 years, <strong>in</strong> a junior high<br />

school sett<strong>in</strong>g. Although variability <strong>in</strong> participant results was noted, a functional relationship between the<br />

structured literacy <strong>in</strong>tervention package and sound to letter match<strong>in</strong>g ability was found. Results of this<br />

<strong>in</strong>vestigation <strong>in</strong>dicate that this <strong>in</strong>tervention package appears promis<strong>in</strong>g for facilitat<strong>in</strong>g learn<strong>in</strong>g of sound-toletter<br />

match<strong>in</strong>g skills <strong>in</strong> students <strong>with</strong> complex communication needs who use augmentative/alternative<br />

communication systems. However, due to the nature of s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject research, generalizability of results is <strong>in</strong><br />

question. Additionally, further research is needed to determ<strong>in</strong>e the effect of this <strong>in</strong>tervention on word decod<strong>in</strong>g<br />

and read<strong>in</strong>g comprehension skills.<br />

One of the primary goals for the education of<br />

all students, <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g those <strong>with</strong> complex<br />

communication needs (CCN) who use alternative<br />

and augmentative communication<br />

(AAC), is the development of literacy. As<br />

clearly stipulated by the No Child Left Beh<strong>in</strong>d<br />

(NCLB) Act and the Individuals <strong>with</strong> Disabilities<br />

Education Improvement Act ([IDEIA]<br />

IDEIA, 2004; NCLB, 2004), <strong>in</strong>struction and<br />

assessment of read<strong>in</strong>g and literacy skills must<br />

be addressed for children <strong>with</strong> disabilities.<br />

The IDEIA specifically requires “consideration”<br />

of assistive technology (AT) devices,<br />

which <strong>in</strong>clude AAC and services when develop<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Individualized Education Plans [IEPs;<br />

20 U.S.C. § 1401(1)-(2)]. For students who are<br />

AAC users, literacy takes on heightened importance.<br />

For students <strong>with</strong> developmental<br />

disabilities, literacy can foster new experiences<br />

and for students <strong>with</strong> severe physical limitations<br />

the acquisition and demonstration of<br />

literacy skills may be their primary entry <strong>in</strong>to<br />

Correspondence concern<strong>in</strong>g this article should<br />

be addressed to Rita L. Bailey, Department of <strong>Communication</strong><br />

Sciences and Disorders, Campus Box<br />

4720, Ill<strong>in</strong>ois State University, Normal, IL 61790-<br />

4720.<br />

the job market (Smith & Blischak, 1997). <strong>Literacy</strong><br />

development for students who use AAC<br />

is a complex issue, s<strong>in</strong>ce their language development,<br />

their opportunities to practice skills<br />

associated <strong>with</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g, and their ability to<br />

produce oral read<strong>in</strong>g can be impaired or extremely<br />

limited.<br />

Components of <strong>Literacy</strong><br />

<strong>Literacy</strong> requires knowledge of language components,<br />

such as phonology, morphology, syntax,<br />

semantics, and pragmatics (Strum & Clendon,<br />

2004). Specific skills associated <strong>with</strong><br />

these components <strong>in</strong>clude those that have<br />

been identified as preliteracy skills. These <strong>in</strong>clude<br />

components of phonemic awareness, alphabetic<br />

pr<strong>in</strong>ciple, fluency, concepts about<br />

pr<strong>in</strong>t, vocabulary development, and comprehension<br />

(Clay, 1975; National Read<strong>in</strong>g Panel,<br />

2000; Teale & Sulzby, 1986). Collectively,<br />

these skills provide the foundation for the<br />

development of read<strong>in</strong>g which is fundamental<br />

for <strong>in</strong>dependence <strong>in</strong> our society (International<br />

Read<strong>in</strong>g Association [IRA] & National<br />

Association for the Education of Young Children<br />

[NAEYC], 1996).<br />

Typically develop<strong>in</strong>g children beg<strong>in</strong> to<br />

learn preliteracy skills very early through ex-<br />

352 / Education and Tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Autism and Developmental Disabilities-September 2011


posure to language and pr<strong>in</strong>t, which <strong>in</strong>cludes<br />

story read<strong>in</strong>g and observ<strong>in</strong>g adult literacy activities.<br />

Preliteracy skills beg<strong>in</strong> to develop<br />

when children understand the relationship<br />

between word and pr<strong>in</strong>t, are able to identify<br />

phonemes <strong>in</strong> words, and are able to actively<br />

practice this knowledge through speech<br />

(Hetzroni, 2004). Language is the vehicle<br />

through which literacy is learned, ref<strong>in</strong>ed, and<br />

mastered.<br />

Language development <strong>in</strong> children <strong>with</strong><br />

CCN can be severely impaired due to accompany<strong>in</strong>g<br />

<strong>in</strong>tr<strong>in</strong>sic deficits, such as cognitive,<br />

physical, perceptual, or sensory impairments<br />

(Smith & Blischak, 1997) and significantly different<br />

language learn<strong>in</strong>g opportunities<br />

(Sturm & Clendon, 2004). <strong>Students</strong> <strong>with</strong> CCN<br />

may have restricted access to their environments,<br />

limited <strong>in</strong>teractions <strong>with</strong> communication<br />

partners, and fewer opportunities to communicate<br />

(Light & Drager, 2007; Sutton, Soto,<br />

& Blockberger, 2002). Not only may these<br />

opportunities be limited, but when they do<br />

occur, the quality may also be significantly<br />

altered due to variables associated <strong>with</strong> the<br />

child, the environment, the communication<br />

partner, and the mode or lack of expression.<br />

Children <strong>with</strong> CCN who use AAC may not<br />

have the opportunity to experience language<br />

as children <strong>with</strong>out speech impairments (Sutton<br />

et al., 2002). If language is delayed or<br />

impaired, development of literacy will also be<br />

affected. Language and literacy skills do not<br />

develop <strong>in</strong> isolation but over time and are<br />

dependent on students’ skills as well as environmental<br />

exposure and demands (Scherz &<br />

Hart, 2002). <strong>Students</strong> <strong>with</strong> CCN may face access<br />

barriers to develop<strong>in</strong>g literacy due to limited<br />

speech, lack of conversational opportunities,<br />

and/or concomitant sensory, cognitive,<br />

or motor disabilities (Hetzroni, 2004). Barriers<br />

that may affect literacy development are<br />

limited opportunities to ask questions or provide<br />

<strong>in</strong>put to vocabulary selection, limited<br />

practice <strong>with</strong> books, and low literacy expectations<br />

from education professionals (Hetzroni,<br />

2004; Light & Drager, 2008; Sturm & Clendon,<br />

2004). These obstacles <strong>in</strong> language and<br />

literacy acquisition are not faced by typically<br />

develop<strong>in</strong>g children (Blockberger & Johnston,<br />

2003). Research focus<strong>in</strong>g on students<br />

<strong>with</strong> CCN who use AAC has highlighted the<br />

discrepancy between their cognitive abilities<br />

and predicted literacy skills (Dahlgren Sandberg,<br />

2001; Dahlgren Sandberg & Hjelmquist,<br />

1996; Foley, 1993; Sutton, Soto & Blockberger,<br />

2002). Therefore, it is <strong>in</strong>cumbent on professionals<br />

who work <strong>with</strong> students <strong>with</strong> CCN to<br />

identify appropriate AAC to promote literacy<br />

and to develop effective strategies that foster<br />

acquisition of skills necessary for literacy development.<br />

Phonological Awareness<br />

Phonological awareness, which is awareness of<br />

the sound structure of spoken language, has<br />

been identified as vitally important to read<strong>in</strong>g<br />

and an unequivocal predictor of successful<br />

read<strong>in</strong>g ability (Adams, 1990; Dahlgren Sandberg,<br />

2001). Adams identified three levels of<br />

phonological awareness: (a) words <strong>in</strong> sentences,<br />

(b) syllables <strong>in</strong> words, and (c) sounds<br />

<strong>in</strong> syllables and words. The term phonemic<br />

awareness refers to the segment<strong>in</strong>g and manipulat<strong>in</strong>g<br />

of phonemes (Blischack, Shaw, Lombard<strong>in</strong>o,<br />

& Chiarella, 2004) and these two<br />

skills are vital to develop<strong>in</strong>g read<strong>in</strong>g ability.<br />

Segmentation strongly correlates <strong>with</strong> word<br />

read<strong>in</strong>g and comprehension. Manipulation<br />

<strong>in</strong>volves the ability to segment a word and<br />

then to manipulate one or more of the phonemes<br />

<strong>in</strong> that word. An example of this would<br />

be ask<strong>in</strong>g a student to identify the sounds <strong>in</strong><br />

cat and then replace the first phoneme <strong>with</strong> a<br />

different one such as /m/ to create a new<br />

word, mat. AAC users, who may not have the<br />

ability to produce speech, face unique challenges<br />

<strong>in</strong> the development of phonological<br />

awareness skills (Smith & Blischak, 1997).<br />

Recently, research has empirically validated<br />

strategies for teach<strong>in</strong>g phonological and phonemic<br />

awareness skills to children <strong>with</strong> CCN<br />

who are AAC users (Blischak et al., 2004; Fallon,<br />

Light, McNaughton, Drager, & Hammer,<br />

2004, Johnston, Buchanan, & Davenport,<br />

2009). Blischak et al. used a s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject<br />

multiple basel<strong>in</strong>e design to <strong>in</strong>vestigate the effect<br />

of a literacy <strong>in</strong>tervention <strong>with</strong> three students,<br />

ages 7.0, 5.0, 6.2, <strong>with</strong> severe speech<br />

impairment. The <strong>in</strong>tervention taught students<br />

phoneme-grapheme awareness followed by <strong>in</strong>struction<br />

<strong>in</strong> segment<strong>in</strong>g, manipulat<strong>in</strong>g, and<br />

encod<strong>in</strong>g consonant-vowel-consonant (CVC)<br />

pseudowords. Two of the participants met the<br />

criterion on the grapheme-phoneme aware-<br />

<strong>Improv<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>Literacy</strong> <strong>Skills</strong> / 353


ness tasks after three <strong>in</strong>structional sessions.<br />

One student had repeated difficulty and was<br />

not able to be advanced to segment<strong>in</strong>g, manipulat<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

and encod<strong>in</strong>g CVC pseudowords.<br />

Interest<strong>in</strong>gly, this is the one student who was<br />

us<strong>in</strong>g an AAC device. He was also the youngest<br />

participant <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>vestigation, which may<br />

have affected learn<strong>in</strong>g rate and/or amount.<br />

The two students who received <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong><br />

segment<strong>in</strong>g, manipulat<strong>in</strong>g, and encod<strong>in</strong>g<br />

