01.08.2013 Views

etadd_46(4) - Division on Autism and Developmental Disabilities

etadd_46(4) - Division on Autism and Developmental Disabilities

etadd_46(4) - Division on Autism and Developmental Disabilities

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Training<br />

in<br />

<strong>Autism</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Cumulative Author Index<br />

2001-2010<br />

Focusing <strong>on</strong> individuals with<br />

autism, intellectual disabilities <strong>and</strong> other developmental disabilities<br />

Volume <str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g> Number 4<br />

DAD<br />

D<br />

December 2011


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

The Journal of the <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>,<br />

The Council for Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children<br />

Editor: Stanley H. Zucker<br />

Ariz<strong>on</strong>a State University<br />

Mary Lou Fult<strong>on</strong> Teachers College<br />

C<strong>on</strong>sulting Editors<br />

Martin Agran<br />

Reuben Altman<br />

Phillip J. Belfiore<br />

Shar<strong>on</strong> Borthwick-Duffy<br />

Michael P. Brady<br />

Fredda Brown<br />

Mary Lynne Calhoun<br />

Shar<strong>on</strong> F. Cramer<br />

Caroline Dunn<br />

Lise Fox<br />

David L. Gast<br />

Herbert Goldstein<br />

Juliet E. Hart<br />

Carolyn Hughes<br />

Larry K. Irvin<br />

James V. Kahn<br />

H. Earle Knowlt<strong>on</strong><br />

Barry W. Lavay<br />

Rena Lewis<br />

Kathleen J. Marshall<br />

Editorial Assistant: Silva Hassert<br />

Ariz<strong>on</strong>a State University<br />

Mary Lou Fult<strong>on</strong> Teachers College<br />

John McD<strong>on</strong>nell<br />

Gale M. Morris<strong>on</strong><br />

Gabriel A. Nardi<br />

John Nietupski<br />

James R. Patt<strong>on</strong><br />

Edward A. Polloway<br />

Thomas G. Roberts<br />

Robert S. Rueda<br />

Diane L. Ryndak<br />

Edward J. Sabornie<br />

Laurence R. Sargent<br />

Gary M. Sasso<br />

Tom E. C. Smith<br />

Scott Sparks<br />

Fred Spo<strong>on</strong>er<br />

Robert Stodden<br />

Keith Storey<br />

David L. Westling<br />

John J. Wheeler<br />

Mark Wolery<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong> is sent to all members of the <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong> of The Council for Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children. All <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> members must first be members of The Council for Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> membership dues are $25.00 for regular members <strong>and</strong> $13.00 for full time students. Membership is <strong>on</strong> a yearly basis. All inquiries<br />

c<strong>on</strong>cerning membership, subscripti<strong>on</strong>, advertising, etc. should be sent to the <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 2900 Crystal<br />

Drive, Suite 1000, Arlingt<strong>on</strong>, VA 22202-3557. Advertising rates are available up<strong>on</strong> request.<br />

Manuscripts should be typed, double spaced, <strong>and</strong> sent (five copies) to the Editor: Stanley H. Zucker, Mary Lou Fult<strong>on</strong> Teachers College, Box<br />

871811, Ariz<strong>on</strong>a State University, Tempe, AZ 85287-1811. Each manuscript should have a cover sheet that gives the names, affiliati<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>and</strong><br />

complete addresses of all authors.<br />

Editing policies are based <strong>on</strong> the Publicati<strong>on</strong> Manual, the American Psychological Associati<strong>on</strong>, 2009 revisi<strong>on</strong>. Additi<strong>on</strong>al informati<strong>on</strong> is<br />

provided <strong>on</strong> the inside back cover. Any signed article is the pers<strong>on</strong>al expressi<strong>on</strong> of the author; likewise, any advertisement is the resp<strong>on</strong>sibility<br />

of the advertiser. Neither necessarily carries <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> endorsement unless specifically set forth by adopted resoluti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong> is abstracted <strong>and</strong> indexed in Psychological Abstracts, PsycINFO, e-psyche,<br />

Abstracts for Social Workers, Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal of Rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> Research, Current C<strong>on</strong>tents/Social <strong>and</strong> Behavioral Sciences, Excerpta<br />

Medica, Social Sciences Citati<strong>on</strong> Index, Adolescent Mental Health Abstracts, Educati<strong>on</strong>al Administrati<strong>on</strong> Abstracts, Educati<strong>on</strong>al Research<br />

Abstracts, <strong>and</strong> Language <strong>and</strong> Language Behavior Abstracts. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, it is annotated <strong>and</strong> indexed by the ERIC Clearinghouse <strong>on</strong><br />

H<strong>and</strong>icapped <strong>and</strong> Gifted Children for publicati<strong>on</strong> in the m<strong>on</strong>thly print index Current Index to Journals in Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> the quarterly index,<br />

Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Child Educati<strong>on</strong> Resources.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong> Vol. <str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>, No. 3, September 2011, Copyright 2011 by the <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> Austim<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, The Council for Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

The Council for Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children<br />

Board of Directors<br />

Officers<br />

Past President Emily Bouck<br />

President Teresa Taber-Doughty<br />

President-Elect Richard Gargiulo<br />

Vice President Nikki Murdick<br />

Secretary T<strong>on</strong>i Merfeld<br />

Treasurer Gardner Umbarger<br />

Members<br />

Debra Cote<br />

Mark Francis<br />

Robert S<strong>and</strong>ies<strong>on</strong><br />

Jordan Shurr (Student Governor)<br />

Debora Wichmanowski<br />

Dianne Zager<br />

Executive Director<br />

Tom E. C. Smith<br />

Publicati<strong>on</strong>s Chair<br />

Michael Wehmeyer<br />

Communicati<strong>on</strong>s Chair<br />

Darlene Perner<br />

C<strong>on</strong>ference Coordinator<br />

Cindy Perras<br />

The purposes of this organizati<strong>on</strong> shall be to advance the educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> welfare of pers<strong>on</strong>s with autism <strong>and</strong> developmental disabilities, research<br />

in the educati<strong>on</strong> of pers<strong>on</strong>s with autism <strong>and</strong> developmental disabilities, competency of educators in this field, public underst<strong>and</strong>ing of autism<br />

<strong>and</strong> developmental disabilities, <strong>and</strong> legislati<strong>on</strong> needed to help accomplish these goals. The <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> shall encourage <strong>and</strong> promote professi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

growth, research, <strong>and</strong> the disseminati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> utilizati<strong>on</strong> of research findings.<br />

EDUCATION AND TRAINING IN AUTISM AND DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES (ISSN 2154-1647) (USPS 0168-5000) is published<br />

quarterly in March, June, September, <strong>and</strong> December, by The Council for Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 2900 Crystal Drive, Suite 1000, Arlingt<strong>on</strong>, Virginia 22202-3557. Members’ dues to The Council for<br />

Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong> include $8.00 for subscripti<strong>on</strong> to EDUCATION AND TRAINING IN<br />

AUTISM AND DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES. Subscripti<strong>on</strong> to EDUCATION AND TRAINING IN AUTISM AND DEVELOPMEN-<br />

TAL DISABILITIES is available without membership; Individual—U.S. $60.00 per year; Canada, PUAS, <strong>and</strong> all other countries $44.00;<br />

Instituti<strong>on</strong>s—U.S. $195.00 per year; Canada, PUAS, <strong>and</strong> all other countries $199.50; single copy price is $30.00. U.S. Periodicals postage<br />

is paid at Arlingt<strong>on</strong>, Virginia 22204 <strong>and</strong> additi<strong>on</strong>al mailing offices.<br />

POSTMASTERS: Send address changes to EDUCATION AND TRAINING IN AUTISM AND DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES,<br />

2900 Crystal Drive, Suite 1000, Arlingt<strong>on</strong>, Virginia 22202-3557.


March 2012<br />

Manuscripts Accepted for Future Publicati<strong>on</strong> in<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Bey<strong>on</strong>d time out <strong>and</strong> table time: Today's applied behavioral analysis for students with autism. E.<br />

Am<strong>and</strong>a Boutot <strong>and</strong> Kara Hume, Frank Porter Graham Child Development Institute, University of<br />

North Carolina at Chapel Hill, 517 S. Greensboro, Carrboro, NC 27510.<br />

Experiences of preschoolers with severe disabilities in an inclusive early educati<strong>on</strong> setting: A qualitative<br />

study. Mary Frances Hanline <strong>and</strong> Silvia M. Correa-Torres, School of Teacher Educati<strong>on</strong>, Florida State<br />

University, Tallahassee, FL 32306.<br />

Effects of combined repeated reading <strong>and</strong> questi<strong>on</strong> generati<strong>on</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> young adults with<br />

cognitive disabilities. Youjia Hua, William J. Therrien, Jo M. Hendricks<strong>on</strong>, Suzanne Woods-Groves,<br />

Pamela S. Ries, <strong>and</strong> Julia W. Shaw, University of Iowa, College of Educati<strong>on</strong>, Department of Teaching<br />

<strong>and</strong> Learning, N256 Lindquist Center, Iowa City, IA 52242.<br />

How <strong>and</strong> why do parents choose early intensive behaivoral interventi<strong>on</strong> for their young child with<br />

autism?. Pag<strong>on</strong>a Tzanakaki, Corinna Grindle, Richard P. Hastings, J. Carl Hughes, Hanna Kovshoff,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Bob Remingt<strong>on</strong>, School of Psychology, Adeilad Brigantia, Penrallt Road, Bangor, Gwynedd LL57<br />

2AS UK.<br />

Effects of differential reinforcement of short latencies <strong>on</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se latency, task completi<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong><br />

accuracy of an adolescent with autism. Melanie M. D<strong>on</strong>ohue, Laura Baylot Casey, David F. Bicard,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Sara E. Bicard, College of Educati<strong>on</strong>, The University of Memphis, 405 Ball Hall, Memphis, TN<br />

38152.<br />

Seven reas<strong>on</strong>s to promote st<strong>and</strong>ards-based instructi<strong>on</strong> for students with severe disabilities: A reply to<br />

Ayres, Lowrey, Douglas, & Sievers (2011). Ginevra Courtade, Fred Spo<strong>on</strong>er, Diane Browder, <strong>and</strong><br />

Bree Jimenez, College of Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Human Development, University of Louisville, Louisville, KY<br />

40292.<br />

Assessment of the self-determinati<strong>on</strong> of Spanish students with intellectual disabilities <strong>and</strong> other<br />

educati<strong>on</strong>al needs. María Gómez Vela, Miguel Ángel Verdugo Al<strong>on</strong>so, Francisca Gil G<strong>on</strong>zález, Marta<br />

Badia Corbella, <strong>and</strong> Michael L. Wehmeyer, University of Salamanca, Institute <strong>on</strong> Community Integrati<strong>on</strong><br />

(INICO), Facultad de Psicología, Avda. de la Merced 109-131, 37005 - Salamanca, SPAIN.<br />

Self-advocacy skills as a predictor of student IEP participati<strong>on</strong> am<strong>on</strong>g adolescents with autism spectrum<br />

disorders. Lucy Barnard-Brak <strong>and</strong> Danielle D. Fear<strong>on</strong>, Baylor University, One Bear Place #97301,<br />

Waco, Texas 76798.<br />

Teaching play skills to children with autism through video modeling: Small group arrangement <strong>and</strong><br />

observati<strong>on</strong>al learning. Sema Batu, Arzu Ozen, <strong>and</strong> Sema Batu, Anadolu Universitesi, Engelliler<br />

Arastirma Enstitusu, Eskisehir, 26470 TURKEY.<br />

The Questi<strong>on</strong> Still Remains: What Happens When the Curricular Focus for Students with Severe<br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong> Shifts? A Reply to Courtade, Spo<strong>on</strong>er, Browder, <strong>and</strong> Jimenez (2012). Kevin M. Ayres, K.<br />

Alisa Lowrey, Karen H. Douglas, <strong>and</strong> Courtney Sievers, Department of Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, The<br />

University of Georgia, 516 Aderhold Hall, Athens, GA 30602-7153.<br />

Address is supplied for author in boldface type.


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 2011, <str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>(4), 479-498<br />

© <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Review of Twenty-First Century Portable Electr<strong>on</strong>ic Devices<br />

for Pers<strong>on</strong>s with Moderate Intellectual <strong>Disabilities</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>Autism</strong> Spectrum Disorders<br />

Linda C. Mechling<br />

University of North Carolina Wilmingt<strong>on</strong><br />

Abstract: Use of portable electr<strong>on</strong>ic devices by pers<strong>on</strong>s with moderate intellectual disabilities <strong>and</strong> autism spectrum<br />

disorders is gaining increased research attenti<strong>on</strong>. The purpose of this review was to synthesize twenty-first century<br />

literature (2000-2010) focusing <strong>on</strong> these technologies. Twenty-<strong>on</strong>e studies were identified which evaluated use<br />

of: (a) media players with audio playback; (b) cellular/smartph<strong>on</strong>es; (c) h<strong>and</strong>held computers <strong>and</strong> h<strong>and</strong>held<br />

video players across various skills <strong>and</strong> settings to assist pers<strong>on</strong>s with disabilities. Guidelines <strong>and</strong> recommendati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> future research are provided.<br />

Pers<strong>on</strong>s with moderate intellectual disabilities<br />

(ID) <strong>and</strong> those with a diagnosis of autism<br />

spectrum disorder (ASD) have been shown to<br />

have the ability to self-manage their own behaviors<br />

<strong>and</strong> to independently complete functi<strong>on</strong>al,<br />

daily tasks when provided with the<br />

proper tools <strong>and</strong> technologies (Mechling,<br />

2007; Wehmeyer, Palmer, Smith, Davies, &<br />

Stock, 2008). With the use of assistive technologies<br />

or mainstream technologies, <strong>on</strong>e of the<br />

goals is to increase independence while decreasing<br />

reliance <strong>on</strong> other pers<strong>on</strong>s for assistance.<br />