CVC pseudowords met criterion and reta<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

these abilities dur<strong>in</strong>g ma<strong>in</strong>tenance probes.<br />

In Fallon et al.’s (2004) <strong>in</strong>vestigation, <strong>in</strong>struction<br />

<strong>in</strong> phonemic awareness and phonics<br />

was implemented us<strong>in</strong>g 3-<strong>in</strong>. 3-<strong>in</strong>. lam<strong>in</strong>ated<br />

picture cards that were created us<strong>in</strong>g<br />

Boardmaker ® Software version 3.5. These <strong>in</strong>structional<br />

tools were successfully used to<br />

teach s<strong>in</strong>gle-word read<strong>in</strong>g skills to five children,<br />

ages 9.5 to 14.0 years, <strong>with</strong> severe speech<br />

impairments. With<strong>in</strong> this program, phonemic<br />

awareness and phonics <strong>in</strong>structional lessons<br />

were taught us<strong>in</strong>g multiple <strong>in</strong>structional strategies<br />

that <strong>in</strong>cluded scaffold<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>structional<br />

tasks to promote errorless learn<strong>in</strong>g and direct<br />

<strong>in</strong>struction (Ellis, Worth<strong>in</strong>gton, & Lark<strong>in</strong>,<br />

1994). Results of Fallon et al.’s <strong>in</strong>vestigation<br />

<strong>in</strong>dicated improvements <strong>in</strong> read<strong>in</strong>g target<br />

words, novel words, and generalization to<br />

book contexts. Although pictures were paired<br />

<strong>with</strong> consonant-vowel-consonant (CVC) words<br />

as a stimulus for read<strong>in</strong>g tasks <strong>in</strong> the Fallon et<br />

al. <strong>in</strong>vestigation, contextualized <strong>in</strong>struction<br />

us<strong>in</strong>g books and/or stories was not a feature<br />

of <strong>in</strong>struction. Two recommendations for<br />

modifications for future <strong>in</strong>vestigations made<br />

by the researchers <strong>in</strong>cluded “<strong>in</strong>creased phonological<br />

awareness <strong>in</strong>structional time” and<br />

“the addition of written words dur<strong>in</strong>g phonological<br />

awareness activities” (p. 1436).<br />

Johnston, Buchanan, and Davenport (2009)<br />

<strong>in</strong>vestigated the rate of letter-sound acquisition<br />

<strong>with</strong> two preschoolers <strong>with</strong> autism spectrum<br />

disorder (ASD). An alternat<strong>in</strong>g treatments<br />

design was used and the target letters<br />

were presented <strong>in</strong> two conditions; (a) gradual<br />

array and (b) fixed array. In the gradual array<br />

condition the target letter was presented <strong>in</strong><br />

isolation and seven distracter letters were<br />

gradually added. In the fixed array condition<br />

seven letter distracters were presented <strong>with</strong><br />

the target letter. Results were that acquisition<br />

of letter-sound correspondence was faster<br />

when the letter was presented <strong>in</strong> the fixed<br />

array condition for both participants. The authors<br />

contend that the fixed array condition<br />

was more efficient for letter-sound acquisition<br />

and call for more research on effective strategies<br />

that teach literacy skills to children <strong>with</strong><br />

CCN.<br />

Recently, Strasser and Seplocha (2007) described<br />

the importance of us<strong>in</strong>g picture books<br />

to facilitate literacy <strong>in</strong>struction <strong>in</strong> young children.<br />

Picture books plus pr<strong>in</strong>ted words have<br />

been reported to be effective <strong>in</strong> help<strong>in</strong>g children<br />

ga<strong>in</strong> general knowledge, practice cognitive<br />

th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g, and learn about the rhythms<br />

and conventions of written words (Neuman,<br />

1999). Picture books also expose children to a<br />

rich vocabulary (Strasser & Seplocha). Use of<br />

picture books may provide a contextualized<br />

basis for phonological awareness <strong>in</strong>struction<br />

that facilitates phonemic awareness, awareness<br />

of alphabetic pr<strong>in</strong>ciple, phonics, and<br />

even recognition of sight words. Use of picture<br />

book-based read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terventions may be<br />

similarly important for children <strong>with</strong> CCN.<br />

There has been a call <strong>in</strong> the literature to<br />

further <strong>in</strong>vestigate literacy acquisition <strong>in</strong> students<br />

<strong>with</strong> CCN who use AAC (Dahlgren<br />

Sandberg, 2001; Hetzroni, 2004; Johnston,<br />

Buchanan, & Davenport, 2009; Sturm & Clendon,<br />

2004; Sturm, Erickson, & Yoder, 2002).<br />

Unfortunately, few published reports have<br />

measured the efficacy of <strong>in</strong>structional programs<br />

<strong>in</strong> preliteracy and literacy skills of these<br />

students. This <strong>in</strong>formation is needed to help<br />

professionals determ<strong>in</strong>e the most effective<br />

and efficient <strong>in</strong>structional programs <strong>in</strong> literacy<br />

for students <strong>with</strong> CCN who use AAC.<br />

The purpose of this <strong>in</strong>vestigation was to<br />

determ<strong>in</strong>e the effects of a structured <strong>in</strong>tervention<br />

package on junior high school students’<br />

sound-to-letter match<strong>in</strong>g skills and decod<strong>in</strong>g<br />

of novel words. The structured <strong>in</strong>tervention<br />

package <strong>in</strong>cluded direct, scaffolded, <strong>in</strong>structional<br />

lessons that were implemented us<strong>in</strong>g an<br />

error correction learn<strong>in</strong>g system. Additionally,<br />

a picture book-based phonological and phonemic<br />

awareness component was added to<br />

each <strong>in</strong>structional lesson. The primary goal of<br />

the <strong>in</strong>vestigation was to determ<strong>in</strong>e the effects<br />

of this <strong>in</strong>tervention package on the preliteracy<br />

and literacy skills of students <strong>with</strong> CCN who<br />

use AAC.<br />

354 / Education and Tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Autism and Developmental Disabilities-September 2011


Method<br />

Participants<br />

Four youths (2 boys and 2 girls) rang<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> age<br />

from 12 years, 5 months to 15 years, 11<br />

months participated <strong>in</strong> this <strong>in</strong>vestigation. Participants<br />

attended junior high school <strong>in</strong> an<br />

urban sett<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the Midwest. All participants<br />

were educated <strong>in</strong> two self-conta<strong>in</strong>ed special<br />

education classrooms <strong>with</strong> an emphasis on improv<strong>in</strong>g<br />

and maximiz<strong>in</strong>g functional communication<br />

and life skills. In this sett<strong>in</strong>g, a speechlanguage<br />

pathologist <strong>with</strong> more than 20 years<br />

of experience and advanced tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> AAC<br />

partnered <strong>with</strong> each highly experienced special<br />

education teacher for co-teach<strong>in</strong>g and collaboration<br />

for approximately 40% of each<br />

school day. Three of the four students participat<strong>in</strong>g<br />

<strong>in</strong> the project had been identified <strong>with</strong><br />

ASD and all had been identified <strong>with</strong> moderate<br />

cognitive disabilities and CCN. Additionally,<br />

all used a variety of AAC systems rang<strong>in</strong>g<br />

from low- to high-tech. Low-tech AAC systems<br />

<strong>in</strong> place <strong>in</strong>cluded use of visual strategies and<br />

schedules, communication books, and s<strong>in</strong>gleor<br />

multi-level switches. High-tech AAC systems<br />

used by several participants <strong>in</strong>cluded electronic<br />

devices <strong>with</strong> dynamic displays and<br />

speech output.<br />

Lucy was a 15-year old female <strong>with</strong> primary<br />

diagnoses of Down syndrome, moderate cognitive<br />

impairment, and CCN. Although Lucy<br />

was highly verbal, her utterances were largely<br />

un<strong>in</strong>telligible to unfamiliar communication<br />

partners. <strong>Communication</strong> partners familiar<br />

<strong>with</strong> Lucy also found her verbal language<br />

highly un<strong>in</strong>telligible, even <strong>with</strong> the use of contextual<br />

cues such as body language and gestures.<br />

When the comb<strong>in</strong>ation of words plus<br />

gestures was not successful <strong>in</strong> gett<strong>in</strong>g her message<br />

across, Lucy <strong>in</strong>dependently accessed her<br />

high- tech communication device. With it, she<br />

was able to successfully convey her <strong>in</strong>tended<br />

messages <strong>in</strong> approximately 90% of her attempts.<br />

Accord<strong>in</strong>g to Lucy’s teachers, Lucy<br />

was unable to decode new words, although<br />

she had sight recognition of approximately<br />

100 words and understood their general<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> context. Lucy’s IEP goals related<br />

to language and/or literacy <strong>in</strong>cluded (a)<br />

match<strong>in</strong>g a sentence to a photo that depicted<br />

the sentence given a choice of two or three<br />

sentences; (b) production of consonant<br />

blends; (c) correct use of new vocabulary<br />

words from categories <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g nouns, verbs,<br />

and attributes <strong>in</strong> structured tasks; and (d)<br />

identification of 12 to 15 survival words us<strong>in</strong>g<br />

her AAC device.<br />

Randy was a 15-year old male <strong>with</strong> primary<br />

diagnoses of ASD, moderate cognitive impairment,<br />

and CCN. Randy’s verbalizations were<br />

typically one word <strong>in</strong> length and <strong>in</strong>telligibility<br />

was estimated by his teachers at 80% <strong>in</strong> typical<br />

<strong>in</strong>teractions. However, his spontaneous verbalizations<br />

were largely off-topic and often repetitive.<br />

When not understood, Randy tended<br />

to grow frustrated, and showed his frustration<br />

<strong>with</strong> maladaptive behaviors, such as yell<strong>in</strong>g or<br />

mov<strong>in</strong>g out of his seat. When prompted by a<br />

staff member, Randy was able to successfully<br />

use a low-tech communication system, such as<br />

pre-pr<strong>in</strong>ted communication pages <strong>with</strong> familiar<br />

icons. Randy was able to recognize most of<br />

the words paired <strong>with</strong> icons on his communication<br />

board. Accord<strong>in</strong>g to his teachers,<br />

Randy did not have a good understand<strong>in</strong>g of<br />

the mean<strong>in</strong>g of many of the words that he<br />

could accurately recognize by sight. He was<br />

consistently unable to decode new words. Randy’s<br />

IEP goals related to language and/or literacy<br />

<strong>in</strong>cluded (a) us<strong>in</strong>g vocabulary words accurately<br />

<strong>in</strong> noun, verb, and attribute<br />

categories; (b) <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g utterances to two<br />

words <strong>in</strong> length; (c) match<strong>in</strong>g a written sentence<br />

to a picture; and (d) us<strong>in</strong>g s<strong>in</strong>gle-word<br />

responses to correctly answer ‘wh’ questions<br />

after a teacher read aloud a paragraph.<br />

Amy was a 12-year old female <strong>with</strong> primary<br />

diagnoses of ASD, moderate cognitive impairment,<br />

a classification of other health impairment<br />

due to gastro<strong>in</strong>test<strong>in</strong>al health issues, and<br />

CCN. Amy was estimated at 100% <strong>in</strong>telligible<br />

<strong>in</strong> connected speech; however, <strong>in</strong> approximately<br />

90% of <strong>in</strong>teractions, her utterances<br />

were off-topic. When not understood, she often<br />

used maladaptive behaviors such as yell<strong>in</strong>g<br />

or hitt<strong>in</strong>g. When prompted, Amy was able to<br />

use a low-tech communication system <strong>with</strong> a<br />

small number of symbol-plus-word choices. Although<br />

not confirmed, Amy’s teachers suspected<br />

that she also had some undiagnosed<br />

visual discrim<strong>in</strong>ation deficits, as she had consistently<br />

exhibited difficulty accurately discern<strong>in</strong>g<br />

between symbols. Accord<strong>in</strong>g to her<br />

teachers, Amy was consistently unable to accu-<br />

<strong>Improv<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>Literacy</strong> <strong>Skills</strong> / 355


ately read commonly used s<strong>in</strong>gle words when<br />

not paired <strong>with</strong> pictures or symbols, nor could<br />

she accurately decode unfamiliar words.<br />

Amy’s IEP goals related to language and/or<br />

literacy <strong>in</strong>cluded (a) compos<strong>in</strong>g a 3–4 word<br />

sentence us<strong>in</strong>g vocabulary associated <strong>with</strong> a<br />

picture provided; (b) accurately us<strong>in</strong>g new<br />

vocabulary (nouns, verbs, and attributes) to<br />

describe a picture; (c) follow<strong>in</strong>g a two-step<br />

direction to complete school-related tasks;<br />

and (d) identification of 12–15 written survival<br />

words.<br />

Matthew was a 13-year old male <strong>with</strong> primary<br />

diagnoses of ASD, moderate cognitive impairment,<br />

a sensor<strong>in</strong>eural hear<strong>in</strong>g loss, and CCN.<br />

Matthew’s preferred method of communication<br />

was use of gestures comb<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>with</strong> some<br />

manual signs. When not understood, he was<br />

often able to write, us<strong>in</strong>g “<strong>in</strong>vented” spell<strong>in</strong>g<br />

to help his communication partner understand<br />

him. The accuracy of his <strong>in</strong>vented spell<strong>in</strong>g<br />

was consistently poor, accord<strong>in</strong>g to his<br />

teachers. When these methods were unsuccessful,<br />

Matthew would often <strong>in</strong>dependently<br />

attempt to get his message across <strong>with</strong> his<br />

high-tech communication device. This was<br />

moderately successful, depend<strong>in</strong>g on the content<br />

of his <strong>in</strong>tended message. Accord<strong>in</strong>g to his<br />

teachers, Matthew was able to recognize words<br />

commonly paired <strong>with</strong> icons <strong>in</strong> his classroom.<br />

His teachers reported his decod<strong>in</strong>g skills as<br />

“poor” for novel words. Matthew’s IEP goals<br />

related to language and/or literacy <strong>in</strong>cluded<br />

(a) answer<strong>in</strong>g a ‘wh’ question accurately after<br />

a teacher read a story or paragraph; (b) <strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g<br />

appropriate vocabulary use for verbs,<br />

nouns, and attributes; (c) produc<strong>in</strong>g a 3–4<br />

word utterance on his AAC device us<strong>in</strong>g vocabulary<br />

associated <strong>with</strong> a photo; and (d)<br />

match<strong>in</strong>g a word to a photo given a choice of<br />

two words. See Table 1 for a summary of participant<br />

demographic <strong>in</strong>formation.<br />

Materials<br />

A phoneme-loaded picture book was made for<br />

each of the 18 targeted phonemes. For example,<br />

the phoneme-loaded /m/ picture book<br />

was entitled “Matt’s Monday Morn<strong>in</strong>g Swim.”<br />

All 18 phoneme-loaded books were produced<br />

us<strong>in</strong>g Boardmaker ® symbols and all were written<br />

to meet the follow<strong>in</strong>g criteria: (a) the story<br />

<strong>in</strong>cluded 48-po<strong>in</strong>t Arial font pr<strong>in</strong>t plus associ-<br />

ated Boardmaker ® symbols; (b) the target<br />

phoneme was used a m<strong>in</strong>imum of 15 times at<br />

the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>gs or ends of words; (c) book<br />

topics were chosen to be age-appropriate, but<br />

storyl<strong>in</strong>es were simple; (d) books conta<strong>in</strong>ed as<br />

many concrete, picturable words as possible;<br />

(e) words were taken directly from <strong>in</strong>structional<br />

lessons when possible; (f) use of consonant<br />

blends was m<strong>in</strong>imized around target<br />

phonemes (i.e., Bob <strong>in</strong>stead of Brad); (g) target<br />

phonemes were underl<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>with</strong><strong>in</strong> words;<br />

and (h) each target word was represented <strong>with</strong><br />

the same picture symbol that was used consistently<br />

throughout all books and dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>tervention<br />

activities. A data record<strong>in</strong>g form was<br />

developed for the purpose of collect<strong>in</strong>g procedural<br />

reliability data for each of these criteria<br />

(see Table 2). Dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dividual assessments<br />

for sound-letter match<strong>in</strong>g and word<br />

decod<strong>in</strong>g, 2-<strong>in</strong>. by 4-<strong>in</strong>. cards <strong>with</strong> the targeted<br />

phonemes or words <strong>in</strong> 48-po<strong>in</strong>t Arial font<br />

pr<strong>in</strong>t were made us<strong>in</strong>g Boardmaker ® and then<br />

lam<strong>in</strong>ated.<br />

Intervention<br />

The structured literacy <strong>in</strong>tervention had two<br />

dist<strong>in</strong>ct components. The first component <strong>in</strong>volved<br />

an <strong>in</strong>teractive read<strong>in</strong>g experience <strong>with</strong><br />

a phoneme-loaded book to all participants <strong>in</strong><br />

the <strong>in</strong>tervention group. The next component<br />

consisted of <strong>in</strong>dividual scaffolded phoneme<br />

lessons. Intervention occurred <strong>in</strong> the same<br />

two classrooms and the students were paired<br />

<strong>with</strong> graduate speech-language cl<strong>in</strong>icians dur<strong>in</strong>g<br />

all evaluation and <strong>in</strong>tervention sessions.<br />

The first <strong>in</strong>tervention component, read<strong>in</strong>g a<br />

phoneme-loaded book <strong>with</strong> all participants,<br />

took about 5 m<strong>in</strong>utes. A graduate student cl<strong>in</strong>ician<br />

<strong>in</strong> each classroom read the phoneme<br />

loaded book to the group of participants and<br />

their paired graduate student cl<strong>in</strong>icians and<br />

paused after each page. Dur<strong>in</strong>g the pause, the<br />

paired graduate student cl<strong>in</strong>icians provided<br />

emphasis of targeted phonemes. For example,<br />

when read<strong>in</strong>g the /m/ phoneme-loaded<br />

book, the cl<strong>in</strong>ician would say, “Look — here is<br />

the letter [shows m] that makes the /m. . ./<br />

sound.” In addition, the cl<strong>in</strong>ician po<strong>in</strong>ted to<br />

the words as they were read for the purpose of<br />

<strong>in</strong>creas<strong>in</strong>g the participants’ pr<strong>in</strong>t awareness<br />

skills, such as left-to-right read<strong>in</strong>g and stop-<br />

356 / Education and Tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Autism and Developmental Disabilities-September 2011


TABLE 1<br />

Participant Demographic Information<br />

Participant Name Lucy Randy Amy Matthew<br />

Diagnoses Down syndrome<br />

Moderate cognitive<br />

impairment<br />

CNN<br />

<strong>Communication</strong> Status<br />

(as reported by<br />

school speechlanguage<br />

pathologist)<br />

Estimated Pre-literacy/<br />

<strong>Literacy</strong> <strong>Skills</strong><br />

Estimated at 20%<br />

un<strong>in</strong>telligible <strong>in</strong><br />

verbal exchanges<br />

Consistent use of<br />

body language<br />

and gestures<br />

<strong>with</strong> verbal<br />

messages<br />

High-tech<br />

communicative<br />

device used <strong>in</strong><br />

approximately<br />

80% of<br />

spontaneous<br />

exchanges<br />

Unable to decode<br />

new words<br />

Recognized<br />

approximately<br />

100 sight words<br />

<strong>in</strong> context<br />

p<strong>in</strong>g at the ends of sentences to emphasize<br />

punctuation.<br />

Next, prior to <strong>in</strong>itiat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>dividualized <strong>in</strong>struction<br />

for the day’s targeted lesson, graduate<br />

student cl<strong>in</strong>icians completed brief <strong>in</strong>dividual<br />

sound-to-letter assessments which took<br />

approximately 3–5 m<strong>in</strong>utes to complete. At<br />

the end of every 3 weeks, graduate student<br />

cl<strong>in</strong>icians also completed brief s<strong>in</strong>gle-word<br />

probe assessments dur<strong>in</strong>g this time. All brief<br />

assessments immediately followed the read<strong>in</strong>g<br />

of the phoneme-loaded target book and were<br />

completed <strong>in</strong> the two classrooms by the graduate<br />

student cl<strong>in</strong>icians. All words used <strong>in</strong> word<br />