Today’s portable electr<strong>on</strong>ic technologies,<br />

including h<strong>and</strong>held computers, cellular<br />

(cell) ph<strong>on</strong>es, e-books or electr<strong>on</strong>ic readers,<br />

global positi<strong>on</strong>ing systems (GPS) <strong>and</strong> pers<strong>on</strong>al<br />

media or MP3 players, appear to hold<br />

potential for assisting pers<strong>on</strong>s with moderate<br />

ID <strong>and</strong> those with a diagnosis of ASD. These<br />

portable technologies may be adapted or specially<br />

designed for pers<strong>on</strong>s with disabilities<br />

(i.e., Community Integrati<strong>on</strong> Suite by<br />

Ablelink Technologies; Cyrano Communicator<br />

TM by Kiba Technologies, LLC) or generic,<br />

mainstream technologies such as cell<br />

ph<strong>on</strong>es <strong>and</strong> the iPod by Apple, Inc. that are<br />

Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence c<strong>on</strong>cerning this article should<br />

be addressed to Linda Mechling, University of<br />

North Carolina Wilmingt<strong>on</strong>, Department of Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

of Young Children <strong>and</strong> Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 601<br />

S. College Road, Wilmingt<strong>on</strong>, NC 28404-5940.<br />

mass produced (Carey, Friedman, & Bryen,<br />

2005). Their portability <strong>and</strong> capacity for storing<br />

large amounts of data formulate a tool for<br />

providing multiple uses for pers<strong>on</strong>s with moderate<br />

ID <strong>and</strong> ASD including: (a) remembering<br />

<strong>and</strong> performing steps of a complex task (Riffel<br />

et al., 2005); (b) decisi<strong>on</strong> making (Davies,<br />

Stock, & Wehmeyer, 2003); (c) organizati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> time management (Gillette & DePompei,<br />

2008); <strong>and</strong> (d) self-m<strong>on</strong>itoring <strong>and</strong> self-management<br />

of behaviors (Cihak, Wright, & Ayres,<br />

2010).<br />

While these portable electr<strong>on</strong>ic devices are<br />

rapidly increasing in number <strong>and</strong> advancing<br />

in capabilities, research evaluating their applied<br />

use with pers<strong>on</strong>s with disabilities remains<br />

relatively minute. In their meta-analysis<br />

of single-subject design studies which evaluated<br />

use of technology by pers<strong>on</strong>s with intellectual<br />

disabilities, Wehmeyer et al. (2008)<br />

recommended more research with a wider<br />

range of technology devices <strong>and</strong> reported that<br />

<strong>on</strong>ly .9% of the 81 studies they evaluated used<br />

palmtop computers, 2.2% evaluated palmtop<br />

computers with audio vibrators, <strong>and</strong> 4.8%<br />

evaluated electr<strong>on</strong>ic <strong>and</strong> informati<strong>on</strong> technologies<br />

(i.e., cell ph<strong>on</strong>es).<br />

The purpose of this review was to examine<br />

the most current research which has applied<br />

portable electr<strong>on</strong>ic devices to increase the independent<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>ing of pers<strong>on</strong>s with moderate<br />

ID <strong>and</strong> those with a diagnosis of ASD. By<br />

examining the present status of a relatively<br />

Twenty-First Century Portable Electr<strong>on</strong>ic Devices / 479


new line of research, the review holds potential<br />

for laying the ground work for additi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

lines <strong>and</strong> directi<strong>on</strong>s for future research.<br />

Method<br />

The period of review was limited to those<br />

studies c<strong>on</strong>ducted in the twenty-first century.<br />

Although a review of this limited extent risks<br />

exclusi<strong>on</strong> of some earlier findings, devices<br />

such as pers<strong>on</strong>al digital assistants, which have<br />

been in existence since the beginning of the<br />

eighties, have <strong>on</strong>ly become extensively popular<br />

in the last few years (Nashville, 2009). Likewise,<br />

cellular ph<strong>on</strong>es were a rarity fifteen years<br />

ago (Cell Ph<strong>on</strong>es.org, 2008), <strong>and</strong> the popular<br />

iPod <strong>and</strong> video iPod by Apple, Inc. were introduced<br />

in 2000 <strong>and</strong> 2005 respectively. Therefore,<br />

due to the relatively recent introducti<strong>on</strong><br />

of these portable electr<strong>on</strong>ic technologies into<br />

mainstream society, <strong>and</strong> the rapidly changing<br />

nature of technology, this review extended<br />

from the years 2000–2010. Journal articles<br />

published between those years were located<br />

using an electr<strong>on</strong>ic search through a university<br />

EBSCOhost database (Academic Search<br />

Premier, Eric, MasterFILE Premier, PsychAR-<br />

TICLES, <strong>and</strong> PsycINFO). Specific key words<br />

used in the search were a combinati<strong>on</strong> of the<br />

words disabilities, intellectual disabilities, autism,<br />

autism spectrum disorders, mental retardati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> the words pers<strong>on</strong>al digital assistants,<br />

PDAs, h<strong>and</strong>held computers, palmtop<br />

computers, pocket PCs, cellular ph<strong>on</strong>es, cell<br />

ph<strong>on</strong>es, mobile ph<strong>on</strong>es, smartph<strong>on</strong>es, mobile<br />

technologies, portable electr<strong>on</strong>ic devices,<br />

iPh<strong>on</strong>e, iPod, video iPod, MP3 players, <strong>and</strong><br />

portable media players. In additi<strong>on</strong> to the<br />

electr<strong>on</strong>ic search, a cross-reference, manual<br />

search was made of previously identified articles.<br />

In order to be included in the review, articles<br />

had to meet the following criteria:<br />

1. Use of a quasi-experimental or single-subject<br />

research design.<br />

2. Publicati<strong>on</strong> in peer-reviewed journal published<br />

in the English language.<br />

3. Primary interventi<strong>on</strong> was the evaluati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

a form of portable electr<strong>on</strong>ic technology:<br />

h<strong>and</strong>held computer, cellular ph<strong>on</strong>e, MP3<br />

player.<br />

4. Participants were diagnosed with a moder-<br />

ate intellectual disability <strong>and</strong>/or autism<br />

spectrum disorder.<br />

Results<br />

Twenty-<strong>on</strong>e studies (Table 1) were identified<br />

<strong>and</strong> included in this review. Although not<br />

within the scope of this literature review, readers<br />

may also wish to review the work being<br />

c<strong>on</strong>ducted with h<strong>and</strong>held devices <strong>and</strong> smartph<strong>on</strong>es<br />

as memory <strong>and</strong> organizati<strong>on</strong> aids with<br />

pers<strong>on</strong>s with traumatic brain injury (i.e., De-<br />

Pompei et al., 2008; Gentry, Wallace, Kvarfordt,<br />

& Lynch, 2008; Gillette & DePopmei,<br />

2008; Hart, Buchhofer, & Vaccaro, 2004; Hart,<br />

O’Neil-Pirozzi, & Morita, 2003; Wade & Troy,<br />

2001). The review is organized around three<br />

types of portable electr<strong>on</strong>ic technologies:<br />

h<strong>and</strong>held computers <strong>and</strong> h<strong>and</strong>held video<br />

players (17 studies), cellular ph<strong>on</strong>es (3 studies),<br />

<strong>and</strong> MP3 players (1 study). Skills addressed<br />

were: functi<strong>on</strong>al, multi-step skills (15<br />

studies); time management <strong>and</strong> organizati<strong>on</strong><br />

skills (4 studies), <strong>and</strong> independent transiti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

(2 studies). The majority of participants were<br />

15 years of age <strong>and</strong> older (18 studies) while<br />

seven studies included students under the age<br />

of 15 years <strong>and</strong> two studies included elementary<br />

age students. The majority of the studies<br />

evaluated use of portable electr<strong>on</strong>ic devices<br />

with students with moderate ID (17 studies)<br />

<strong>and</strong> five of the studies included students with<br />

ASD.<br />

Media Players with Audio Playback<br />

In 2007, Millard reported that “every m<strong>on</strong>th a<br />

new portable MP3 player is <strong>on</strong> the market”<br />

with capabilities <strong>and</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>s extending bey<strong>on</strong>d<br />

music players. She further sites the increased<br />

use of these players as a creative<br />

means for providing alternative methods for<br />

students with disabilities to learn. Simply applied,<br />

MP3 players can deliver auditory informati<strong>on</strong><br />

to students in a step-by-step format for<br />

completing multiple step tasks or they can be<br />

used to prompt <strong>on</strong>-task behaviors. Media players<br />

such as the iPod provide auditory prompts<br />

<strong>and</strong> informati<strong>on</strong> in much the same way that<br />

earlier studies used portable cassette players<br />

(Post & Storey, 2003; Taber, Alberto, &<br />

Fredrick, 1998), but provide more sophisticated<br />

means for navigating through recorded<br />

480 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


TABLE 1<br />

Overview of H<strong>and</strong>held Technology<br />

Research Design Setting Results<br />

Author(s) Participants Target Skill<br />

(Dependent Variable)<br />

Alternating treatment Domestic living area<br />

of high school<br />

Operate debit & copy<br />

machine<br />

Media Players with Audio<br />

Playback<br />

Taber-Doughty, (2005) n 3CA 15-21yrs<br />

Moderate ID<br />

Picture prompting system,<br />

SLP, & MP 3 Player<br />

effective & efficient<br />

procedures.<br />

Performance superior<br />

when students used<br />

their preferred system.<br />

Center-based Compared to a<br />

mainstream cell ph<strong>on</strong>e,<br />

participants required<br />

fewer prompts & made<br />

fewer errors with the<br />

Within- Subjects<br />

paired samples<br />

Operate adapted<br />

ph<strong>on</strong>e functi<strong>on</strong>s &<br />

mainstream ph<strong>on</strong>e<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

n 22 CA 18-<br />

21yrs Full scale IQ<br />

range 47–69<br />

Cellular/Smartph<strong>on</strong>e<br />

Stock, Davies, Wehmeyer,<br />

& Palmer (2008)<br />

adapted ph<strong>on</strong>e.<br />

Participants effectively<br />

dialed ph<strong>on</strong>e numbers.<br />

Difficulty describing<br />

School-based.<br />

Generalizati<strong>on</strong> to<br />

community.<br />

Multiple probe across<br />

groups<br />

Dial ph<strong>on</strong>e # from<br />

printed card when<br />

lost. Describe<br />

n 14 CA 11-<br />

14yrs Moderate ID<br />

Taber, Alberto, Hughes,<br />

& Seltzer (2002)<br />

locati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

locati<strong>on</strong><br />

Participants effectively<br />

used speed dial<br />

functi<strong>on</strong> to place<br />

ph<strong>on</strong>e call & to answer<br />

ph<strong>on</strong>e when lost in<br />

school or community.<br />

Difficulty describing<br />

locati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

School-based<br />

Generalizati<strong>on</strong> to<br />

community.<br />

Multiple probe across<br />

students<br />

Operate speed dial,<br />

answer ph<strong>on</strong>e<br />

when lost. Describe<br />

locati<strong>on</strong><br />

n 6CA 14-18yrs<br />

Moderate ID<br />

Taber, Alberto, Seltzer, &<br />

Hughes (2003)<br />

(C<strong>on</strong>tinued)<br />

Twenty-First Century Portable Electr<strong>on</strong>ic Devices / 481


TABLE 1—(C<strong>on</strong>tinued)<br />

Overview of H<strong>and</strong>held Technology<br />

Research Design Setting Results<br />

Author(s) Participants Target Skill<br />

(Dependent Variable)<br />

n 12 CA 19-<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>yrs Full scale IQ<br />

range 45–90<br />

PDA with Text, Sound,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Light Cues<br />

Davies, Stock, Wehmeyer<br />

(2002a)<br />

PDA with specially<br />

designed prompting<br />

software more effective<br />

than written schedule<br />

for prompting initiati<strong>on</strong><br />

Community<br />

vocati<strong>on</strong>al site<br />

Two-group withinsubjects<br />

design<br />

Compared<br />

performance of<br />

tasks <strong>on</strong> schedule<br />

using a written<br />

schedule or PDA<br />

of tasks.<br />

School setting Students were able to use<br />

the PDA as a task<br />

management tool.<br />

Maintenance of skill up<br />

to 8 weeks.<br />

Quasi-experimental<br />

pre- & postassessment<br />

Independent<br />

operati<strong>on</strong> of PDA<br />

(enter<br />

appointments,<br />

schedules,<br />

assignments) &<br />

frequency of use of<br />

PDA<br />

n 22 CA 14-<br />

18yrs <strong>Autism</strong><br />

Gentry, Wallace,<br />

Kvarfordt, & Lynch<br />

(2010)<br />

Highest rate of <strong>on</strong>-time<br />

behavior using the<br />

PDA.<br />

School-based<br />

settings<br />

Comparis<strong>on</strong> of<br />

incidence rate ratios<br />

across c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s &<br />

periods using a<br />

Poiss<strong>on</strong> regressi<strong>on</strong><br />

On-time behavior<br />

using: written time<br />

& task list; paper<br />

planner, & PDA<br />

n 20 CA 6-20yrs<br />

Mild - moderate<br />

ID<br />

Gillette & Depompei<br />

(2008)<br />

482 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011<br />

Community settings Increased independent<br />

transiti<strong>on</strong>s between<br />

tasks. Maintenance up<br />

to 9 weeks.<br />

Multiple probe across<br />

students<br />

Transiti<strong>on</strong> between<br />

vocati<strong>on</strong>al tasks<br />

n 4CA 16-17yrs<br />

Moderate to<br />

Severe ID<br />

PDA with Picture Cues<br />

Cihak, Kessler, & Alberto<br />

(2008)