ASD<br />

Moderate cognitive<br />

impairment<br />

CNN<br />

Estimated at 80%<br />

un<strong>in</strong>telligible <strong>in</strong><br />

verbal exchanges,<br />

however, 60% of<br />

speech off-topic<br />

and repetitive<br />

Consistent,<br />

spontaneous use<br />

of one word<br />

utterances<br />

When use<br />

prompted, lowtech<br />

AAC<br />

facilitated 3–4<br />

word utterances<br />

and on-topic<br />

essages<br />

Unable to decode<br />

new words<br />

Poor understand<strong>in</strong>g<br />

of word<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>gs,<br />

although has<br />

sight vocabulary<br />

of 50 words<br />

ASD<br />

Moderate cognitive<br />

impairment<br />

OHI (gastro<strong>in</strong>test<strong>in</strong>al<br />

issues)<br />

CNN<br />

Estimated at 100%<br />

<strong>in</strong>telligible,<br />

however, 90% of<br />

speech off-topic<br />

Low-tech<br />

communicative<br />

device used for<br />

topic ma<strong>in</strong>tenance<br />

when cued<br />

Unable to decode<br />

new words<br />

Poor semantic<br />

understand<strong>in</strong>g<br />

(word mean<strong>in</strong>gs).<br />

No reported sight<br />

vocabulary<br />

ASD<br />

Moderate cognitive<br />

impairment<br />

Sensor<strong>in</strong>eural<br />

hear<strong>in</strong>g loss,<br />

<strong>in</strong>consistent use<br />

of hear<strong>in</strong>g aid<br />

CNN<br />

Estimated at 100%<br />

un<strong>in</strong>telligible <strong>in</strong><br />

verbal attempts<br />

Consistent use of<br />

gestures and<br />

some true sign<br />

language forms<br />

Consistent use of<br />

<strong>in</strong>vented spell<strong>in</strong>g<br />

to aid<br />

communication<br />

breakdowns<br />

us<strong>in</strong>g high-tech<br />

communication<br />

device<br />

Unable to decode<br />

new words<br />

Recognizes<br />

approximately<br />

50 words when<br />

paired <strong>with</strong><br />

picture icons.<br />

No reported<br />

sight vocabulary<br />

probe assessments were two to four letters <strong>in</strong><br />

length, but all conta<strong>in</strong>ed two to three sounds<br />

<strong>in</strong> each word (e.g., putt, egg, and up).<br />

The second component of the <strong>in</strong>tervention<br />

was <strong>in</strong>dividual phoneme scaffolded lessons<br />

and these lessons took about 20 m<strong>in</strong>utes per<br />

<strong>in</strong>tervention session. A total of 10 lessons were<br />

developed for this project; these were structured<br />

and scaffolded to target specific phonemes<br />

and skills, beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g <strong>with</strong> preliteracy<br />

skills and end<strong>in</strong>g <strong>with</strong> word decod<strong>in</strong>g. The 18<br />

target phonemes were divided <strong>in</strong>to one of<br />

three sound-letter sets and each sound-letter<br />

set was targeted for 3 weeks of <strong>in</strong>struction.<br />

<strong>Improv<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>Literacy</strong> <strong>Skills</strong> / 357


TABLE 2<br />

Procedural Reliability Data Sheet for Phonological/Phonemic Awareness Picture Book Activity<br />

Book Reader: __________ Reliability Checker: __________ Reliability % __________ Date: __________<br />

Record<strong>in</strong>g Code: if cl<strong>in</strong>ician performs planned behavior or book conta<strong>in</strong>s target component/feature;<br />

leave blank if cl<strong>in</strong>ician does not perform planned behavior or book doesn’t <strong>in</strong>clude<br />

target component/feature<br />

___Cl<strong>in</strong>ician preteaches sound-letter<br />

i.e., this letter says /b/ (show b). You can hear it <strong>in</strong> the word Bob, /b, b/ Bob. I also hear it <strong>in</strong><br />

this word “bat, /b, b bat”. Let’s read a story about Bob. Listen for the /b/ sound while we read.<br />

We will po<strong>in</strong>t to the letter that makes the /b/ sound while we read. Or. ...<br />

I know a story <strong>with</strong> a character called Mary Mouse who drank lots of milk. Who drank the milk? The<br />

words ‘Mary, Mouse, and milk all beg<strong>in</strong> <strong>with</strong> the same sound: the /m/ sound. Watch my mouth<br />

/mmmm/. Do you hear that /mmmm/ sound <strong>in</strong> the word Mary?’<br />

___ Story <strong>in</strong>cludes pr<strong>in</strong>t plus simple picture<br />

___ Target phoneme is used a m<strong>in</strong>imum of 15 times, at the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g or end of words<br />

___ Book <strong>in</strong>cludes 10 or fewer pages<br />

___ Book <strong>in</strong>cludes age-appropriate topic, but simple storyl<strong>in</strong>e<br />

___ Book <strong>in</strong>cludes concrete (picturable) words as much as possible<br />

Use word lists when possible<br />

___ Book <strong>in</strong>cludes few blends (i.e., Bob <strong>in</strong>stead of Brad)<br />

___ Target phoneme is underl<strong>in</strong>ed <strong>in</strong> each of the words that conta<strong>in</strong>s it<br />

___ Target words have associated pictures<br />

# CLINICIAN BEHAVIORS/BOOK COMPONENT/FEATURES POSSIBLE: 9<br />

# CLINICIAN BEHAVIORS PERFORMED OR BOOK COMPONENT/FEATURES PRESENT: ___<br />

PROCEDURAL RELIABILITY %: ____ (# criteria/behavior accurate <strong>in</strong> X of 9 X 100)<br />

COMMENTS:________________________________<br />

The first sound-letter set consisted of phonemes<br />

/m, f, a, t, p, o/; the second soundletter<br />

set consisted of phonemes /b, g, i, s, l,<br />

e/; and the third sound-letter set consisted of<br />

phonemes /d, n, u, r, w, c/.<br />

The 10 <strong>in</strong>dividual phoneme lessons were<br />

scaffolded by difficulty level, beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g <strong>with</strong> a<br />

sound-to-letter match<strong>in</strong>g identification task<br />

and end<strong>in</strong>g <strong>with</strong> novel word decod<strong>in</strong>g skills.<br />

Each lesson <strong>in</strong>cluded the follow<strong>in</strong>g steps.<br />

1. Sound-to-letter match<strong>in</strong>g identification<br />

(i.e., Which letter says m-m-m-?)<br />

2. Sound/letter identification, beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g of<br />

a word (i.e., Which letter says m-m-m- <strong>in</strong><br />

this word: “mmm-ap”?)<br />

3. Sound/letter identification, end of a word<br />

(same cue as #2 but <strong>with</strong> word end<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong><br />

target sound such as “ha-mmm.”)<br />

4. Sound/letter identification <strong>in</strong> beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

and ends of words <strong>with</strong><strong>in</strong> sentences (same<br />

cue as #2 <strong>with</strong> sentences).<br />

5. Correspond<strong>in</strong>g letter/sound production<br />

us<strong>in</strong>g voice or AAC (i.e., What sound does<br />

this letter make?)<br />

6. Identification of words that start <strong>with</strong> target<br />

sounds <strong>in</strong> picture choices (Which picture<br />

word starts <strong>with</strong> the letter that makes<br />

the sound m-m-m-?)<br />

7. Identification of target letter at the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g<br />

of a word produced by cl<strong>in</strong>ician (i.e.,<br />

Which letter makes the sound at the beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g<br />

of the word m-m-m-at?)<br />

8. Identification of target letter at the end of<br />

a word produced by cl<strong>in</strong>ician (i.e., Which<br />

letter makes the sound at the end of the<br />

word sa-m-m-m?)<br />

9. Identify telescoped words (i.e., Which picture<br />

shows m-m-m-a-a-a-t?) produced by<br />

cl<strong>in</strong>ician by po<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g to a picture paired<br />

<strong>with</strong> the word that represents it.<br />

10. Identify (decode) a word produced normally<br />

by a cl<strong>in</strong>ician by po<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g to a word<br />

that represents it (Which word says mat?).<br />

The graduate student cl<strong>in</strong>ician chose the<br />

358 / Education and Tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Autism and Developmental Disabilities-September 2011


step <strong>with</strong> which to beg<strong>in</strong> the lesson based on<br />

the student’s <strong>in</strong>dividual performance data<br />

from the previous session. The graduate student<br />

cl<strong>in</strong>ician began each lesson by present<strong>in</strong>g<br />

the participant <strong>with</strong> a fixed array of two of<br />

the six target phonemes from the current<br />

sound-letter set. As the participant demonstrated<br />

success <strong>with</strong> the fixed array, s/he was<br />

given an additional letter/sound choice until<br />

there was a possible choice of six target phonemes.<br />

For example, <strong>in</strong> the first lesson, a participant<br />

would be explicitly taught to recognize<br />

the target letter ‘m’ and associate it <strong>with</strong><br />

the /m/ sound. The student was then provided<br />

opportunities to show comprehension<br />

by choos<strong>in</strong>g the target phoneme from a<br />

choice of two letters. Once the student chose<br />

the correct letter <strong>in</strong> 100% of trials, an additional<br />

letter choice was added until the participant<br />

could correctly identify the target letter<br />

<strong>in</strong> 100% of opportunities <strong>with</strong> a choice of all<br />

six letters <strong>in</strong> the set. These lessons were loosely<br />

modeled after the Fallon et al. (2004) description<br />

of preliteracy-to-literacy <strong>in</strong>tervention and<br />

modified per teacher suggestion for specific<br />

sound targets across students. That is, the<br />

classroom teachers determ<strong>in</strong>ed f<strong>in</strong>al letter/<br />

sound targets for students <strong>in</strong> the project<br />

prior to lesson development. The lessons<br />

were delivered <strong>in</strong> a systematic fashion for 9<br />

weeks.<br />

An error correction strategy was <strong>in</strong>cluded <strong>in</strong><br />

the <strong>in</strong>structional program. For example, if a<br />

student chose an <strong>in</strong>correct letter after hear<strong>in</strong>g<br />

the phoneme cue, the cl<strong>in</strong>ician told the participant<br />

that the choice was <strong>in</strong>correct, held up<br />

the correct letter, and said, “This is the letter<br />

that makes the sound /m-m-m-/.” The cl<strong>in</strong>ician<br />

then immediately provided the stimulus<br />

question aga<strong>in</strong> (i.e., “Which letter makes the<br />

sound /m-m-m-/?”) and helped the participant<br />

choose the correct response by look<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

po<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g, and/or physical assistance as<br />

needed. If the participant then chose the correct<br />

response or <strong>in</strong>itially chose the correct<br />

response <strong>in</strong>dependently, the cl<strong>in</strong>ician positively<br />

re<strong>in</strong>forced the correct choice (i.e., “Yes,<br />

this is the letter that makes the sound /m-mm-/.<br />

Great work!”). Some of the participants<br />

were <strong>in</strong>different to verbal praise. In this classroom,<br />

a system of food re<strong>in</strong>forcers was used to<br />

re<strong>in</strong>force desired behaviors, so <strong>with</strong> some participants<br />

(Randy and Amy), a food re<strong>in</strong>forcer,<br />

such as an apple slice or a piece of cracker,<br />

was paired <strong>with</strong> verbal praise. All re<strong>in</strong>forcers<br />

were given for <strong>in</strong>dependent correct choices<br />

only, not for correct responses that resulted<br />

from the error correction system.<br />

Assessment<br />

The week prior to beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>tervention was<br />

dedicated to pretest<strong>in</strong>g sound-to-letter match<strong>in</strong>g<br />

skills and conduct<strong>in</strong>g an assessment of<br />

word decod<strong>in</strong>g skills for all students. An additional<br />

week was dedicated to posttest<strong>in</strong>g us<strong>in</strong>g<br />

the same assessments follow<strong>in</strong>g the 9-week <strong>in</strong>tervention<br />

period. A total of three different<br />

sets of six letters/sounds were targeted us<strong>in</strong>g<br />

these structured lessons every 3 weeks, so that<br />

by the end of the project, students had received<br />

<strong>in</strong>struction on a total of 18 sound/<br />

letter targets (see Table 3 for sample lessons).<br />

Additional posttests us<strong>in</strong>g the same assessments<br />

were conducted 5 months after the<br />

completion of the project to assess ma<strong>in</strong>tenance<br />

and generalization of ga<strong>in</strong>s achieved<br />

dur<strong>in</strong>g the project.<br />

Research Design<br />

A s<strong>in</strong>gle-subject multiple basel<strong>in</strong>e design<br />

(Baer, Wolf, & Risley, 1968) replicated across<br />

four students was employed to evaluate the<br />

effectiveness of a structured <strong>in</strong>tervention<br />

package. The goal of the <strong>in</strong>vestigation was to<br />

determ<strong>in</strong>e the effects of the <strong>in</strong>structional<br />

package on two dependent variables: (a)<br />

sound-to-letter match<strong>in</strong>g skills and (b) s<strong>in</strong>gleword<br />

decod<strong>in</strong>g tasks <strong>in</strong>volv<strong>in</strong>g novel words.<br />

The project <strong>in</strong>cluded basel<strong>in</strong>e, <strong>in</strong>tervention,<br />

and ma<strong>in</strong>tenance conditions <strong>with</strong> data collection<br />

on sound-to-letter match<strong>in</strong>g skills occurr<strong>in</strong>g<br />

dur<strong>in</strong>g each session.<br />

Data collection on s<strong>in</strong>gle-word decod<strong>in</strong>g<br />

tasks occurred every 3 rd week, for a total of<br />

three probes. These s<strong>in</strong>gle-word assessments<br />

consisted of an identification/decod<strong>in</strong>g task<br />

of 18 target words chosen from a total pool of<br />

54 words not used dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>tervention activities<br />

(i.e., ‘novel’ words). Each of the 18 words<br />

began <strong>with</strong> a different one of the 18 letters<br />

targeted <strong>in</strong> the <strong>in</strong>tervention. Three novel<br />

words beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g <strong>with</strong> the same letter were<br />

presented to each participant. The <strong>in</strong>terventionists’<br />

cue was “Which word says [target<br />

<strong>Improv<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>Literacy</strong> <strong>Skills</strong> / 359


TABLE 3<br />

Sample Lessons 1 and 5<br />

Lesson Activities:<br />

1. sound-letter match<strong>in</strong>g task<br />

a. Cl<strong>in</strong>ician makes the phoneme sound while show<strong>in</strong>g the letter (i.e., This is the letter that says<br />

/mmmm/. You can hear it <strong>in</strong> the word ‘money’ “m, m, m money”. Did you hear the /mmmm/<br />

sound? This letter says ‘/mmm/. Next, give the student a choice of 2 letters to pick from, 10<br />

trials. Beg<strong>in</strong> <strong>with</strong> /m, f/; change their position on the table each time. **Use error correction<br />

strategy <strong>with</strong> each <strong>in</strong>correct choice. If participant misses, cl<strong>in</strong>ician <strong>in</strong>structs while po<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g to the<br />

correct choice. (i.e., That’s not the letter that says /m-m-m/. Here is the letter that says ‘m-m-m’).<br />

Then, start on the next trial. Cont<strong>in</strong>ue correct<strong>in</strong>g until the student chooses correctly. Use positive<br />

re<strong>in</strong>forcement when correct. (i.e., That’s right! That’s the letter that says m-m-m).<br />

When accurate at 100% 10 trials (10 times <strong>in</strong> a row <strong>with</strong> or <strong>with</strong>out error correction strategy) move to next<br />

letter below:<br />

b. Increase to 3 letters /m, f, a/ document<strong>in</strong>g % accuracy<br />

c. Increase to 4 letters /m, f, a, t/, document<strong>in</strong>g accuracy<br />

d. Increase to 5 letters /m, f, a, t, p/ document<strong>in</strong>g accuracy<br />

e. Increase to 6 letters /m, f, a, t, p, o/ document<strong>in</strong>g accuracy<br />

When student can correctly identify the target letter (i.e., /m/) from a choice of 6, start back at step a.,<br />

beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g <strong>with</strong> the next target letter/phoneme (f)<br />

When the student can accurately complete all of the steps (a–e) <strong>with</strong> all target letters and phonemes,<br />

proceed to # 2.<br />

5. Given a letter, the student will produce the sound that it makes. The cue is: What sound does this<br />

letter make?<br />

If AAC device used: You can create overlays for the devices so that only the desired letter choices are<br />

show<strong>in</strong>g at each level.<br />

**Error correction strategy- If student misses, cl<strong>in</strong>ician repeats “that’s not the letter that says ”___”_;<br />

this is the letter that says “_____”, while po<strong>in</strong>t<strong>in</strong>g to or select<strong>in</strong>g the correct choice and produc<strong>in</strong>g<br />

the correct sound. Then, start aga<strong>in</strong>.<br />

a. /m, f/ 10 trials of each, can mix up presentation order.<br />

When accurate at 100% 10 trials of each letter <strong>with</strong> or <strong>with</strong>out error correction strategyb.<br />

3 letter choices /m, f, a/<br />

c. 4 letter choices /m, f, a, t/<br />

d. 5 letter choices /m, f, a, t, p/<br />

e. 6 letter choices /m, f, a, t, p, o/<br />

When student can correctly produce (verbally or <strong>with</strong> AAC system) the target letter from a choice of 6 to<br />

criterion, proceed to # 6.<br />

word]?” The participants were <strong>in</strong>structed to<br />

<strong>in</strong>dicate their choice from the array of three<br />

choices provided.<br />

Data on sound-to-letter match<strong>in</strong>g skills for<br />

all 18 sounds were collected prior to each<br />

lesson. Additionally, pretest<strong>in</strong>g and posttest<strong>in</strong>g<br />

sessions of sound-to-letter match<strong>in</strong>g skills<br />

and word decod<strong>in</strong>g tasks occurred dur<strong>in</strong>g 1<br />

week prior to and after the 9-week <strong>in</strong>tervention<br />

period. Sound-to-letter match<strong>in</strong>g assessments<br />

were conducted three times dur<strong>in</strong>g<br />

basel<strong>in</strong>e and posttest<strong>in</strong>g conditions, while the<br />

word decod<strong>in</strong>g skills assessment of all 54<br />

words (each assessed twice per assessment session<br />

for a total of 108 word trials) was conducted<br />

once dur<strong>in</strong>g the basel<strong>in</strong>e and posttest<strong>in</strong>g<br />

conditions.<br />

Graduate student cl<strong>in</strong>icians recorded <strong>in</strong>dividual<br />

accuracy data on all steps <strong>in</strong> the lessons.<br />

When a performance criterion of 100% accuracy<br />

was reached on each step of the lesson<br />

(<strong>with</strong> or <strong>with</strong>out the use of the error correction<br />

strategy), the graduate student cl<strong>in</strong>ician<br />

began <strong>in</strong>struction on the next step of the les-<br />

360 / Education and Tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Autism and Developmental Disabilities-September 2011


son. When all steps <strong>in</strong> the lesson were completed<br />

<strong>with</strong> 100% accuracy <strong>with</strong> or <strong>with</strong>out the<br />

error correction strategy, a new step of the<br />

lesson was <strong>in</strong>troduced.<br />

Results<br />

Individual Participant Results<br />

Lucy’s performance data. As shown <strong>in</strong> Figure<br />

1, Lucy made progress <strong>in</strong> accuracy of<br />

sound-to-letter match<strong>in</strong>g identification <strong>in</strong> all<br />

three of the targeted letter sets dur<strong>in</strong>g the<br />

project. In the first letter set, /m, f, a, t, p, o/,<br />

she improved from a mean 58% to 69% accuracy<br />

<strong>in</strong> the sound-to-letter match<strong>in</strong>g task, a<br />

ga<strong>in</strong> of 11 percentage po<strong>in</strong>ts from the basel<strong>in</strong>e<br />

condition through the <strong>in</strong>tervention condition.<br />

Dur<strong>in</strong>g the 9 weeks of the <strong>in</strong>tervention<br />

condition, each letter set was targeted for only<br />

3 weeks, so this represents considerable progress.<br />

Lucy made the least amount of progress<br />

<strong>in</strong> sound-to-letter match<strong>in</strong>g identification for<br />