TABLE 1—(C<strong>on</strong>tinued)<br />

Overview of H<strong>and</strong>held Technology<br />

Research Design Setting Results<br />

Author(s) Participants Target Skill<br />

(Dependent Variable)<br />

Community settings Independent task<br />

completi<strong>on</strong> using the<br />

PDA. Maintenance up to<br />

Multiple probe across<br />

tasks<br />

Independent task<br />

completi<strong>on</strong> of<br />

progressively more<br />

n 4CA 18-19yrs<br />

Moderate ID<br />

Cihak, Kessler, & Alberto<br />

(2007)<br />

9 weeks.<br />

difficult tasks<br />

Self-model pictures <strong>on</strong> the<br />

PDA resulted in increased<br />

task engagement &<br />

decreased teacher<br />

ABAB Middle school<br />

general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

classrooms<br />

Self-m<strong>on</strong>itoring of<br />

task engagement<br />

n 3CA 11-13yrs<br />

High functi<strong>on</strong>ing<br />

autism<br />

Cihak, Wright, & Ayres<br />

(2010)<br />

prompts.<br />

Use of PDA with specially<br />

designed software<br />

incorporating decisi<strong>on</strong><br />

points resulted in<br />

increased independence<br />

& accuracy assembling<br />

Community<br />

vocati<strong>on</strong>al setting<br />

Two-group withinsubjects<br />

design<br />

Software & Pocket PC<br />

assembly<br />

n 40 CA 18-<br />

54yrs Full scale IQ<br />

range 24–76<br />

Davis, Stock, &<br />

Wehmeyer (2003)<br />

both tasks.<br />

PDA with specially designed<br />

software produced<br />

improved task accuracy &<br />

decreased reliance <strong>on</strong><br />

Community<br />

vocati<strong>on</strong>al setting<br />

Two-group withinsubjects<br />

design<br />

Assemble pizza boxes<br />

& package software<br />

n 10 CA 18-<br />

70yrs Full scale IQ<br />

range 39–72<br />

Davies, Stock, &<br />

Wehneyer (2002b)<br />

adult prompts.<br />

Alternating treatment Day activity center Students completed more<br />

steps independently when<br />

using the PDA compared<br />

to a picture based system.<br />

Steps were also clustered<br />

into fewer pictures <strong>on</strong> the<br />

PDA.<br />

Cleaning, table<br />

setting, food<br />

preparati<strong>on</strong><br />

n 6CA 23-47yrs<br />

IQ unavailable<br />

Adaptive Behavior<br />

Scale, age<br />

equivalents 2-6.5<br />

years for daily<br />

living<br />

Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, O’Reilly,<br />

Seedhouse, Furniss, &<br />

Cunha (2000)<br />

Twenty-First Century Portable Electr<strong>on</strong>ic Devices / 483


TABLE 1—(C<strong>on</strong>tinued)<br />

Overview of H<strong>and</strong>held Technology<br />

Research Design Setting Results<br />

Author(s) Participants Target Skill<br />

(Dependent Variable)<br />

Use of PDA with specially<br />

designed software increased<br />

number of steps performed<br />

without adult prompting.<br />

Decrease in durati<strong>on</strong> time<br />

to complete tasks.<br />

School, group<br />

home, retirement<br />

home, restaurant<br />

Multiple probe across<br />

participants<br />

Table setting, rolling<br />

silverware, laundry<br />

Riffel et al. (2005) n 4CA 16-21yrs<br />

Mild to moderate<br />

ID, <strong>Autism</strong><br />

PDA with Video Cues &<br />

H<strong>and</strong>held Video<br />

Players<br />

ABAB Elementary school Increased independent<br />

transiti<strong>on</strong>ing with video<br />

models presented <strong>on</strong><br />

video iPod. Performance<br />

decreased with withdrawal<br />

Transiti<strong>on</strong>s between<br />

school locati<strong>on</strong>s &<br />

activities<br />

n 4CA 6-8yrs<br />

<strong>Autism</strong><br />

Cihak, Fahrenkrog,<br />

Ayres, & Smith (2010)<br />

of video models.<br />

Use of PDA with multiple<br />

prompt levels (audio,<br />

picture, video) resulted in<br />

immediate increase in<br />

independent completi<strong>on</strong> of<br />

cooking recipes. Students<br />

prompts used. Performance<br />

Home living area of<br />

high school<br />

Food preparati<strong>on</strong> Multiple probe across<br />

tasks<br />

n 3CA 15-17yrs<br />

Moderate ID<br />

Mechling, Gast, & Seid<br />

(2010)<br />

484 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011<br />

maintained over time.<br />

Use of PDA with multiple<br />

prompt levels (audio,<br />

picture, video) replicated<br />

results of Mechling et al.<br />

(2010) although use &<br />

self-adjustment of prompt<br />

levels differed from<br />

previous findings.<br />

Home living area of<br />

high school<br />

Food preparati<strong>on</strong> Multiple probe across<br />

tasks<br />

n 3CA 16-17yrs<br />

<strong>Autism</strong><br />

Mechling, Gast, & Seid<br />

(2009)


TABLE 1—(C<strong>on</strong>tinued)<br />

Overview of H<strong>and</strong>held Technology<br />

Research Design Setting Results<br />

Author(s) Participants Target Skill<br />

(Dependent Variable)<br />

University campus Use of PDA with multiple<br />

prompt levels (audio,<br />

picture, video) resulted in<br />

students independently<br />

locating three different<br />

destinati<strong>on</strong>s. Students selfadjusted<br />

prompt levels<br />

used. Maintenance of<br />

Multiple probe across<br />

tasks<br />

Pedestrian travel<br />

(walking) locating<br />

l<strong>and</strong>marks &<br />

destinati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

Mechling & Seid (2011) n 3CA 21-22yrs<br />

Moderate ID<br />

skills over time.<br />

Alternating treatments Community library Both simultaneous &<br />

delayed video modelling<br />

effective. Preferred<br />

modeling system more<br />

effective for 2 students.<br />

Generalizati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

performance across<br />

Locating books &<br />

DVDs. Use of<br />

computer to locate<br />

call numbers<br />

n 3CA 13-15yrs<br />

Moderate ID<br />

Taber-Doughty, Patt<strong>on</strong>, &<br />

Brennan (2008)<br />

libraries.<br />

Video prompting & feedback,<br />

using a Video iPod,<br />

resulted in an increase in<br />

correct resp<strong>on</strong>ding across<br />

tasks & decrease in adult<br />

prompting for error<br />

correcti<strong>on</strong> & use of the<br />

No-kill animal<br />

shelter<br />

Multiple probe across<br />

tasks<br />

Emptying trash,<br />

mopping, cleaning<br />

bathroom, cleaning<br />

kennels<br />

n 1CA 17yrs<br />

Moderate ID<br />

Van Laarhoven, Van<br />

Laarhoven-Myers,<br />

Grider, & Grider<br />

(2009)<br />

device.<br />

Video modeling & feed<br />

back & least to most<br />

prompting resulted in<br />

increased independent<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ding & decrease in<br />

adult prompting<br />

Red Robin &<br />

Applebee’s<br />

restaurants<br />

Multiple probe across<br />

tasks<br />

Sorting & sanitizing<br />

silverware,<br />

porti<strong>on</strong>ing recipes,<br />

cleaning &<br />

sanitizing work<br />

area, clocking in &<br />

out, rolling<br />

silverware<br />

n 2CA 18yrs<br />

Mild to moderate<br />

ID<br />

Van Laarhoven, Van<br />

Laarhoven-Myers, &<br />

Zurita (2007)<br />

Twenty-First Century Portable Electr<strong>on</strong>ic Devices / 485


Figure 1. MP3 Player (Model: SM-320V). Pine<br />

Technology, Ltd.<br />

segments <strong>on</strong> the device. In additi<strong>on</strong>, media<br />

players such as video iPods now feature video<br />

playback (see secti<strong>on</strong> titled H<strong>and</strong>held Computers<br />

<strong>and</strong> H<strong>and</strong>held Video Players).<br />

In the <strong>on</strong>ly identified study evaluating use<br />

of a media player with audio playback, Taber-<br />

Doughty (2005) used a D’music MP3 Player<br />

(Pine Technology, Ltd) (Figure 1) to deliver<br />

auditory prompts while comparing the effects<br />

<strong>and</strong> efficiency of student choice <strong>and</strong> task performance<br />

between prompting methods (MP3<br />

player, system of least prompts, <strong>and</strong> picture<br />

prompts). Data were collected for the percent<br />

of task steps completed independently <strong>and</strong><br />

the durati<strong>on</strong> of task completi<strong>on</strong> for the tasks<br />

of operating a copying machine <strong>and</strong> making<br />

purchases using a debit card machine. Results<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strated that each system was effective<br />

<strong>and</strong> efficient for five high school age students<br />

with moderate ID <strong>and</strong> that students’ performances<br />

were superior when using their system<br />

of choice.<br />

Although the experimental design did not<br />

meet the criteria for inclusi<strong>on</strong> in the current<br />

literature review, <strong>on</strong>e additi<strong>on</strong>al study was<br />

identified which used a portable cassette<br />

player to prompt students with moderate intellectual<br />

<strong>and</strong> visual disabilities (Lanci<strong>on</strong>i,<br />

O’Reilly & Oliva, 2001). The study dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

that although more sophisticated devices<br />

may be available, researchers <strong>and</strong> practiti<strong>on</strong>ers<br />

are still finding value in lighter tech<br />

systems. An interesting finding <strong>and</strong> focus of<br />

the study was that following task mastery using<br />

step-by-step auditory instructi<strong>on</strong>s, the partici-<br />

pants were able to complete tasks when two<br />

auditory steps from the original instructi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

were clustered together.<br />

Cellular/Smartph<strong>on</strong>es<br />

In additi<strong>on</strong> to meeting basic communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

needs, it is suggested that use of cell ph<strong>on</strong>es by<br />

pers<strong>on</strong>s with disabilities can address safety issues<br />

(i.e., being lost, being str<strong>and</strong>ed if a wheelchair<br />

breaks down) or be used as a memory<br />

aid (alarms <strong>and</strong> reminder features) (Bryen,<br />

Carey, & Friedman, 2007). In their survey of<br />

cell ph<strong>on</strong>e use by 83 pers<strong>on</strong>s with intellectual<br />

disabilities, Bryen et al. found that in additi<strong>on</strong><br />

to day-to-day communicati<strong>on</strong>, cell ph<strong>on</strong>es<br />

were most often used for emergencies, storing<br />

ph<strong>on</strong>e numbers, <strong>and</strong> storing calendar informati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

In the current review, two of the three<br />

studies which evaluated use of cell ph<strong>on</strong>es by<br />

pers<strong>on</strong>s with moderate intellectual disabilities,<br />

used a cell ph<strong>on</strong>e as an interventi<strong>on</strong> for the<br />

emergency situati<strong>on</strong> of being lost in the community.<br />

In the first study, Taber, Alberto,<br />

Hughes, <strong>and</strong> Seltzer (2002) found that 14<br />

middle school students were able to use a cell<br />

ph<strong>on</strong>e to dial a number by copying a ph<strong>on</strong>e<br />

number from a printed card <strong>and</strong> to describe<br />

their physical locati<strong>on</strong>. Sessi<strong>on</strong>s were first c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

at school <strong>and</strong> then generalized to a<br />

community setting. In a final phase, students<br />

also dialed a different ph<strong>on</strong>e number than the<br />

<strong>on</strong>e used during training.<br />

Because some students had difficulty dialing<br />

the ph<strong>on</strong>e number in the first study, speed<br />

dialing was used in a sec<strong>on</strong>d study by Taber,<br />

Alberto, Seltzer, <strong>and</strong> Hughes (2003). In additi<strong>on</strong><br />

to the change in dialing, they also evaluated<br />

six sec<strong>on</strong>dary age students’ abilities to<br />

answer a cell ph<strong>on</strong>e <strong>and</strong> describe their locati<strong>on</strong><br />

to the caller for those students who were<br />

not able to recognize that they were lost. Once<br />

again, training took place in the school setting<br />

<strong>and</strong> then generalizati<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s occurred in<br />

two community settings. Results were again<br />

supportive of cell ph<strong>on</strong>e use by students with<br />

moderate intellectual disabilities when lost in<br />

the community. One interesting result of each<br />

of these studies was that in additi<strong>on</strong> to some<br />

issues with operati<strong>on</strong> of the cell ph<strong>on</strong>e itself,<br />

students had the greatest amount of difficulty<br />

describing their physical locati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

In spite of their potential benefits, Bryen et<br />

486 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


al. (2007) still found a gap between the use of<br />

cell ph<strong>on</strong>es by people with intellectual disabilities<br />

compared to pers<strong>on</strong>s without disabilities.<br />

In their report they found that n<strong>on</strong>-use by<br />

pers<strong>on</strong>s with intellectual disabilities was primarily<br />

due to cost, percepti<strong>on</strong> of not needing<br />

a cell ph<strong>on</strong>e, <strong>and</strong> lack of accessibility. To address<br />

the issue of cognitive accessibility, Stock,<br />

Davies, Wehmeyer, <strong>and</strong> Palmer (2008) evaluated<br />

a specially designed multimedia software<br />

prototype, Pocket ACE (AbleLink Technologies),<br />

with 22 participants with intellectual disabilities<br />

(range IQ score 47–69). The program<br />

operated <strong>on</strong> the Pocket PC 2002 Ph<strong>on</strong>e editi<strong>on</strong><br />

of a mainstream PDA <strong>and</strong> incorporated a<br />

picture-based address book <strong>and</strong> simplified system<br />

for operating ph<strong>on</strong>e functi<strong>on</strong>s of the<br />

PDA. Adapted features allowed participants to<br />

place ph<strong>on</strong>e calls by tapping pictures <strong>on</strong> the<br />

PDA screen <strong>and</strong> to see a picture of the pers<strong>on</strong><br />

calling them when they received a call. When<br />

compared to a mainstream Nokia cell ph<strong>on</strong>e,<br />

participants required fewer prompts <strong>and</strong><br />

made fewer errors when placing <strong>and</strong> receiving<br />

calls.<br />

With the widespread availability <strong>and</strong> relatively<br />

inexpensive cost of cell ph<strong>on</strong>es, it may<br />

be time for the field of special educati<strong>on</strong> to<br />

look in-depth into cell ph<strong>on</strong>e use bey<strong>on</strong>d<br />

emergency applicati<strong>on</strong>s. Other features <strong>and</strong><br />

uses identified by Bryen et al. (2007) were:<br />

paging <strong>and</strong> text messaging, c<strong>on</strong>necting to the<br />

internet, use of voice recogniti<strong>on</strong> capabilities,<br />

speed dialing, voice mail opti<strong>on</strong>s, transmitting<br />

computer files, taking digital photographs,<br />

<strong>and</strong> video-calling. Incorporating these features<br />

into a cell ph<strong>on</strong>e is now recognized<br />

within the realm of smartph<strong>on</strong>es. Smartph<strong>on</strong>es<br />

are electr<strong>on</strong>ic h<strong>and</strong>held devices that<br />

integrate the functi<strong>on</strong>ality of a mobile ph<strong>on</strong>e,<br />

pers<strong>on</strong>al digital assistant (PDA), or other informati<strong>on</strong><br />

appliances to offer features bey<strong>on</strong>d<br />

making voice calls (www.en.wikti<strong>on</strong>ary.org/<br />

wiki/smartph<strong>on</strong>e). Smartph<strong>on</strong>es include devices<br />

such as BlackBerry, Razr, iPh<strong>on</strong>e <strong>and</strong><br />

Palm Treo (www.sag.org/c<strong>on</strong>tent/new-mediaglossary).<br />

Smartph<strong>on</strong>es may also come equipped with<br />

built-in cameras <strong>and</strong> screens for visual, realtime<br />

communicati<strong>on</strong>s. In the early 1990s<br />

AT&T introduced its VideoPh<strong>on</strong>e, but the<br />

b<strong>and</strong>width limitati<strong>on</strong> of dial-up ph<strong>on</strong>e lines,<br />

high cost of entry ($1,000 each), <strong>and</strong> require-<br />

Figure 2. Videoph<strong>on</strong>e (Avaya Nortel 1535 IP<br />

model). Retrieved from http://en.wikipedia.<br />

org/wiki/Videoph<strong>on</strong>e<br />

ments for both communicati<strong>on</strong> parties to own<br />

the videoph<strong>on</strong>es, prevented them from taking<br />

off (www.answers.com/topic/videoph<strong>on</strong>e-1).<br />

High-speed cable <strong>and</strong> DSL allowed videoph<strong>on</strong>ing<br />

to eventually become popular by using<br />

a computer <strong>and</strong> specialized software. In<br />

2006 Skype popularized the use of videoph<strong>on</strong>es<br />

by offering free worldwide calling<br />

(www.answers.com/topic/videoph<strong>on</strong>e-1).<br />

In additi<strong>on</strong> to computer-based systems, videoph<strong>on</strong>es<br />

are also available in desktop or l<strong>and</strong><br />

line models which provide video <strong>and</strong> audio<br />

transmissi<strong>on</strong>s for communicati<strong>on</strong> between<br />

people in real-time. These videoph<strong>on</strong>es are<br />

currently popular am<strong>on</strong>g deaf pers<strong>on</strong>s who<br />

use them with sign language <strong>and</strong> am<strong>on</strong>g pers<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with limited mobility (www.answers.com/<br />

topic/videoph<strong>on</strong>e) (Figure 2).<br />

Video calling <strong>and</strong> downloading multimedia<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tent <strong>on</strong> mobile smartph<strong>on</strong>es are currently<br />

available with models such as the S<strong>on</strong>y-Ericss<strong>on</strong><br />

K800 (www.en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Videoph<strong>on</strong>e)<br />

(Figure 3) <strong>and</strong> the newly released<br />

iPh<strong>on</strong>e 4 which allows video chats using a<br />

feature called FaceTime (www.apple.com/<br />

iph<strong>on</strong>e/features/facetime.html) (Figure 4).<br />

Renblad (1999) reported the positive results<br />

of using early picture teleph<strong>on</strong>es <strong>and</strong><br />

video teleph<strong>on</strong>es (videoteleph<strong>on</strong>y) to increase<br />

communicati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> social interacti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

am<strong>on</strong>g pers<strong>on</strong>s with intellectual disabilities. In<br />

his review of the literature of studies c<strong>on</strong>-<br />

Twenty-First Century Portable Electr<strong>on</strong>ic Devices / 487


Figure 3. S<strong>on</strong>y-Ericss<strong>on</strong> K800. Retrieved from http://<br />

www.s<strong>on</strong>yericss<strong>on</strong>.com/cws/products/<br />

mobileph<strong>on</strong>es/overview/k800i<br />

ducted in Europe, these technologies were<br />

reported as beneficial for assisting pers<strong>on</strong>s in<br />

making c<strong>on</strong>tacts outside of their work place<br />

<strong>and</strong> home which might otherwise be difficult<br />

to make. Yet, at the time of this review, presenting<br />

video in a portable cell ph<strong>on</strong>e format,<br />

had not been researched as a tool for independence<br />

by pers<strong>on</strong>s with moderate ID or<br />

ASD. Future research needs to investigate the<br />

applicati<strong>on</strong> of portable electr<strong>on</strong>ic devices with<br />

video capability with pers<strong>on</strong>s with moderate<br />

Figure 4. iPh<strong>on</strong>e 4. Retrieved from http://www.<br />

apple.com/iph<strong>on</strong>e/features/facetime.html<br />

ID <strong>and</strong> ASD. When re-examining the research<br />

of Taber et al. (2002, 2003) in which students<br />

had difficult verbally describing their locati<strong>on</strong><br />

when lost, it appears that incorporating video<br />

<strong>on</strong> cell ph<strong>on</strong>es would allow students to video<br />

record <strong>and</strong> send visual images of their locati<strong>on</strong><br />

(to the pers<strong>on</strong> trying to locate them)<br />

which would provide an important applicati<strong>on</strong><br />

for this technology.<br />

H<strong>and</strong>held Computers <strong>and</strong> H<strong>and</strong>held Video<br />

Players<br />

H<strong>and</strong>held computers, often referred to today<br />

as PDAs (pers<strong>on</strong>al digital assistants), are also<br />

known as palmtop computers <strong>and</strong> Pocket PCs.<br />

These PDAs have been around since the beginning<br />

of the eighties, but have <strong>on</strong>ly become<br />

popular in the last few years due to innovative<br />

technologies such as 3G mobile teleph<strong>on</strong>y<br />

<strong>and</strong> wireless c<strong>on</strong>nectivity (Nashville, 2009).<br />

These features provide PDAs with the ability<br />

to do many of the things that a PC can do,<br />

such as c<strong>on</strong>necting to the internet, running<br />

third party applicati<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>and</strong> serving as a mobile<br />

ph<strong>on</strong>e (Nashville). In the field of special<br />

educati<strong>on</strong>, PDAs may provide digital c<strong>on</strong>tent<br />

(i.e., pictures <strong>and</strong> video) in flexible formats<br />

that can be made meaningful to individual<br />

students with disabilities (Abell, Bauder, Simm<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

& Shar<strong>on</strong>, 2003). In their meta-analysis<br />

of single-subject design studies, Wehmeyer et<br />

al. (2008) reported palmtop computers to be<br />

a simple <strong>and</strong> effective use of technology for<br />

prompting pers<strong>on</strong>s with intellectual disabilities.<br />

They suggested that these devices are<br />

highly effective in their use of cognitive access<br />

features such as touch screens as well as their<br />

audio <strong>and</strong> video output <strong>and</strong> input capabilities.<br />

Further, their portability, relative affordability,<br />

customizati<strong>on</strong> features, <strong>and</strong> ability to store<br />

large amounts of data, provide a means to<br />

address the needs of pers<strong>on</strong>s with intellectual<br />

disabilities (Stock, Davies, Davies, & Wehmeyer,<br />

2006).<br />

In the current review, identificati<strong>on</strong> of studies<br />

using h<strong>and</strong>held computers <strong>and</strong> h<strong>and</strong>held<br />

video players were categorized as those using:<br />

(a) text, sound, <strong>and</strong> light; (b) picture cuing<br />

with <strong>and</strong> without voice recording; <strong>and</strong> (c)<br />

video recordings. Within those categories, the<br />

identified purposes of the devices were to<br />

functi<strong>on</strong> as: (a) reminders <strong>and</strong> tools for time<br />

488 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


management; (b) transiti<strong>on</strong> aids; <strong>and</strong> (c)<br />

prompts <strong>and</strong> models for completing multistep<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>al skills.<br />

PDA with text, sound, <strong>and</strong> light cues. PDAs,<br />

which were originally designed to provide<br />

electr<strong>on</strong>ic task organizati<strong>on</strong>, can now be programmed<br />

to include complex activity schedules<br />

whereby each task can be linked to a<br />

reminder alarm to prompt students to check<br />

their schedule (Gentry, Wallace, Kvarfordt, &<br />

Lynch, 2010). The basic features of text,<br />

sound, <strong>and</strong> flashing lights have been used in<br />

this capacity to remind students to complete<br />

tasks. Gentry et al. used a quasi-experimental,<br />

pre- <strong>and</strong> post-assessment design to evaluate<br />

the ability of 22 high school students with ASD<br />

to enter appointments, schedules, <strong>and</strong> assignments<br />

<strong>on</strong>to a Palm Zire 31 PDA with a reminder<br />

alarm linked to each entry. Results<br />

showed that students were able to use the PDA<br />

as an electr<strong>on</strong>ic task management tool <strong>and</strong> to<br />

maintain their ability to use the cognitive aid<br />

over an eight-week period following a brief<br />

training period.<br />

Davies, Stock, <strong>and</strong> Wehmeyer (2002a) used<br />

specially designed software, Schedule Assistant<br />

(AbleLink Technologies), as a time management<br />

tool that operated <strong>on</strong> a Windows CE<br />

palmtop computer platform. The program<br />

provided an auditory beep to cue 12 students<br />

with intellectual disabilities (IQ scores ranged<br />

from 45–90) to check their PDA schedule followed<br />

by a recorded auditory cue telling them<br />

what task to perform. When compared to a<br />

traditi<strong>on</strong>al written schedule, results dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

that the palmtop computer with schedule<br />

prompting software was more effective<br />

than the written schedule for prompting initiati<strong>on</strong><br />

of tasks.<br />

Twenty students with intellectual disabilities<br />

were included in a comparis<strong>on</strong> study which<br />

evaluated use of a written time <strong>and</strong> task list,<br />

paper planner, <strong>and</strong> PDA with students with<br />

mild to moderate ID (Gillette & DePompei,<br />

2008). Students resp<strong>on</strong>ded with the highest<br />

rate of <strong>on</strong>-time behavior using a 1-Dell Axim<br />

<strong>and</strong> a 2-Palm Zire 71 or 72 PDA with an alarm<br />

functi<strong>on</strong> compared to a list or planner. The<br />

alarm functi<strong>on</strong> served as an effective reminder<br />

to prompt students to read the written message<br />

<strong>on</strong> the PDA screen which indicated the<br />

task to be completed. The researchers attributed<br />

the positive results to the audible “beep”<br />

provided by the PDA since each system was<br />

comprised of a similar list of assigned tasks.<br />

In light of these positive results using the<br />

more basic features of a PDA, future research<br />

may want to include evaluati<strong>on</strong> of alarming or<br />

auditory signaling features as reminders for<br />

additi<strong>on</strong>al types of tasks from those which<br />

have been evaluated. These may include selfmanagement<br />

of health <strong>and</strong> safety issues such<br />

as reminders to brush <strong>on</strong>e’s teeth (O’Hara,<br />

Seafriff-Curtin, Levitz, Davies, & Stock, 2008),<br />

checking the lock <strong>on</strong> the fr<strong>on</strong>t door of an<br />

apartment, or turning off the kitchen stove.<br />

Individuals with moderate ID or ASD may<br />

have the ability to complete these tasks, but<br />

may require a reminder to do so. For many,<br />

the inability to remember to complete such<br />

tasks may prohibit them from participating in<br />

less restrictive living <strong>and</strong> work situati<strong>on</strong>s or<br />

require external reminders to be delivered by<br />

other adults.<br />

PDA with picture cues. Digital c<strong>on</strong>tent, in<br />

the form of pictures, can also be incorporated<br />

<strong>on</strong>to h<strong>and</strong>held devices <strong>and</strong> have primarily<br />

been used to provide step-by-step instructi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for performing multi-step functi<strong>on</strong>al skills that<br />

may be new or difficult for the learner (i.e.,<br />

operating a washing machine) <strong>and</strong> tasks<br />

which are performed infrequently (i.e., baking<br />

a birthday cake). With these systems, informati<strong>on</strong><br />

is presented <strong>on</strong> a single picture <strong>and</strong><br />

the student performs the step based <strong>on</strong> the<br />

informati<strong>on</strong> provided in the picture before<br />

advancing the system to the next picture<br />

(step) in the task sequence.<br />

Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, O’Reily, Seedhouse, Furniss, <strong>and</strong><br />

Cunha (2000) found that six students with<br />

intellectual disabilities correctly performed a<br />

greater number of cleaning, food preparati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> table setting steps when pictures<br />

were presented <strong>on</strong> an IBM 110 palm-top computer<br />

compared to a card-based picture system.<br />

The researchers further found that task<br />

steps could be clustered into fewer pictures as<br />

students’ task performances improved. One<br />

reas<strong>on</strong> attributed to the differences in performance<br />

was the ease of navigati<strong>on</strong> with the<br />

PDA (pushing <strong>on</strong>e butt<strong>on</strong> to advance the program)<br />

compared to physical manipulati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

the card system.<br />

Cihak, Wright, <strong>and</strong> Ayres (2010) used selfmodeling<br />

static picture prompts via an HP<br />

iPAQ Mobile Media Compani<strong>on</strong> h<strong>and</strong>held<br />

Twenty-First Century Portable Electr<strong>on</strong>ic Devices / 489


computer with three middle school students<br />

diagnosed with high-functi<strong>on</strong>ing autism. Different<br />

photographs showing the individual<br />

student modeling task engagement (i.e., writing,<br />

reading) were downloaded <strong>on</strong>to Power-<br />

Point slides so that <strong>on</strong>e photograph was displayed<br />

every 30 sec<strong>on</strong>ds <strong>and</strong> the program<br />

advanced automatically during the class period.<br />

In additi<strong>on</strong> to use of the PDA, students<br />

self-recorded their task engagement <strong>on</strong> a 3x5<br />

inch index card as each new picture appeared.<br />

Results supported prompts delivered by the<br />

h<strong>and</strong>held computer <strong>and</strong> self-recording by students<br />

for increasing task engagement <strong>and</strong> decreasing<br />

teacher directed prompts. Students<br />

were further able to generalize use of the<br />

system across general educati<strong>on</strong> classrooms<br />

(i.e., math, science, language arts).<br />

Picture-based PDA systems can also incorporate<br />

voice recordings to provide additi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

informati<strong>on</strong> to that provided by the visual pictures.<br />

Students typically touch the picture or a<br />

butt<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the PDA to hear a verbal descripti<strong>on</strong><br />

of how to perform the step. Davies, Stock,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Wehmeyer (2002b) provided informati<strong>on</strong><br />

to students using picture voice recording<br />

format. They evaluated the software program,<br />

Visual Assistant (AbleLink Technologies),<br />

which ran <strong>on</strong> a Windows CE platform of a<br />

h<strong>and</strong>held computer. Ten adults with intellectual<br />

disabilities (Mean IQ 54.8) viewed stepby-step<br />

pictures al<strong>on</strong>g with verbal instructi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

as they completed steps for assembling pizza<br />

boxes <strong>and</strong> packaging software. Students dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

improved task accuracy <strong>and</strong> decreased<br />

reliance <strong>on</strong> adult prompts across both<br />

tasks within a vocati<strong>on</strong>al setting. Using the<br />

same Visual Assistant program <strong>and</strong> a Cassiopeia<br />

TFT palmtop computer, Riffel et al.<br />

(2005) dem<strong>on</strong>strated the ability of four transiti<strong>on</strong>-age<br />

students with mild to moderate ID<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>e student with ASD to increase the<br />

number of steps they completed without external<br />

adult prompting (i.e., doing laundry,<br />

rolling silverware, <strong>and</strong> setting tables) <strong>and</strong> to<br />

decrease durati<strong>on</strong> time spent <strong>on</strong> each task.<br />