the second letter set, /b, g, i, s, l, e/, improv<strong>in</strong>g<br />

from a mean 57% to 59% accuracy. She<br />

improved from a mean 41% accuracy <strong>in</strong><br />

sound-to-letter match<strong>in</strong>g dur<strong>in</strong>g the basel<strong>in</strong>e<br />

condition to a mean 45% accuracy for Letter<br />

Set 3, /d, n, u, r, w, c/, demonstrat<strong>in</strong>g an<br />

<strong>in</strong>crease of 4 percentage po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> the soundto-letter<br />

match<strong>in</strong>g task for Letter Set 3. Due to<br />

a family trip, Lucy missed 5 consecutive school<br />

days while Letter Set 2 was targeted. This may<br />

have impacted the amount of progress that<br />

she made on Letter Set 2. Sound-to-letter<br />

match<strong>in</strong>g skills were reassessed 5 months after<br />

completion of the <strong>in</strong>tervention to determ<strong>in</strong>e<br />

level of ma<strong>in</strong>tenance of skills. Lucy’s score on<br />

the assessment yielded the follow<strong>in</strong>g results:<br />

75% accuracy (Set 1 letters), 67% accuracy<br />

(Set 2 letters), and 42% accuracy (Set 3 letters).<br />

Accord<strong>in</strong>g to these results, progress cont<strong>in</strong>ued<br />

on Set 1 and 2 letters, while a decrease<br />

of only 3% accuracy was measured <strong>in</strong> the<br />

sound-to-letter match<strong>in</strong>g task associated <strong>with</strong><br />

Set 3 letters.<br />

Lucy’s pre-<strong>in</strong>tervention assessment of decod<strong>in</strong>g<br />

of all 54 novel words (assessed two<br />

times each) yielded accuracy levels for decod<strong>in</strong>g<br />

these novel words at 36% (Word Set 1),<br />

42% (Word Set 2), and 56% (Word Set 3).<br />

Intermittent word probes were also conducted<br />

to assess eight of these words, once every 3<br />

weeks. These three assessments yielded the<br />

follow<strong>in</strong>g accuracy levels: 16%, 16%, and 83%<br />

(Set 1 words), 33%, 42%, and 58% (Set 2<br />

words), and 25%, 42%, and 58% (Set 3<br />

words). Lucy’s accuracy <strong>in</strong>creased for all<br />

probe assessments over the course of the <strong>in</strong>tervention.<br />

Post<strong>in</strong>tervention assessments of all<br />

words yielded decod<strong>in</strong>g accuracy levels of 50%<br />

(Set 1 words), 42% (Set 2 words), and 42%<br />

(Set 3 words), <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>crease <strong>in</strong> this<br />

skill <strong>in</strong> two of the three word sets. All words<br />

were reassessed 5 months follow<strong>in</strong>g the completion<br />

of the <strong>in</strong>tervention. This time, Lucy<br />

scored 75% accuracy (Set 1), 67% accuracy<br />

(Set 2), and 42% accuracy (Set 3). These<br />

scores reflected an <strong>in</strong>crease of 39 percentage<br />

po<strong>in</strong>ts for Set 1 words and 25 percentage<br />

po<strong>in</strong>ts for Set 2 words <strong>in</strong> Lucy’s ability to<br />

decode the novel words. A decrease <strong>in</strong> accurate<br />

decod<strong>in</strong>g of 14 percentage po<strong>in</strong>ts was<br />

recorded for Set 3 words.<br />

Randy’s performance data. Randy made<br />

progress <strong>in</strong> accuracy of sound-to-letter match<strong>in</strong>g<br />

identification <strong>in</strong> all three of the targeted<br />

letter sets dur<strong>in</strong>g the project (see Figure 2). In<br />

the first letter set, /m, f, a, t, p, o/, he improved<br />

from a mean 27% to 44% accuracy <strong>in</strong><br />

the sound-to-letter match<strong>in</strong>g task, a ga<strong>in</strong> of 17<br />

percentage po<strong>in</strong>ts from the basel<strong>in</strong>e condition<br />

through the <strong>in</strong>tervention condition.<br />

Randy made the greatest progress <strong>in</strong> the second<br />

letter set, /b, g, i, s, l, e/, improv<strong>in</strong>g from<br />

a mean 26% to a mean 69%, a ga<strong>in</strong> of 43<br />

percentage po<strong>in</strong>ts from pre-to post<strong>in</strong>tervention.<br />

He also improved from a mean 25%<br />

accuracy <strong>in</strong> sound-to-letter match<strong>in</strong>g dur<strong>in</strong>g<br />

the basel<strong>in</strong>e condition to a mean 43% accuracy<br />

for Letter Set 3, /d, n, u, r, w, c/, demonstrat<strong>in</strong>g<br />

an <strong>in</strong>crease of 18 percentage po<strong>in</strong>ts<br />

<strong>in</strong> the sound-to-letter match<strong>in</strong>g task for Letter<br />

Set 3.<br />

Randy’s pre-<strong>in</strong>tervention assessment of decod<strong>in</strong>g<br />

of all 54 novel words yielded accuracy<br />

levels at 28% (Word Set 1), 56% (Word Set 2),<br />

and 53% (Word Set 3). Intermittent word<br />

probes were also used to assess eight of these<br />

words, once every 3 weeks. These three assessments<br />

yielded the follow<strong>in</strong>g accuracy levels:<br />

25%, 42%, and 83% (Set 1 words), 42%, 58%,<br />

and 92% (Set 2 words), and 25%, 67%, and<br />

50% (Set 3 words). Randy’s accuracy <strong>in</strong>creased<br />

for all probe assessments over the<br />

course of the <strong>in</strong>tervention. Post<strong>in</strong>tervention<br />

<strong>Improv<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>Literacy</strong> <strong>Skills</strong> / 361


assessments of all words yielded decod<strong>in</strong>g accuracy<br />

levels of 53% (Set 1 words), 56% (Set 2<br />

words), and 53% (Set 3 words), <strong>in</strong>dicat<strong>in</strong>g an<br />

<strong>in</strong>crease of 25 percentage po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> Set 1 letters,<br />

while the accuracy level was ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

for Set 2 and 3 letters. All words were reassessed<br />

5 months follow<strong>in</strong>g the completion of<br />

Figure 1. Graphic display of Lucy’s performance data.<br />

the <strong>in</strong>tervention. At the 5-month follow-up,<br />

Randy cont<strong>in</strong>ued to show improvement <strong>in</strong> decod<strong>in</strong>g<br />

skills, <strong>with</strong> an 83% accuracy (Word Set<br />

1), 83% accuracy (Word Set 2), and 75% accuracy<br />

(Word Set 3). These scores reflected an<br />

<strong>in</strong>crease of 11 percentage po<strong>in</strong>ts for Set 1<br />

words, 12 percentage po<strong>in</strong>ts for Set 2 words,<br />

362 / Education and Tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Autism and Developmental Disabilities-September 2011


and 11 percentage po<strong>in</strong>ts for Set 3 words <strong>in</strong><br />

Randy’s ability to decode these novel words.<br />

Amy’s performance data. Amy’s performance<br />

data are shown <strong>in</strong> Figure 3. She made<br />

progress <strong>in</strong> accuracy of sound-to-letter match<strong>in</strong>g<br />

identification <strong>in</strong> two of three targeted letter<br />

sets dur<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>tervention. In the first<br />

letter set, /m, f, a, t, p, o/, she improved from<br />

a mean 23% to 36% accuracy <strong>in</strong> the sound-toletter<br />

match<strong>in</strong>g task, a ga<strong>in</strong> of 13 percentage<br />

Figure 2. Graphic display of Randy’s performance data.<br />

po<strong>in</strong>ts from the basel<strong>in</strong>e condition through<br />

the <strong>in</strong>tervention condition. Amy also evidenced<br />

a ga<strong>in</strong> of 11 percentage po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> the<br />

sound-to-letter match<strong>in</strong>g task for Letter Set 2,<br />

/b, g, i, s, l, e/, mov<strong>in</strong>g from 23% accuracy to<br />

34% accuracy. A decrease of 1% was measured<br />

for the sound-to-letter match<strong>in</strong>g task for Letter<br />

Set 3, /d, n, u, r, w, c/, from 22% mean<br />

accuracy pre-<strong>in</strong>tervention to 21% accuracy<br />

dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>tervention sessions. Amy’s graduate<br />

<strong>Improv<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>Literacy</strong> <strong>Skills</strong> / 363


student <strong>in</strong>terventionist specifically noted<br />

Amy’s great difficulty <strong>in</strong> visually differentiat<strong>in</strong>g<br />

/n/ and /u/ and /i/ and /l/.<br />

Amy’s pre-<strong>in</strong>tervention assessment of decod<strong>in</strong>g<br />

of all 54 novel words yielded accuracy<br />

levels at 33% (Word Set 1), 33% (Word Set 2),<br />

and 39% (Word Set 3). Intermittent word<br />

probes were also used to assess 18 of these<br />

Figure 3. Graphic display of Amy’s performance data.<br />

words, once every 3 weeks. These three assessments<br />

yielded the follow<strong>in</strong>g accuracy levels:<br />

50%, 25%, and 50% (Set 1 words), 68%, 42%,<br />

and 68% (Set 2 words), and 42%, 25%, and<br />

50% (Set 3 words). With two exceptions dur<strong>in</strong>g<br />

probe evaluations, Amy’s accuracy <strong>in</strong>creased<br />

for all probe assessments over the<br />

course of the <strong>in</strong>tervention. Post-<strong>in</strong>tervention<br />

364 / Education and Tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Autism and Developmental Disabilities-September 2011


assessments of all words yielded decod<strong>in</strong>g accuracy<br />

levels at 53% (Set 1 words), an <strong>in</strong>crease<br />

of 20 percentage po<strong>in</strong>ts. An accuracy level of<br />

39% was measured for Set 2 words, reflect<strong>in</strong>g<br />

an <strong>in</strong>crease of 6 percentage po<strong>in</strong>ts. Accuracy<br />

was measured at 36% for Set 3 words, reflect<strong>in</strong>g<br />

a decrease of 3 percentage po<strong>in</strong>ts. All<br />

words were reassessed 5 months follow<strong>in</strong>g the<br />

completion of the <strong>in</strong>tervention. At the<br />

5-month follow-up probe, Amy’s novel word<br />

decod<strong>in</strong>g accuracy was measured at 33% (Set<br />

1 words), 42% (Set 2 words), and 25% (Set 3<br />

words). These scores reflected Amy’s ma<strong>in</strong>tenance<br />

of word decod<strong>in</strong>g skills for Set 1 words,<br />

an <strong>in</strong>crease of 6 percentage po<strong>in</strong>ts for Set 2<br />

words, and a decrease of 14 percentage po<strong>in</strong>ts<br />

for decod<strong>in</strong>g Set 3 words.<br />

Matthew’s performance data. Matthew made<br />

progress <strong>in</strong> accuracy of sound-to-letter match<strong>in</strong>g<br />

identification <strong>in</strong> two of three of the targeted<br />

letter sets dur<strong>in</strong>g the project (see Figure<br />

4). In the first letter set, /m, f, a, t, p, o/, he<br />

improved from a mean 33% to 40% accuracy<br />

<strong>in</strong> the sound-to-letter match<strong>in</strong>g task, a ga<strong>in</strong> of<br />

7 percentage po<strong>in</strong>ts from the basel<strong>in</strong>e condition<br />

through the end of the <strong>in</strong>tervention condition.<br />

Matthew made the greatest progress <strong>in</strong><br />

the second letter set, /b, g, i, s, l, e/, improv<strong>in</strong>g<br />

from a mean 38% to 62%, a ga<strong>in</strong> of 24<br />

percentage po<strong>in</strong>ts from pre- to post<strong>in</strong>tervention.<br />

Matthew ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed sound-to-letter<br />

match<strong>in</strong>g accuracy at 35% for Letter Set 3, /d,<br />

n, u, r, w, c/.<br />

Matthew’s pre-<strong>in</strong>tervention assessment of<br />

decod<strong>in</strong>g of all 54 novel words yielded accuracy<br />

levels at 42% (Word Set 1), 44% (Word<br />

Set 2), and 44% (Word Set 3). Intermittent<br />

word probes were also used to assess 18 of<br />

these words, each assessed twice per assessment<br />

once every 3 weeks. These three assessments<br />

yielded the follow<strong>in</strong>g accuracy levels:<br />

50%, 8%, and 83% (Set 1 words), 66%, 33%,<br />

and 83% (Set 2 words), and 50%, 33%, and<br />

75% (Set 3 words). Matthew’s accuracy <strong>in</strong>creased<br />

dur<strong>in</strong>g the majority of probe assessments<br />

over the course of the <strong>in</strong>tervention.<br />

Post<strong>in</strong>tervention assessments of all words<br />

yielded decod<strong>in</strong>g accuracy levels of 53% (Set 1<br />

words), reflect<strong>in</strong>g an <strong>in</strong>crease of 11 percentage<br />

po<strong>in</strong>ts. Matthew received a score of 33%<br />

accuracy for Set 2 words, reflect<strong>in</strong>g a decrease<br />

of 11 percentage po<strong>in</strong>ts. For Letter Set 3<br />

words, he scored 39% accuracy, which reflects<br />

a decrease of 5 percentage po<strong>in</strong>ts. All words<br />

were reassessed 5 months follow<strong>in</strong>g the completion<br />

of the <strong>in</strong>tervention. At the 5-month<br />

follow-up probe, Matthew achieved 50% accuracy<br />

<strong>in</strong> Set 1 words, 75% accuracy <strong>in</strong> Set 2<br />

words, and 33% accuracy <strong>in</strong> Set 3 words.<br />

These scores reflected a slight decrease of 3<br />

percentage po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> Matthew’s ability to decode<br />

these novel words for Set 1 words, an<br />

<strong>in</strong>crease of 42 percentage po<strong>in</strong>ts <strong>in</strong> word decod<strong>in</strong>g<br />

ability for Set 2 words, and a decrease<br />

of 6 percentage po<strong>in</strong>ts for Set 3 words.<br />

Trends across Participants<br />

Each participant demonstrated overall improvement<br />

<strong>in</strong> sound-to-letter match<strong>in</strong>g skills<br />

<strong>in</strong> at least two of three sound-letter sets targeted<br />

across the 9-week <strong>in</strong>tervention. All of<br />

the participants exhibited some variability <strong>in</strong><br />

skills across <strong>in</strong>tervention sessions, which was<br />

often associated <strong>with</strong> decreased attention to<br />

tasks dur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>tervention sessions. Of the four<br />

participants, Randy made the most consistent<br />

ga<strong>in</strong>s across all sound-letter sets. Matthew<br />

made the next highest ga<strong>in</strong>s overall <strong>in</strong> soundto-letter<br />

match<strong>in</strong>g skills. He had a significant<br />

hear<strong>in</strong>g loss, and just as the 3 rd sound-letter<br />

set was targeted, his graduate student cl<strong>in</strong>ician<br />

was <strong>in</strong>formed that he had received a new hear<strong>in</strong>g<br />

aid which was worn dur<strong>in</strong>g his assessments<br />

and <strong>in</strong>tervention activities. He had previously<br />

worn hear<strong>in</strong>g assistance <strong>in</strong>consistently. His<br />

teachers had mentioned that it was often “act<strong>in</strong>g<br />

up,” needed repair, or was left at home.<br />

Matthew’s data clearly show the positive effect<br />

of the use of this hear<strong>in</strong>g assistance that enabled<br />

him to make connections from the<br />

sound/visual cue and target letters. Amy and<br />

Lucy also made moderate <strong>in</strong>creases <strong>in</strong> soundto-letter<br />

match<strong>in</strong>g skills <strong>in</strong> two of three and<br />

one of three letter sets, respectively.<br />

Ga<strong>in</strong>s were observed for Randy and Amy <strong>in</strong><br />

the whole-word decod<strong>in</strong>g task. Matthew and<br />

Lucy did not make consistent or last<strong>in</strong>g progress<br />

<strong>in</strong> word decod<strong>in</strong>g. Laura and Randy cont<strong>in</strong>ued<br />

to evidence marked progress at the<br />

5-month follow-up probe. Matthew’s scores<br />

decreased m<strong>in</strong>imally <strong>in</strong> Letter Set 1 and 3<br />

words, and <strong>in</strong>creased by 42 percentage po<strong>in</strong>ts<br />

<strong>in</strong> Letter Set 2 words. Amy’s word decod<strong>in</strong>g<br />

scores, however, decreased at the 5-month follow-up<br />

assessment.<br />

<strong>Improv<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>Literacy</strong> <strong>Skills</strong> / 365


Procedural and Performance Reliability Data<br />

Figure 4. Graphic display of Matthew’s performance data.<br />

Graduate student cl<strong>in</strong>icians assigned to collect<br />

<strong>in</strong>terrater and performance reliability data<br />

collected data for 93% of <strong>in</strong>tervention and<br />

77% of assessment sessions. Procedural reliability<br />

data were calculated at a correlation<br />

coefficient of .96 for <strong>in</strong>tervention procedures,<br />

which was considered acceptable at above .90.<br />

Likewise, <strong>in</strong>terraters collected <strong>in</strong>dependent<br />

participant performance data for assessment<br />

and <strong>in</strong>tervention sessions. A correlation coefficient<br />

was calculated at .94 for all assessment<br />

data, which was above the predeterm<strong>in</strong>ed acceptable<br />

level of .90.<br />

Discussion<br />

Results of this study <strong>in</strong>dicate a functional relationship<br />

between the structured literacy <strong>in</strong>tervention<br />

and improved sound-to-letter<br />

366 / Education and Tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Autism and Developmental Disabilities-September 2011


match<strong>in</strong>g skills. Results of all participant data<br />

did not clearly establish a functional relationship<br />

between identification/decod<strong>in</strong>g of novel<br />

words, although two participants (Randy and<br />

Matthew) clearly improved and/or ma<strong>in</strong>ta<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

progress made <strong>in</strong> decod<strong>in</strong>g novel<br />

words dur<strong>in</strong>g the <strong>in</strong>vestigation and at the<br />

5-month follow-up assessment. These f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

are similar to those reported <strong>in</strong> Fallon et al.’s<br />

(2004) <strong>in</strong>vestigation and confirm the importance<br />

of literacy <strong>in</strong>struction for students <strong>with</strong><br />

CCN. Laura’s results appear to have been negatively<br />

impacted <strong>in</strong> Set 2 letters, when she<br />

missed several days of <strong>in</strong>tervention due to a<br />

family trip. Conversely, Matthew’s <strong>in</strong>dividual<br />

results appear to have been positively affected<br />

by a new hear<strong>in</strong>g aid device worn more consistently<br />

dur<strong>in</strong>g assessments and <strong>in</strong>terventions<br />

than he had made previously <strong>with</strong> the old,<br />

<strong>in</strong>consistently word hear<strong>in</strong>g aid device.<br />

The structured literacy <strong>in</strong>tervention <strong>in</strong>cluded<br />

read<strong>in</strong>g a phoneme-loaded picture<br />

book to the whole group, <strong>in</strong>dividual assessments,<br />

and <strong>in</strong>dividual scaffolded lessons <strong>with</strong><br />

error correction <strong>in</strong>struction. Results of this<br />

<strong>in</strong>vestigation provide evidence of the importance<br />

of the use of picture book-based phonological<br />

and phonemic awareness <strong>in</strong>terventions.<br />

Picture books have been identified as a<br />

developmentally appropriate way to <strong>in</strong>troduce<br />

read<strong>in</strong>g skills (Strasser & Seplocha, 2007) <strong>in</strong><br />

young children. Results of the current <strong>in</strong>vestigation<br />

suggest that these <strong>in</strong>terventions may be<br />

equally important <strong>in</strong> children <strong>with</strong> CCN. Unfortunately,<br />

specific effects attributable to the<br />

<strong>in</strong>clusion of a picture book-based <strong>in</strong>tervention<br />

were not isolated <strong>in</strong> participant outcomes <strong>in</strong><br />

this <strong>in</strong>vestigation. Because of the comb<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