Davies <strong>and</strong> colleagues (2003) evaluated an<br />

additi<strong>on</strong>al software prototype, Pocket Compass<br />

(AbleLink Technologies), which incorporated<br />

decisi<strong>on</strong> points into a picture audio<br />

prompting system. The software applicati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

which operated <strong>on</strong> a Pocket PC palmtop computer<br />

platform, provided multiple pictures <strong>on</strong><br />

Figure 5. Pocket Compass. AbleLink Technogies.<br />

the screen <strong>and</strong> audio instructi<strong>on</strong>s which corresp<strong>on</strong>ded<br />

to different opti<strong>on</strong>s available to the<br />

student (i.e., different colors of CDs to put<br />

into a box) (Figure 5). When the student<br />

touched <strong>on</strong>e of the decisi<strong>on</strong> point pictures,<br />

the program advanced to the corresp<strong>on</strong>ding<br />

sequence of pictures <strong>and</strong> auditory cues. Forty<br />

transiti<strong>on</strong>-age students with intellectual disabilities<br />

(mean IQ 55.53) participated in<br />

the beta test <strong>and</strong> increased their independence<br />

<strong>and</strong> accuracy in completing assembly<br />

tasks within a vocati<strong>on</strong>al setting as well as their<br />

ability to navigate decisi<strong>on</strong> points.<br />

Cihak, Kessler, <strong>and</strong> Alberto (2007; 2008)<br />

c<strong>on</strong>ducted two similar studies to evaluate use<br />

of picture <strong>and</strong> auditory prompts via a PDA to<br />

prompt independent task completi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

transiti<strong>on</strong>s by students with moderate intellectual<br />

disabilities. Each study presented the vi-<br />

490 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


sual <strong>and</strong> auditory cues <strong>on</strong> an Axim 30 h<strong>and</strong>held<br />

computer by Dell. In the first study four<br />

students completed four separate communitybased<br />

tasks (i.e., stocking milk, making subrolls)<br />

with each subsequent task increasing in<br />

the number of steps required for completi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

The PDA, used in c<strong>on</strong>juncti<strong>on</strong> with a least-tomost<br />

prompting system delivered by the instructor,<br />

was an effective tool for delivering<br />

prompts to students <strong>and</strong> task performance was<br />

maintained up to 9 weeks. In the sec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

study, four of the same tasks were used from<br />

the first study, al<strong>on</strong>g with six additi<strong>on</strong>al tasks,<br />

to evaluate independent transiti<strong>on</strong>s between<br />

tasks by four students with moderate to severe<br />

intellectual disabilities. Similar results indicated<br />

that the h<strong>and</strong>held prompting system<br />

was an effective tool for increasing independent<br />

task transiti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> skills were <strong>on</strong>ce<br />

again maintained for up to 9 weeks.<br />

In summary, although similar to picturebased<br />

booklets, the presentati<strong>on</strong> of pictures<br />

<strong>on</strong> electr<strong>on</strong>ic PDAs, may provide a more efficient<br />

<strong>and</strong> effective means for delivering<br />

prompts whereby students may find the traditi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

form of presentati<strong>on</strong> to be more cumbersome<br />

to manipulate <strong>and</strong> may lose their<br />

place in the sequence (Lanci<strong>on</strong>i et al., 2000).<br />

Electr<strong>on</strong>ic picture-based systems with <strong>and</strong><br />

without voice recordings were both found to<br />

be effective in this review. In light of research<br />

which indicates that students with ASD may be<br />

str<strong>on</strong>ger visual than auditory learners (Quill,<br />

1995; West, 2008), what remains unanswered<br />

is whether it is necessary to include voice recordings<br />

in c<strong>on</strong>juncti<strong>on</strong> with pictures. In additi<strong>on</strong><br />

to examining the need for voice recordings,<br />

future research should also c<strong>on</strong>tinue the<br />

line of investigati<strong>on</strong> initiated by Lanci<strong>on</strong>i et al.<br />

c<strong>on</strong>cerning clustering multiple pictures into<br />

fewer pictures (2000) <strong>and</strong> clustering individual<br />

verbal prompts of steps into l<strong>on</strong>ger<br />

streams of auditory recordings (2001) as tasks<br />

are acquired. Research will need to examine<br />

not <strong>on</strong>ly students’ abilities to use these features,<br />

but the flexibility of systems for making<br />

these adjustments with regards to preparati<strong>on</strong><br />

time. The line of research initiated by Davies<br />

et al. (2003) into the capabilities of PDAs to<br />

functi<strong>on</strong> in a n<strong>on</strong>-linear format also warrants<br />

more attenti<strong>on</strong>. With such programming, pictures<br />

<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>e screen can be linked to a different<br />

sequence of pictures depending <strong>on</strong> the<br />

choice or decisi<strong>on</strong> made by the user (i.e.,<br />

which recipe to cook, laundry load size for a<br />

washing machine).<br />

PDA with video cues <strong>and</strong> h<strong>and</strong>held video players.<br />

In a recent literature review of assistive technology<br />

devices (including PDAs) used as selfmanagement<br />

tools for prompting students<br />

with ID, no studies were available (through<br />

2005) evaluating the use of video presented<br />

<strong>on</strong> h<strong>and</strong>helds (Mechling, 2007). The current<br />

review identified eight studies between the<br />

years 2007 <strong>and</strong> 2010 addressing use of video<br />

modeling or prompting presented <strong>on</strong> portable<br />

h<strong>and</strong>held devices as interventi<strong>on</strong> tools for<br />

students with moderate ID <strong>and</strong> ASD.<br />

Video modeling, watching an entire video<br />

recording dem<strong>on</strong>strating how to perform a<br />

task prior to completing the task, was used in<br />

three of the eight identified studies utilizing<br />

video <strong>on</strong> h<strong>and</strong>held devices. Van Laarhoven,<br />

Van Laarhoven-Myers, <strong>and</strong> Zurita (2007)<br />

worked with two high school aged males with<br />

mild <strong>and</strong> moderate intellectual disabilities in<br />

two employment settings (Red Robin <strong>and</strong><br />

Applebee’s) using video modeling presented<br />

<strong>on</strong> an HP iPAQ hg2700 series Pocket PC. Each<br />

student completed three different tasks (i.e.,<br />

rolling silverware, porti<strong>on</strong>ing recipes) using<br />

video modeling <strong>and</strong> video feedback (re-watching<br />

the video after errors occurred) in c<strong>on</strong>juncti<strong>on</strong><br />

with a least-to-most prompting system<br />

delivered by the instructor. Both students<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strated an increase in independent resp<strong>on</strong>ding<br />

<strong>and</strong> a decrease in external adult<br />

prompting while using the device.<br />

Taber-Doughty, Patt<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> Brennan<br />

(2008) used a 30GB Apple video iPod (Figure<br />

6) to deliver simultaneous prompting in a<br />

library whereby students watched an entire<br />

video task chain, with audio instructi<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

while simultaneously completing each step.<br />

This format was used for locating books <strong>and</strong><br />

DVDs <strong>and</strong> using a computer to obtain call<br />

numbers by three middle school students with<br />

moderate intellectual disabilities. This study<br />

compared simultaneous video modeling <strong>and</strong><br />

delayed video modeling in which a student<br />

watched a video model at least <strong>on</strong>e hour prior<br />

to traveling to the library <strong>and</strong> locating the<br />

items. The delayed video modeling was presented<br />

<strong>on</strong> a VCR/DVD player. Both systems<br />

were found to be effective <strong>and</strong> students were<br />

able to generalize the skills to a sec<strong>on</strong>d library,<br />

Twenty-First Century Portable Electr<strong>on</strong>ic Devices / 491


Figure 6. Video iPod. Apple, Inc.<br />

however, when using the video iPod with simultaneous<br />

prompting, acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

of skills was slightly greater for two<br />

students for the computer task <strong>and</strong> for all<br />

three students when locating DVDs <strong>and</strong><br />

books. Of further interest was the preference<br />

of two of the students for using the video iPod.<br />

A video iPod, with video modeling was also<br />

used by Cihak, Fahrenkrog, Ayres, <strong>and</strong> Smith<br />

(2010) to increase independent transiti<strong>on</strong> behaviors<br />

of four elementary students diagnosed<br />

with ASD within a general educati<strong>on</strong> classroom.<br />

A combinati<strong>on</strong> of video self-modeling<br />

without audio (video clips of the student as<br />

the model lining up, walking in the hallway,<br />

<strong>and</strong> entering the next area of the school) <strong>and</strong><br />

pers<strong>on</strong> first point-of-view (students’ vantage<br />

point as if they were walking to the next area)<br />

were used to create the video models of ten<br />

daily transiti<strong>on</strong>s. At the beginning of each<br />

transiti<strong>on</strong> the student watched the video<br />

model <strong>on</strong> the video iPod followed by a teacher<br />

prompt to “line up” <strong>and</strong> begin the transiti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Students experienced an increase in independent<br />

transiti<strong>on</strong>s when using the h<strong>and</strong>held<br />

video device paired with a least-to-most<br />

prompting system delivered by the instructor<br />

<strong>and</strong> descending trends in performance when<br />

the device was removed.<br />

A third study also used a video iPod, but<br />

compared to the previously reviewed studies,<br />

the researchers used video prompting <strong>and</strong><br />

feedback <strong>on</strong> the device rather than video<br />

modeling (Van Laarhoven, Johns<strong>on</strong>, Van<br />

Laarhoven-Myers, Grider & Grider, 2009).<br />

Video prompting requires the user to watch<br />

<strong>on</strong>e video segment <strong>on</strong> the device, complete<br />

the corresp<strong>on</strong>ding step, return to the device,<br />

watch the next video segment in the task sequence<br />

<strong>and</strong> so forth. Van Laarhoven <strong>and</strong> others<br />

used video prompting <strong>and</strong> feedback to<br />

present steps to a 17-year-old young man for<br />

completing work-related tasks in a no-kill animal<br />

shelter. Compared to the Van Laarhoven<br />

et al. (2007) study, the participant referred<br />

back to individual video segments (rather<br />

than the entire video) when an error occurred<br />

<strong>on</strong> a particular step The program also c<strong>on</strong>tained<br />

still photographs at the beginning of<br />

each video clip to present critical features of<br />

steps <strong>and</strong> a title screen at the end of each<br />

video clip to prompt the student to stop the<br />

device <strong>and</strong> complete the step. Voice over narrati<strong>on</strong><br />

was also uploaded to the video iPod.<br />

Introducti<strong>on</strong> of the video-based materials resulted<br />

in an increase in correct resp<strong>on</strong>ding by<br />

the participant across three job-related tasks<br />

<strong>and</strong> a decrease in adult prompting for error<br />

correcti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> use of the device.<br />

In the final three studies identified in this<br />

review, PDAs were used to present multiple<br />

media opti<strong>on</strong>s to the users including: voice<br />

recordings, digital photographs, <strong>and</strong> digital<br />

video. Unlike the other studies reviewed<br />

which incorporated video <strong>on</strong>to h<strong>and</strong>held devices,<br />

each of these studies used a specialized<br />

assistive technology device rather than a mainstream,<br />

commercially available product. The<br />

studies used a Cyrano Communicator TM<br />

(Kiba Technologies, LLC.) which was originally<br />

designed as a portable augmentative<br />

communicati<strong>on</strong> device. The device operated<br />

using specially designed software <strong>on</strong> a commercially<br />

available PDA (Hewlett Packard<br />

iPAQ Pocket PC or Pidi<strong>on</strong> BM-150R) with<br />

multimedia features that allowed the user to<br />

492 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


access pictures, video, text, <strong>and</strong> audio all <strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>on</strong>e screen. In each of the three studies the<br />

PDA was used for prompting step-by-step completi<strong>on</strong><br />

of multi-step functi<strong>on</strong>al skills. Students<br />

could look at a picture to receive informati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

repeatedly touch a picture <strong>on</strong> the<br />

screen <strong>and</strong> hear a voice recording, touch a<br />

video ic<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> play a video recording, or advance<br />

the program to subsequent screens<br />

(task steps). Using these multiple prompting<br />

features students could choose <strong>and</strong> self-adjust<br />

the level of prompting delivered by the device<br />

for each step of the task. Unlike the studies<br />

previously reviewed in this secti<strong>on</strong>, no external<br />

adult prompting (i.e., least-to-most<br />

prompting system) was provided for task completi<strong>on</strong><br />

although the instructors did provide<br />

prompts for use of the device.<br />

The first study evaluated the effects of multiple<br />

prompt levels <strong>on</strong> the independent preparati<strong>on</strong><br />

of recipes by three high school age<br />

students with moderate intellectual disabilities<br />

(Mechling, Gast, & Seid, 2010). Immediate<br />

<strong>and</strong> abrupt increases in the percentage of<br />

steps completed independently were dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

by each of the participants following<br />

introducti<strong>on</strong> of the PDA system <strong>and</strong> performance<br />

was maintained over time. Results also<br />

indicated that participants initially used more<br />

intrusive levels of prompts <strong>and</strong> self-faded<br />

these levels of prompts (i.e., video to photos)<br />

<strong>and</strong> later reinstated use of more intrusive<br />

prompt levels, as needed, during maintenance<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