nature of the <strong>in</strong>tervention package, it is not<br />

clear what effect(s) <strong>in</strong>dividual components of<br />

the <strong>in</strong>tervention had on ga<strong>in</strong>s achieved by<br />

participants. Component analyses of such <strong>in</strong>tervention<br />

“packages” are recommended to<br />

determ<strong>in</strong>e which aspects of the <strong>in</strong>terventions<br />

are more salient—and for which types of<br />

learners and AAC users.<br />

All of the participants exhibited some variability<br />

<strong>in</strong> skills across <strong>in</strong>tervention sessions.<br />

The researchers attributed this variability to<br />

participants’ <strong>in</strong>consistency <strong>in</strong> their ability to<br />

focus on tasks at hand across <strong>in</strong>tervention sessions.<br />

This <strong>in</strong>consistency has been described<br />

as a characteristic of students <strong>with</strong> ASD and<br />

moderate cognitive disabilities (Sherer &<br />

Shreibman, 2005).<br />

Limitations of the Study<br />

The <strong>in</strong>terventions were provided <strong>in</strong> two classrooms<br />

dur<strong>in</strong>g school hours. Therefore, classroom<br />

noises and activities were noted by graduate<br />

student cl<strong>in</strong>icians to have distracted the<br />

students on more than one occasion, which<br />

may have limited the effects of the <strong>in</strong>tervention.<br />

However, noise and activities are typical<br />

of naturalistic junior high level classroom environments.<br />

Therefore, additional methods to<br />

m<strong>in</strong>imize typical classroom noise and activities<br />

were not attempted.<br />

Letter sets were orig<strong>in</strong>ally selected <strong>in</strong> this<br />

project for sound comb<strong>in</strong>ations only, <strong>with</strong>out<br />

consideration of the visual characteristics of<br />

the letters. Two of the graduate student cl<strong>in</strong>icians<br />

reported that the students <strong>with</strong> whom<br />

they worked had difficulty <strong>with</strong> visual discrim<strong>in</strong>ation<br />

of similar-look<strong>in</strong>g letters. For example,<br />

Letter Set 1 conta<strong>in</strong>ed both /i/ and /l/.<br />

When these letters were pr<strong>in</strong>ted <strong>in</strong> 48-po<strong>in</strong>t<br />

Arial font and bold text, these participants<br />

often <strong>in</strong>correctly chose the /i/ for /l/ and<br />

vice versa dur<strong>in</strong>g sound-to-letter match<strong>in</strong>g<br />

tasks. These visual similarities between some<br />

of the letters <strong>in</strong> the letter sets may have also<br />

decreased <strong>in</strong>dividual participant accuracy.<br />

Conclusion<br />

Atta<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g literacy is a complex process that is<br />

made more difficult if students have CCN.<br />

However, literacy is a skill that is vital and<br />

contributes immensely to lead<strong>in</strong>g a productive<br />

and fulfill<strong>in</strong>g life. More research is needed to<br />

develop and determ<strong>in</strong>e the effectiveness of<br />

<strong>in</strong>terventions to develop literacy <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividuals<br />

<strong>with</strong> CCN. This study attempted to develop<br />

and evaluate one literacy <strong>in</strong>tervention for<br />

youths <strong>with</strong> CCN that can be implemented <strong>in</strong><br />

older students <strong>with</strong> limited literacy skills.<br />

References<br />

Adams, M. J. (1990). Beg<strong>in</strong>n<strong>in</strong>g to read: Th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g and<br />

learn<strong>in</strong>g about pr<strong>in</strong>t. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.<br />

Baer, D. M., Wolf, M. M., & Risley, T. R. (1968).<br />

Some current dimensions of applied behavior<br />

<strong>Improv<strong>in</strong>g</strong> <strong>Literacy</strong> <strong>Skills</strong> / 367


analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1, 91–<br />

97.<br />

Blischack, D. M., Shah, S. D., Lombard<strong>in</strong>o, L. J., &<br />

Chiarella, K. (2004). Effects of phonemic awareness<br />

<strong>in</strong>struction on the encod<strong>in</strong>g skills of children<br />

<strong>with</strong> severe speech impairment. Disability and Rehabilitation,<br />

26, 1295–1304.<br />

Blockberger, S., & Johnston, J. R. (2003). Grammatical<br />

morphology acquisition by children <strong>with</strong><br />

complex communication needs. Augmentative and<br />

Alternative <strong>Communication</strong>, 19, 207–221.<br />

Clay, M. M. (1975). What did I write? Auckland, New<br />

Zealand: He<strong>in</strong>emann.<br />

Dahlgren Sandberg, A. (2001). Read<strong>in</strong>g and spell<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

phonological awareness, and work<strong>in</strong>g memory<br />

<strong>in</strong> children <strong>with</strong> severe speech impairments: A<br />

longitud<strong>in</strong>al study. AAC: Augmentative and Alternative<br />

<strong>Communication</strong>, 17, 11–26.<br />

Dahlgren Sandberg, A., & Hjelmquist, E. (1996).<br />

Phonological awareness and literacy <strong>in</strong> nonspeak<strong>in</strong>g<br />

children: A prelim<strong>in</strong>ary study. European<br />

Journal of Disorders of <strong>Communication</strong>, 31, 289–308.<br />

Ellis, E. S., Worth<strong>in</strong>gton, I., & Lark<strong>in</strong>, M. (1994).<br />

Research synthesis on effective teach<strong>in</strong>g pr<strong>in</strong>ciples and<br />

the design of quality tools for educators: Executive summary<br />

(Tech. Rep. No. 6). Eugene: University of<br />

Oregon.<br />

Fallon, K. A., Light, J., McNaughton, D., Drager, K.,<br />

& Hammer, C. (2004). The effects of direct <strong>in</strong>struction<br />

on the s<strong>in</strong>gle-word read<strong>in</strong>g skills of children<br />

who require augmentative and alternative<br />

communication. Journal of Speech, Language, and<br />

Hear<strong>in</strong>g Research, 47, 1427–1439.<br />

Foley, B. E. (1993). The development of literacy <strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>in</strong>dividuals <strong>with</strong> severe congenital speech and motor<br />

impairments. Topics <strong>in</strong> Language Disorders, 13,<br />

16–32.<br />

Hetzroni, O. E. (2004). AAC and literacy. Disability<br />

and Rehabilitation, 26(21/22), 1305–1312.<br />

Individuals <strong>with</strong> Disabilities Education Improvement Act<br />

of 2004, 20 U.S.C. § 1400 et seq.<br />

International Read<strong>in</strong>g Association (IRA) & National<br />

Association for the Education of Young Children<br />

(NAEYC). (1998). Learn<strong>in</strong>g to read and write:<br />

Developmentally appropriate practices for young<br />

children. A jo<strong>in</strong>t position statement of the International<br />

Read<strong>in</strong>g Association and the National<br />

Association for the Education of Young Children.<br />

Young Children, 53, 30–46. Retrieved September<br />

26, 2006, from www.naeyc.org/about/positions/<br />

pdf/PSREAD98.PDF<br />

Johnston, S. S., Buchanan, S., & Davenport. L.<br />

(2009). Comparison of fixed and gradual array<br />

when teach<strong>in</strong>g sound-letter correspondence to<br />

two children <strong>with</strong> autism who use AAC. Augmentative<br />

and Alternative <strong>Communication</strong>, 25, 136–144.<br />

Light, J., & Drager, K. (2007). AAC technologies for<br />

young children <strong>with</strong> complex communication<br />

needs: State of the science and future research<br />

directions. AAC: Augmentative and Alternative <strong>Communication</strong>,<br />

23, 204–216.<br />

National Read<strong>in</strong>g Panel. (2000). Teach<strong>in</strong>g children to<br />

read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research<br />

literature on read<strong>in</strong>g and its implications for<br />

read<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>struction. Retrieved October 8, 2007,<br />

from http://www.nichd.nih.gov/publications/<br />

nrp/upload/smallbook_pdf.pdf<br />

Neuman, S. B. (1999). Books make a difference: A<br />

study of access to literacy. Read<strong>in</strong>g Research Quarterly,<br />

34, 286–311.<br />

No Child Left Beh<strong>in</strong>d Act, 20 U.S.C. 6301 et seq. (2001).<br />

Sherer, M. R., & Schreibman, L. (2005). Individual<br />

behavioral profiles and predictors of treatment<br />

effectiveness for children <strong>with</strong> autism. Journal of<br />

Consult<strong>in</strong>g and Cl<strong>in</strong>ical Psychology, 73, 525–538.<br />

Scherz, J., & Hart, P. (2002). Language, literacy, and<br />

AAC: Time to move forward. ASHA Leader, 7(16),<br />

17.<br />

Smith, M. M., & Blischak, D. M. (1997). <strong>Literacy</strong>. In<br />

L. L. Lloyd, D. R., Fuller, & H. H. Arvedson<br />

(Eds.), Augmentative and alternative communication:<br />

Handbook of pr<strong>in</strong>ciples and practices (pp. 414–444).<br />

Needham Heights, MA: Allyn and Bacon.<br />

Strasser, J., & Seplocha, H. (2007). Us<strong>in</strong>g picture<br />

books to support young children’s literacy. Childhood<br />

Education, 84, 219–225.<br />

Strum, J. M., & Clendon, S. A. (2004). Augmentative<br />

and alternative communication, language, and<br />

literacy: Foster<strong>in</strong>g the relationship. Topics <strong>in</strong> Language<br />

Disorders, 24, 76–91.<br />

Sturm, J. M., Erickson, K. A., & Yoder, D. E. (2003).<br />

State of the science: Enhanc<strong>in</strong>g literacy participation<br />

through AAC technologies. Journal of Assistive<br />

Technology, 14, 45–54.<br />

Sutton, A., Soto, G., & Blockberger, S. (2002).<br />

Grammatical issues <strong>in</strong> graphic symbol communication.<br />

Augmentative and Alternative <strong>Communication</strong>,<br />

18, 192–204.<br />

Teale, W. H., & Sulzby, E. (1986). Emergent literacy:<br />

Writ<strong>in</strong>g and read<strong>in</strong>g. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.<br />

Received: 20 July 2010<br />

Initial Acceptance: 15 September 2010<br />

F<strong>in</strong>al Acceptance: 21 November 2010<br />

368 / Education and Tra<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> Autism and Developmental Disabilities-September 2011

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!