In resp<strong>on</strong>se to the Mechling et al. (2010)<br />

study, a sec<strong>on</strong>d study was implemented in order<br />

to evaluate the effects of the PDA procedure<br />

<strong>on</strong> the same food preparati<strong>on</strong> tasks, but<br />

with three high school age students with ASD<br />

(Mechling, Gast, & Seid, 2009). Results replicated<br />

those of the previous study in support of<br />

a PDA with video, pictures, <strong>and</strong> auditory<br />

prompts as a self-prompting device to assist<br />

students with ASD to perform multi-step tasks.<br />

Differences did exist between the two studies<br />

in regards to the levels of prompts used by<br />

students with ASD. Although they also selfadjusted<br />

the use of prompt levels, overall their<br />

tendency was to c<strong>on</strong>tinue to rely <strong>on</strong> prompts<br />

from the PDA within <strong>and</strong> across recipes even<br />

though they were able to complete the recipes<br />

independently when the PDA was removed.<br />

In a third study Mechling <strong>and</strong> Seid (2011)<br />

Figure 7. Cyrano Communicator TM. Kiba Technologies,<br />

LLC.<br />

evaluated the effectiveness of the PDA with<br />

multiple prompt levels to prompt independent<br />

pedestrian travel by three transiti<strong>on</strong> age<br />

students with moderate intellectual disabilities.<br />

Similar to a global positi<strong>on</strong>ing system<br />

(GPS), students who were unable to read<br />

maps or text used photographs <strong>and</strong> video recordings<br />

of l<strong>and</strong>marks al<strong>on</strong>g the routes to<br />

independently reach three different destinati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<strong>on</strong> a college campus (Figure 7). Similar<br />

to the other studies using multiple prompt<br />

levels <strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>e screen, the three students selfadjusted<br />

their use of prompt levels as they<br />

became more familiar with each route.<br />

Results of this review <strong>on</strong> the use of h<strong>and</strong>held<br />

computers with students with moderate<br />

ID <strong>and</strong> ASD dem<strong>on</strong>strate that these small portable<br />

systems provide some definite advantages<br />

for individuals, including portability. For<br />

Twenty-First Century Portable Electr<strong>on</strong>ic Devices / 493


example, in c<strong>on</strong>trast to previous studies examining<br />

video <strong>and</strong> transiti<strong>on</strong>s, students in the<br />

Cihak et al. (2010) study did not have to return<br />

to a “home base” in order to view the<br />

video <strong>on</strong> a televisi<strong>on</strong> or computer screen. Instead,<br />

the portable device moved with the students<br />

across envir<strong>on</strong>ments. Similarly, in the<br />

Mechling <strong>and</strong> Seid (2011) <strong>and</strong> Van Laarhoven<br />

et al. (2009) studies, students were able to<br />

walk with the PDA while locating destinati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<strong>and</strong> completing job tasks within an animal<br />

shelter.<br />

Although the results have all been positive<br />

when using PDAs with different presentati<strong>on</strong><br />

modes (i.e., picture, video), accessibility issues<br />

remain with these mainstream devices. Stock<br />

et al. (2006) identified <strong>and</strong> affectively addressed<br />

some of these barriers by developing<br />

<strong>and</strong> evaluating a simplified multimedia software<br />

system, Pocket Voyager (AbleLink Technologies)<br />

for use with a PDA. With their prototype,<br />

they created: (a) oversized ic<strong>on</strong>s to<br />

address the issue of physical access with small<br />

ic<strong>on</strong>s; (b) digital pictures for identificati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tacts in the address book for pers<strong>on</strong>s who<br />

had difficulty reading text; (c) audio messages<br />

to assist underst<strong>and</strong>ing of what applicati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

were represented by each ic<strong>on</strong>; (d) recorded<br />

audio files for reading ph<strong>on</strong>e numbers in the<br />

address book; <strong>and</strong> (e) numbers in larger f<strong>on</strong>ts<br />

for pers<strong>on</strong>s who had difficulty recognizing<br />

<strong>and</strong> dialing numbers. They further identified<br />

the problems pers<strong>on</strong>s have with complex PDA<br />

operating systems <strong>and</strong> provided greater c<strong>on</strong>sistency<br />

across steps for starting different applicati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

in the system. When compared to<br />

use of a st<strong>and</strong>ard Windows CE operating system<br />

<strong>and</strong> Pocket PC, 32 participants with intellectual<br />

disabilities (mean IQ score 56.1) required<br />

fewer prompts <strong>and</strong> committed fewer<br />

errors when using the specially designed software<br />

program.<br />

In additi<strong>on</strong> to access when using h<strong>and</strong>held<br />

computers, further research <strong>and</strong> development<br />

should c<strong>on</strong>tinue to investigate the following:<br />

● use of video prompting compared to video<br />

modeling <strong>on</strong> portable h<strong>and</strong>held devices.<br />

● comparative effects of different systems<br />

(i.e., picture-based <strong>and</strong> video-based systems;<br />

self-operated auditory prompting <strong>and</strong> video<br />

prompting) (Taber-Doughty et al., 2008).<br />

● importance of verbal/voice over recordings<br />

when using picture-based <strong>and</strong> video-based<br />

systems (Rayner, Denholm, & Sigafoos,<br />

2009).<br />

● effects of h<strong>and</strong>held devices <strong>on</strong> different<br />

types of task (i.e., fine motor compared to<br />

gross motor) (Furniss et al., 1999).<br />

● provisi<strong>on</strong> of high-tech h<strong>and</strong>held systems<br />

during acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of skills followed by use<br />

of light-tech (picture or auditory) systems<br />

during review or c<strong>on</strong>tinuous performance<br />

of skills.<br />

● provisi<strong>on</strong> of multiple prompt levels (text,<br />

audio, picture, <strong>and</strong> video) <strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>e device or<br />

<strong>on</strong>e screen of a device.<br />

● ability of users to self-adjust the prompt levels<br />

used <strong>on</strong> devices <strong>and</strong> development of systems<br />

that permit this adjustment (Van Laarhoven<br />

& Van Laarhoven-Myers, 2006).<br />

● effects of clustering multiple pictures of<br />

steps into fewer pictures (Lanci<strong>on</strong>i et al.,<br />

2000), lengthening or shortening verbal recordings<br />

(Lanci<strong>on</strong>i et al., 2001), <strong>and</strong> adjusting<br />

the length of video recordings (Cannella-Mal<strong>on</strong>e<br />

et al., 2006) as tasks are<br />

learned <strong>and</strong> repeated.<br />

● use of wide screen or zoom shots with photographs<br />

<strong>and</strong> videos when presenting different<br />

comp<strong>on</strong>ents of tasks (Van Laarhoven et<br />

al., 2007).<br />

● comparative effects of screen size <strong>and</strong> images<br />

for delivering informati<strong>on</strong> through pictures<br />

<strong>and</strong> video (Stock et al., 2008).<br />

Discussi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Recommendati<strong>on</strong>s for Future<br />

Research<br />

Research into the potential benefits of portable<br />

electr<strong>on</strong>ic technologies for pers<strong>on</strong>s with a<br />

diagnosis of moderate intellectual disabilities<br />

<strong>and</strong> autism spectrum disorders is in its initial<br />

stages. Studies to date support the use of these<br />

technologies across envir<strong>on</strong>ments including<br />

work, school, <strong>and</strong> community settings <strong>and</strong><br />

across skills including functi<strong>on</strong>al multi-step<br />

skills, transiti<strong>on</strong>ing between tasks <strong>and</strong> envir<strong>on</strong>ments,<br />

<strong>and</strong> time <strong>and</strong> task management.<br />

In spite of the positive results reported in<br />

the studies reviewed, pers<strong>on</strong>s with intellectual<br />

disabilities have traditi<strong>on</strong>ally accessed cutting<br />

edge technologies far less often than those<br />

without disabilities (Carey et al., 2005). In<br />

their survey of 83 adults with intellectual disabilities,<br />

Carey <strong>and</strong> colleagues found that <strong>on</strong>ly<br />

494 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


41% used a computer, 25.3% used the Internet,<br />

27.7 used cell ph<strong>on</strong>es, <strong>and</strong> 10.8% used<br />

electr<strong>on</strong>ic organizers <strong>on</strong> a regular basis. They<br />

reported that primary barriers to use included<br />

lack of access, lack of training <strong>and</strong> support,<br />

<strong>and</strong> expense. They further found that age,<br />

employment status, <strong>and</strong> self-perceived ability<br />

to perform fine motor tasks, significantly affected<br />

use of these technologies. More specifically,<br />

younger pers<strong>on</strong>s used more technology;<br />

those employed in competitive employment<br />

<strong>and</strong> those unemployed used more technology<br />

than those in sheltered workshops: <strong>and</strong> pers<strong>on</strong>s<br />

who were manually able to copy informati<strong>on</strong><br />

(i.e., write/copy an address from a business<br />

card) used more technology.<br />

To address the issues of availability <strong>and</strong> expense,<br />

some researchers support the use of<br />

mainstream, generic devices that are designed<br />

for the general populati<strong>on</strong> in mass quantities<br />

(Cihak et al., 2008). In developing these generic<br />

devices, commercial producers are increasingly<br />

following the principles of universal<br />

design which allow accessibility to all users (as<br />

much as possible) without incorporati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

special designs or customizati<strong>on</strong> (Cihak et<br />

al.). Wehmeyer et al. (2008) recommend future<br />

research <strong>and</strong> development across a wide<br />

range of technologies, including newer electr<strong>on</strong>ic<br />

<strong>and</strong> informati<strong>on</strong> systems which employ<br />

aspects of universal design to determine their<br />

applicability to pers<strong>on</strong>s with intellectual disabilities.<br />

In additi<strong>on</strong> to being readily available<br />

<strong>and</strong> less expensive, mainstream h<strong>and</strong>held devices<br />

<strong>and</strong> ph<strong>on</strong>es are reported to be n<strong>on</strong>-stigmatizing<br />

means for providing assistance to<br />

pers<strong>on</strong>s with intellectual disabilities because<br />

they are predominantly used by the general<br />

public (Davies et al., 2002b; Gentry et al.,<br />

2010; Gillette & DePompei, 2008; Myles, Fergus<strong>on</strong>,<br />

& Hagiwara, 2007). Results of the reviewed<br />

studies further indicate that students<br />

like h<strong>and</strong>held devices <strong>and</strong> are motivated to<br />

use them (Cihak et al., 2010; Mechling & Seid,<br />

2011; Taber et al., 2003; Taber-Doughty et al.,<br />

2008; Van Laarhoven et al., 2007). However,<br />

the old saying, “<strong>on</strong>e size does not fit all” may<br />

also apply to the use of portable electr<strong>on</strong>ic<br />

devices. For many users, cognitive <strong>and</strong> physical<br />

accessibility are c<strong>on</strong>cerns <strong>and</strong> there c<strong>on</strong>tinues<br />

to be a need to modify mainstream<br />

software which operates portable electr<strong>on</strong>ic<br />

devices in order to increase their accessibility<br />

<strong>and</strong> use by pers<strong>on</strong>s with intellectual disabilities<br />

(Stock et al., 2006; 2008). Researchers<br />

may wish to evaluate commercial mainstream<br />

products such as the video iPod to determine<br />

if they are as effective as those made specifically<br />

for pers<strong>on</strong>s with disabilities (i.e., Ablelink<br />

Technologies, Kiba Technologies, LLC.) in<br />

delivering informati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> providing access.<br />

Other directi<strong>on</strong>s for future research center<br />

around the need to explore the applicati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

currently available features offered by PDAs<br />

<strong>and</strong> smartph<strong>on</strong>es (Gentry et al., 2010) These<br />

include:<br />

● text to speech features <strong>on</strong> portable devices<br />

so that informati<strong>on</strong> (i.e., emails, c<strong>on</strong>tact informati<strong>on</strong>)<br />

can be read to the user. F<strong>on</strong>ix<br />

VoiceCentral 3.1 (F<strong>on</strong>ix Speech, 2008) has<br />

built-in text-to-speech software that allows a<br />

Pocket PC to read informati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Voice-<br />

Central Black Swan, available for iPh<strong>on</strong>es<br />

(Riverturn, Inc., 2009–2010), has this capability.<br />

● voice recogniti<strong>on</strong> for operating applicati<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

making ph<strong>on</strong>e calls etc. Features are<br />

now being offered through products such as<br />

Drag<strong>on</strong> Pdsay (Nuance Communicati<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

Inc., 2010) which provides voice input as<br />

well as text to speech features.<br />

● video teleph<strong>on</strong>ing to increase skills <strong>and</strong> opportunities<br />

such as social, communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

(Renblad, 1999), <strong>and</strong> safety skills.<br />

● video playback <strong>on</strong> smartph<strong>on</strong>es to prompt<br />

task completi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

In additi<strong>on</strong> to these directi<strong>on</strong>s, it appears<br />

that special educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> related fields<br />

should also explore development of applicati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

that will afford the following:<br />

● use of electr<strong>on</strong>ic readers <strong>on</strong> a PDA or smartph<strong>on</strong>e<br />

which would allow instant photographs<br />

to be taken of text which could be<br />

c<strong>on</strong>verted to speech to allow ready access to<br />

print materials. This feature, which is available<br />

<strong>on</strong> the Intel Reader (Intel Corporati<strong>on</strong>)<br />

would allow pers<strong>on</strong>s with moderate ID<br />

or ASD to take a photograph in a community<br />

setting (i.e., street sign, grocery aisle)<br />

<strong>and</strong> have the informati<strong>on</strong> read to them.<br />

● simple to use GPS systems which provide<br />

pictorial, auditory, <strong>and</strong> video informati<strong>on</strong> to<br />

users with moderate intellectual disabilities<br />

who are walking (Mechling & Seid, 2011) or<br />

Twenty-First Century Portable Electr<strong>on</strong>ic Devices / 495


using public transportati<strong>on</strong>. Devices such as<br />

the Trekker Breeze (HumanWare, 2005–<br />

2009) are currently used by pers<strong>on</strong>s who are<br />

blind to provide auditory informati<strong>on</strong> about<br />

locati<strong>on</strong>s, directi<strong>on</strong>s, routes, <strong>and</strong> l<strong>and</strong>marks.<br />

While c<strong>on</strong>sidering these listed features, it<br />

appears relevant to individually evaluate them<br />

as independent variables as well as to evaluate<br />

them in c<strong>on</strong>juncti<strong>on</strong> with each other such as<br />

using a single device for prompting step-bystep<br />

task completi<strong>on</strong>, providing reminders to<br />

complete a task, <strong>and</strong> serving as a travel aid. It<br />

is important to recognize that future technologies<br />

that merge functi<strong>on</strong>s into <strong>on</strong>e device,<br />

mainstream or customized, will be made available<br />

so that students will have access to multiple<br />

features <strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>e device.<br />

Finally, while it is important to realize these<br />

portable electr<strong>on</strong>ic devices may not be appropriate<br />

for every<strong>on</strong>e, <strong>and</strong> that pers<strong>on</strong>s in more<br />

restrictive settings with fewer task dem<strong>and</strong>s<br />

may have less need for PDAs <strong>and</strong> smartph<strong>on</strong>es<br />

(DePompei et al., 2008), it is also possible that<br />

these innovati<strong>on</strong>s will create opportunities for<br />

access <strong>and</strong> engagement in living, work, <strong>and</strong><br />

recreati<strong>on</strong>al envir<strong>on</strong>ments that are currently<br />

not available to pers<strong>on</strong>s with more significant<br />

disabilities.<br />

References<br />

Abbell, M., Bauder, D., Simm<strong>on</strong>s, T., & Shar<strong>on</strong>, D.<br />

(2003). Using pers<strong>on</strong>al digital assistants (PDA) to<br />

c<strong>on</strong>nect students with special needs to the general<br />

curriculum. Closing the Gap, 22(1), 20.<br />

AbleLink Technologies. www.ablelinktech.com<br />

Ayres, K. M., & Lang<strong>on</strong>e, J. (2008). Video supports<br />

for teaching students with developmental disabilities<br />

<strong>and</strong> autism: Twenty-five years of research <strong>and</strong><br />

development. Journal of Special Educati<strong>on</strong> Technology,<br />

23(3), 1–8.<br />

Bryen, D. N., Carey, A., & Friedman, M. (2007). Cell<br />

ph<strong>on</strong>e use by adults with intellectual disabilities.<br />

Intellectual <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 45, 1–9.<br />

Cannella-Mal<strong>on</strong>e H., Sigafoos, J., O’Reilly, M., De<br />

La Cruz, B., Edrisinha, C., & Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, G. E.<br />

(2006). Comparing video prompting to video<br />

modeling for teaching daily livings skills to six<br />

adults with developmental disabilities. Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 41, 344–<br />

356.<br />

Carey, A. C., Friedman, M. G., & Bryen, D. N.<br />

(2005). Use of electr<strong>on</strong>ic technologies by people<br />

with intellectual disabilities. Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

43, 322–333.<br />

Cell Ph<strong>on</strong>es.org (2008). Cell ph<strong>on</strong>e history. Retrieved<br />

from www.cellph<strong>on</strong>es.org/cell-ph<strong>on</strong>e<br />

history.html <strong>on</strong> June 23, 2010.<br />

Cihak, D., Fahrenkrog, C., Ayres, K. M., & Smith, C.<br />

(2010). The use of video modeling via a video<br />

iPod <strong>and</strong> a system of least prompts to improve<br />

transiti<strong>on</strong>al behaviors for students with autism<br />

spectrum disorders in the general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

classroom. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

12, 103–115.<br />

Cihak, D. F., Kessler, K. B., & Alberto, P. A. (2007).<br />

Generalized use of a h<strong>and</strong>held prompting system.<br />

Research in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 28, 397–408.<br />

Cihak, D. F., Kessler, K. B., & Alberto, P. A. (2008).<br />

Use of a h<strong>and</strong>held prompting system to transiti<strong>on</strong><br />

independently through vocati<strong>on</strong>al tasks for students<br />

with moderate <strong>and</strong> severe intellectual disabilities.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 43, 102–110.<br />

Cihak, D. F., Wright, R., & Ayres, K. M. (2010). Use<br />

of self-modeling static-picture prompts via a h<strong>and</strong>held<br />

computer to facilitate self-m<strong>on</strong>itoring in the<br />

general educati<strong>on</strong> classroom. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training<br />

in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 45,<br />

136–149.<br />

Davies, D. K., Stock, S. E., & Wehmeyer, M. L.<br />

(2003). A palmtop computer-based intelligent aid<br />

for individuals with intellectual disabilities to increase<br />

independent decisi<strong>on</strong> making. Research &<br />

Practice for Pers<strong>on</strong>s with Severe <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 28, 182–<br />

193.<br />

Davies, D. K., Stock, S. E., & Wehmeyer, M. L.<br />

(2002a). Enhancing independent time-management<br />

skills to individuals with mental retardati<strong>on</strong><br />

using a palmtop pers<strong>on</strong>al computer. Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

5, 358–365.<br />

Davies, D. K., Stock, S. E., & Wehmeyer, M. L.<br />

(2002b). Enhancing independent task performance<br />

for individuals with mental retardati<strong>on</strong><br />

through use of a h<strong>and</strong>held self-directed visual<br />

<strong>and</strong> audio prompting system. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training<br />

in Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>,<br />

37, 209–218.<br />

DePompei, R., Gillette, Y., Goetz, E., Xenopoulos-<br />

Oddss<strong>on</strong>, A., Bryen, D., & Dowds, M. (2008).<br />

Practical applicati<strong>on</strong>s for use of PDAs <strong>and</strong> smartph<strong>on</strong>es<br />

with children <strong>and</strong> adolescents who have<br />

traumatic brain injury. NeuroRehabilitati<strong>on</strong>, 23,<br />

487–499.<br />

F<strong>on</strong>ix Speech. (2008). F<strong>on</strong>ix VoiceCentral 3.1. Retrieved<br />

from www.f<strong>on</strong>ixspeech.com June 26, 2010<br />

Furniss, F., Ward, A., Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, G., Rocha, N.,<br />

Cunha, B., Seedhouse, et al. (1999). A palmtopbased<br />

job aid for workers with severe intellectual<br />

disabilities. Technology <strong>and</strong> Disability, 10, 53–67.<br />

Gentry, T., Wallace, C., Kvarfordt, C., & Lynch, K. B.<br />

496 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


(2010). Pers<strong>on</strong>al digital assistants as cognitive aids<br />

for individuals with autism: Results of a community-based<br />

trial. Journal of Vocati<strong>on</strong>al Rehabilitati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

32, 101–107.<br />

Gentry, T., Wallace, C., Kvarfordt, C., & Lynch, K. B.<br />

(2008). Pers<strong>on</strong>al digital assistants as cognitive aids<br />

for individuals with severe traumatic brain injury:<br />

A community-based trial. Brain Injury, 22, 19–24.<br />

Gillette, Y., & DePompei, R. (2008). The potential<br />

of electr<strong>on</strong>ic organizers as a tool in the cognitive<br />

rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> of young people. NeuroRehabilitati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

19, 233–243.<br />

Hart, T., Buchhofer, R. & Vaccaro, M. (2004). Portable<br />

electr<strong>on</strong>ic devices as memory <strong>and</strong> organizati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

aids after traumatic brain injury: A c<strong>on</strong>sumer<br />

survey study. Journal of Head Trauma<br />

Rehabilitati<strong>on</strong>, 19, 351–356.<br />

Hart, T., O’Neil-Pirozzi, T., & Morita, C. (2003).<br />

Clinician expectati<strong>on</strong>s for portable electr<strong>on</strong>ic devices<br />

as cognitive-behavioural orthoses in traumatic<br />

brain injury rehabilitati<strong>on</strong>, Brain Injury, 17,<br />

401–411.<br />

HumanWare. (2005-2009). Trekker Breeze. L<strong>on</strong>gueuil,<br />

Canada.<br />

Intel Corporati<strong>on</strong>. Intel Reader. Retrieved from<br />

www.intel.com/healthcare/reader/users.htm June<br />

26, 2010.<br />

Kiba Technologies, LLC. Cyrano Communicator.<br />

Retrieved from www.cyranocommunicator.com<br />

June 26, 2010.<br />

Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, G. E., O’Reilly, M. F., Seedhouse, P.,<br />

Furniss, F., & Cunha. (2000). Promoting independent<br />

task performance by pers<strong>on</strong>s with severe<br />

developmental disabilities through a new computer-aided<br />

system. Behavior Modificati<strong>on</strong>, 24, 700–<br />

718.<br />

Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, G. E., O’Reilly, M. F., & Oliva, D. (2001).<br />

Self-operated verbal instructi<strong>on</strong>s for people with<br />

intellectual <strong>and</strong> visual disabilities: Using instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

cluster after task acquisiti<strong>on</strong>. Internati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Journal of Disability, Development <strong>and</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong>, 48,<br />

304–312.<br />

Mechling, L. C. (2007). Assistive technology as a<br />

self-management tool for prompting students<br />

with intellectual disabilities to initiate <strong>and</strong> complete<br />

daily tasks: A literature review. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Training in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 42, 252–269.<br />

Mechling, L. C., Gast, D. L., & Seid, N. H. (2010).<br />

Evaluati<strong>on</strong> of a pers<strong>on</strong>al digital assistant as a selfprompting<br />

device for increasing multi-step task<br />

completi<strong>on</strong> by students with moderate intellectual<br />

disabilities. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 45, 422–439.<br />

Mechling, L. C., Gast, D. L., & Seid, N. H. (2009).<br />

Using a pers<strong>on</strong>al digital assistant to increase independent<br />

task completi<strong>on</strong> by students with autism<br />

spectrum disorder. Journal of <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

Disorders, 39, 1420–1434.<br />

Mechling, L. C. & Seid, N. H. (2011). Use of a<br />

h<strong>and</strong>-held pers<strong>on</strong>al digital assistant (PDA) to selfprompt<br />

pedestrian travel by young adults with<br />

moderate intellectual disabilities. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>,<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>, 220–237.<br />

Millard, E. (2007). Learning with pers<strong>on</strong>al media<br />

players. District Administrati<strong>on</strong>, 43, 61–64.<br />

Myles, B. S., Fergus<strong>on</strong>, H., & Hagiwara, T. (2007).<br />

Using a pers<strong>on</strong>al digital assistant to improve the<br />

recording of homework assignments by an adolescent<br />

with Asperger syndrome. Focus <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Other <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 22, 96–99.<br />

Nashville, N. (2009). History of the palmtop computer,<br />

Artipot. Retrieved from www.artipot.<br />

com/articles/378059/the-history-of-the-palmtopcomputer.htm<br />

<strong>on</strong> June 23, 2010.<br />

Nuance Communicati<strong>on</strong>s, Inc. (2010). Drag<strong>on</strong> Pdsay.<br />

Retrieved from www.nuance.com <strong>on</strong> June 26,<br />

2010<br />

O’Hara, D. M., Seagriff-Curtin, P., Levitz, M., Davies,<br />

D., & Stock, S. (2008). Using pers<strong>on</strong>al digital<br />

assistants to improve self-care in oral health. Journal<br />

of Telemedicine <strong>and</strong> Telecare, 14, 150–151.<br />

Pine Technology, Ltd. D’music MP3 Player. Retrieved<br />

from www.chpine.com June 26, 2010.<br />

Post, M., & Storey, K. (2002). Review of using auditory<br />

prompting systems with pers<strong>on</strong>s who have<br />

moderate to severe disabilities. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Training in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 37, 317–327.<br />

Quill, K. A. (1995). Visually cued instructi<strong>on</strong> for<br />

children with autism <strong>and</strong> pervasive developmental<br />

disorders. Focus <strong>on</strong> Autistic Behavior, 10, 10–20.<br />

Rayner, C., Denholm, C., & Sigafoos, J. (2009). Video-based<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> for individuals with autism:<br />

Key questi<strong>on</strong>s that remain unanswered. Research<br />

in <strong>Autism</strong> Spectrum Disorders, 3, 291–303.<br />

Renblad, K. (1999). The potential for advanced<br />

technologies to broaden the outreach of social<br />

network of pers<strong>on</strong>s with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. A<br />

literature study. Technology <strong>and</strong> Disability, 10, 175–<br />

180.<br />

Riffel, L. A., Wehmeyer, M. L., Turnbull, A. P.,<br />

Davies, D., Stock, S. & Fisher, S. (2005). Promoting<br />

independent performance of transiti<strong>on</strong>-related<br />

tasks using a palmtop PC-based self-directed<br />

visual <strong>and</strong> auditory prompting system. Journal of<br />

Special Educati<strong>on</strong> Technology, 20(2), 5–14.<br />

Riverturn, Inc. (2009-2010). VoiceCentral, VoiceCentral<br />

Black Swan. Retrieved from www.riverturn.com<br />

June 26, 2010.<br />

Stock, S. E., Davies, D. K., Davies, K. R., & Wehmeyer,<br />

M. L. (2006). Evaluati<strong>on</strong> of an applicati<strong>on</strong><br />

for making palmtop computers accessible to individuals<br />

with intellectual disabilities. Journal of<br />

Intellectual & <strong>Developmental</strong> Disability, 31, 39–<str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>.<br />

Stock, S. E., Davies, D. K., Wehmeyer, M. L., &<br />

Palmer, S. B. (2008). Evaluati<strong>on</strong> of cognitively<br />

Twenty-First Century Portable Electr<strong>on</strong>ic Devices / 497


accessible software to increase independent access<br />

to cellph<strong>on</strong>e technology for people with intellectual<br />

disability. Journal of Intellectual Disability<br />

Research, 52, 1155–1164.<br />

Taber, T. A., Alberto, P. A., & Frederick, L. D.<br />

(1998). Use of self-operated auditory prompts by<br />

workers with moderate mental retardati<strong>on</strong> to<br />

transiti<strong>on</strong> independently through vocati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

tasks. Research in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 19, 327–<br />

345.<br />

Taber, T. A., Alberto, P. A., Hughes, M., & Seltzer,<br />

A. (2002). A strategy for students with moderate<br />

disabilities when lost in the community. Research<br />

& Practice for Pers<strong>on</strong>s with Severe <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 27,<br />

141–152.<br />

Taber, T. A., Alberto, P. A., Seltzer, A., & Hughes,<br />

M. (2003). Obtaining assistance when lost in the<br />

community using cell ph<strong>on</strong>es. Research & Practice<br />

for Pers<strong>on</strong>s with Severe <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 28, 105–116.<br />

Taber-Doughty, T. (2005). C<strong>on</strong>sidering student<br />

choice when selecting instructi<strong>on</strong>al strategies: a<br />

comparis<strong>on</strong> of three prompting systems. Research<br />

in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 26, 411–432.<br />

Taber-Doughty, T., Patt<strong>on</strong>, S. E., & Brennan, S.<br />

(2008). Simultaneous <strong>and</strong> delayed video modeling:<br />

An examinati<strong>on</strong> of system effectiveness <strong>and</strong><br />

student preferences. Journal of Special Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

Technology, 23(1), 1–18.<br />

Van Laarhoven, T., Van Laarhoven-Myers, T.<br />

(2006). Comparis<strong>on</strong> of three video-based instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

procedures for teaching daily living skills to<br />

pers<strong>on</strong>s with developmental disabilities. Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 41, 365–<br />

381.<br />

Van Laarhoven, T., Van Laarhoven-Myers, T., &<br />

Zurita, L. M. (2007). The effectiveness of using a<br />

Pocket PC as a video modeling <strong>and</strong> feedback<br />

device for individuals with developmental disabilities<br />

in vocati<strong>on</strong>al settings. Assistive Technology Outcomes<br />

<strong>and</strong> Benefits, 14(1), 28–45.<br />

Van Laarhoven, T., Johns<strong>on</strong>, J. W., Van Laarhoven-<br />

Myers, T., Grider, K. L., & Grider, K. M. (2009).<br />

The effectiveness of using a video iPod as a<br />

prompting device in employment settings. Journal<br />

of Behavioral Educati<strong>on</strong>, 18, 119–141.<br />

Wade, T. K., & Troy, J. C. (2001). Mobile ph<strong>on</strong>es as<br />

a new memory aid: A preliminary investigati<strong>on</strong><br />

using case studies. Brain Injury, 15, 305–320.<br />

Wehmeyer, M. L., Palmer, S. B., Smith, S. J., Davies,<br />

D. K., & Stock, S. (2008). The efficacy of technology<br />

use by people with intellectual disability: A<br />

single-subject design Meta-analysis. Journal of Special<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> Technology, 23(3), 21–30.<br />

West, E. A. (2008). Effects of verbal cues versus<br />

pictorial cues <strong>on</strong> the transfer of stimulus c<strong>on</strong>trol<br />

for children with autism. Focus <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Other<br />

<strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 23, 229–241.<br />

Received: 27 July 2010<br />

Initial Acceptance: 4 October 2010<br />

Final Acceptance: 18 November 2010<br />

498 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 2011, <str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>(4), 499–513<br />

© <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Video Modeling <strong>and</strong> Prompting: A Comparis<strong>on</strong> of Two<br />

Strategies for Teaching Cooking Skills to Students with Mild<br />

Intellectual <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Teresa Taber-Doughty, Emily C. Bouck, Kinsey Tom, Andrea D. Jasper,<br />

Sara M. Flanagan, <strong>and</strong> Laura Bassette<br />

Purdue University<br />

Abstract: Self-operated video prompting <strong>and</strong> video modeling was compared when used by three sec<strong>on</strong>dary<br />

students with mild intellectual disabilities as they completed novel recipes during cooking activities. Alternating<br />

between video systems, students completed twelve recipes within their classroom kitchen. An alternating<br />

treatment design with a follow-up <strong>and</strong> withdrawal probe was used to illustrate the effectiveness of both systems<br />

<strong>on</strong> each student’s independent task performance. Results indicated increased independence following video<br />

system use by all three students with video modeling more effective for two students <strong>and</strong> video prompting more<br />

effective for the third. Future directi<strong>on</strong>s for research are presented.<br />

Students with mild intellectual disabilities are<br />

those who typically face a range of challenges<br />

related to learning, including difficulty generalizing<br />

<strong>and</strong>/or transferring informati<strong>on</strong>, inputting<br />

<strong>and</strong> retrieving informati<strong>on</strong> from<br />

memory, <strong>and</strong> short attenti<strong>on</strong> spans (Belm<strong>on</strong>t,<br />

1966; Dunn, 1973; Kirk, 1972; Spitz, 1973;<br />

Stephens, 1972; Thomas, 1996; Zeaman &<br />

House, 1963, 1979). Specifically, students with<br />

mild intellectual disabilities are characterized<br />

by “significantly subaverage intellectual functi<strong>on</strong>ing,<br />

existing c<strong>on</strong>currently with related<br />

limitati<strong>on</strong>s in two or more of the following<br />

applicable adaptive skill areas: communicati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

self-care, home living, social skills, community<br />

use, self directi<strong>on</strong>, health <strong>and</strong> safety,<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>al academics, leisure, <strong>and</strong> work” (Polloway,<br />

Patt<strong>on</strong>, Smith, & Buck, 1997, p. 298).<br />

However, despite the term “mild,” these students<br />

do not necessarily possess mild learning<br />

challenges (Luckass<strong>on</strong> et al., 2002; Polloway,<br />

2004; 2005). They experience numerous<br />

The final four authors listed provided equal effort<br />

into to this study <strong>and</strong> thus, their order should not be<br />

interpreted as a greater c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong> over a preceding<br />

author. Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence c<strong>on</strong>cerning this article<br />

should be addressed to Dr. Teresa Taber-Doughty,<br />

5162 BRNG Hall, Purdue University, 100 North University<br />

Street, West Lafayette, IN 47907-2098, 765-<br />

494-7345, tabert@purdue.edu<br />

learning challenges c<strong>on</strong>siderably impacting<br />

their functi<strong>on</strong>ing in current <strong>and</strong> potential future<br />

envir<strong>on</strong>ments.<br />

Students with mild intellectual disabilities<br />

<strong>on</strong>ce received a substantial focus in research<br />

<strong>and</strong> practice; however, in recent decades, attenti<strong>on</strong><br />

to the educati<strong>on</strong>al issues of these students<br />

declined (Bouck, 2007; Edgar, 1987;<br />

Polloway, 2006). This lack of c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong><br />

extends to issues of curriculum <strong>and</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

where historically these students theoretically<br />

received a functi<strong>on</strong>al curriculum–a curriculum<br />

focused <strong>on</strong> life skills that enable<br />

adults to be successful in life, work, <strong>and</strong> participati<strong>on</strong><br />

in all facets of an inclusive community<br />

(Brown et al., 1979; Cr<strong>on</strong>in, 1996; Patt<strong>on</strong>,<br />

Polloway, & Smith, 2000). However, over the<br />

past few decades, researchers suggested a decrease<br />

<strong>on</strong> a functi<strong>on</strong>al or life skills approach<br />

in practice <strong>and</strong> research (Alwell & Cobb,<br />

2009; Billingsley, 1997; Billingsley & Alberts<strong>on</strong>,<br />

1999; Nietupski, Hamre-Nietupski,<br />

Curtin, & Shrikanth,1997), although without<br />

data as to the effectiveness of other curricula.<br />

For example, Bouck (2004a) reported a<br />

range of curricular approaches being used by<br />

teachers for sec<strong>on</strong>dary students with mild intellectual<br />

disabilities including a functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

curriculum (19.0%), a special educati<strong>on</strong> curriculum<br />

(23.8%), a general educati<strong>on</strong> curriculum<br />

(15.3%), a lower grade level curriculum<br />

Video Modeling <strong>and</strong> Prompting / 499


(14.3%), a unique curriculum (13.8%), no<br />

curriculum (4.8%), <strong>and</strong> a vocati<strong>on</strong>al curriculum<br />

(1.1%). Her results suggested limited attenti<strong>on</strong><br />

to a functi<strong>on</strong>al curriculum for this<br />

populati<strong>on</strong>, akin to the critique by Patt<strong>on</strong> et<br />

al. (2000) regarding the practice of educating<br />

students with mild intellectual disabilities (i.e.,<br />

a watered-down general educati<strong>on</strong> curriculum<br />

lacking specialized instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>crete<br />

educati<strong>on</strong>al benefits).<br />

Currently, the curricular focus for students<br />

with mild intellectual disabilities is believed to<br />

be mixed <strong>and</strong> variable, although educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

policy within the last decade placed greater<br />

emphasis <strong>on</strong> students with mild intellectual<br />

disabilities receiving <strong>and</strong> succeeding in a general<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> curriculum (Bouck, 2007;<br />

Bouck, Bassette, Taber-Doughty, Flanagan, &<br />

Szwed, 2009; Patt<strong>on</strong> et al., 2000). No Child Left<br />

Behind (NCLB, 2002) <strong>and</strong> the Individuals with<br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong> Act (IDEA, 2004) privilege<br />

students taking general large scale assessments,<br />

suggesting the curricular focus is a general<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> curriculum to the detriment of<br />

other approaches (i.e., functi<strong>on</strong>al) (Bouck,<br />

2007). Patt<strong>on</strong> et al. questi<strong>on</strong>ed the availability<br />

of specialized curriculum currently in schools<br />

for these students.<br />

The lack of specialized curriculum for students<br />

with mild intellectual disabilities may be<br />

problematic given research suggesting the<br />

poor postschool outcomes typically experienced<br />

by this populati<strong>on</strong>. For example, students<br />

typically face lower rates of employment,<br />

independent living, <strong>and</strong> postsec<strong>on</strong>dary<br />

school attendance (Blackorby & Wagner,<br />

1996; Kaye, 1997; Newman, 2005; Newman,<br />

Wagner, Cameto, & Knokey, 2009). While typically<br />

not measured in the instruments assessing<br />

other outcomes, students with mild intellectual<br />

disabilities also face challenges related<br />

to daily living skills (Lynch & Beare, 1990).<br />

Thus, a need exists for elements of a functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

curriculum for these students. Alwell<br />

<strong>and</strong> Cobb (2009) identified the lack of research<br />

attenti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>al life skills was<br />

particularly apparent for students with high<br />

incidence disabilities (e.g., mild intellectual<br />

disabilities; Reschly, 2002) as well as research<br />

of high quality <strong>on</strong> the impact of life skills<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Bouck <strong>and</strong> Flanagan (2010) note the limited<br />

current research <strong>on</strong> teaching functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

life skills to students with mild intellectual<br />

disabilities. In a systematic review, the authors<br />

found seven articles published between 1994<br />

<strong>and</strong> 2009 <strong>on</strong> teaching at least <strong>on</strong>e student with<br />

a mild intellectual disability a functi<strong>on</strong>al curriculum<br />

or elements of a functi<strong>on</strong>al curriculum<br />

(i.e., functi<strong>on</strong>al academics, vocati<strong>on</strong>al educati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

community access, daily living,<br />

financial, independent living, transportati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

social/relati<strong>on</strong>ships, <strong>and</strong> self-determinati<strong>on</strong>)<br />

(Patt<strong>on</strong>, Cr<strong>on</strong>in, & Jairrels, 1997). Of the<br />

seven, four focused, in part, <strong>on</strong> daily living<br />

skills with three of those related to food, nutriti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong>/or cooking (Collins, Brans<strong>on</strong>, &<br />

Hall, 1995; Kennedy, Itk<strong>on</strong>en, & Lindquist,<br />

1994; Arnold-Reid, Schloss, & Alper, 1997)<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>e directed at safety (Collins & Stins<strong>on</strong>,<br />

1995). Students improved in the targeted<br />

skills in each intended area; thus, the review<br />

by Bouck <strong>and</strong> Flanagan highlighted the minimal<br />

attenti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> aspects of functi<strong>on</strong>al living<br />

skills while simultaneously showing the effectiveness<br />

for skill acquisiti<strong>on</strong> using this approach.<br />

Strategies to Teach Functi<strong>on</strong>al Skills<br />

Strategies used to teach functi<strong>on</strong>al skills to<br />

students with mild intellectual disabilities<br />

since 1994 included time delay (Collins et al.,<br />

1995; Collins & Stins<strong>on</strong>, 1995; Kennedy et al.,<br />

1994), <strong>on</strong>e-more-than c<strong>on</strong>cept for purchasing<br />

(Denny & Test, 1995), goal setting (Agran,<br />

Blanchard, Wehmeyer, & Hughes, 2002), <strong>and</strong><br />

the system of least prompts (Arnold-Reid et<br />

al., 1997). Each resulted in overall increases in<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>al skill acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

for students with mild intellectual disabilities.<br />

Although no study could be found focusing<br />

<strong>on</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>al skills training in which technology<br />

as an instructi<strong>on</strong>al tool was used, technology<br />

was used for teaching other skills to this<br />

populati<strong>on</strong>. Computerized instructi<strong>on</strong> (e.g.,<br />

computer assisted instructi<strong>on</strong>, pentop computers)<br />

was successfully used to teach mathematics<br />

(Bouck et al., 2009; Fazio & Polsgrove,<br />

1989), social skills (Margalit, 1995), fact retrieval<br />

skills (Edyburn, 1991), <strong>and</strong> word recogniti<strong>on</strong><br />

skills (Lin, Podell, & Rein, 1991). Thus,<br />

the potential exists for students with mild intellectual<br />

disabilities to successfully use various<br />

forms of technology in acquiring <strong>and</strong> generalizing<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>al skills. Researchers have<br />

500 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


however taught functi<strong>on</strong>al skills, using technology,<br />

to students with moderate <strong>and</strong> severe<br />

intellectual disabilities <strong>and</strong> autism spectrum<br />

disorders.<br />

Various technologies are frequently used<br />

when teaching functi<strong>on</strong>al skills to students<br />

with moderate <strong>and</strong> severe intellectual disabilities<br />

<strong>and</strong> autism spectrum disorders. Recent<br />

investigati<strong>on</strong>s reported the effectiveness<br />

of computers (Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, Dijkstra,<br />

O’Reilly, Groeneweg, & Van den Hof, 2000;<br />

Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, Van den Hof, Boelens, Rocha, &<br />

Seedhouse, 1998; Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, Van den Hof,<br />

Furniss, O’Reilly, & Cunha, 1999; Mechling,<br />

2003; 2005; Mechling, Gast, & Lang<strong>on</strong>e,<br />

2002), pers<strong>on</strong>al digital assistants (PDA)<br />

(Cihak, Kessler, & Alberto, 2007), MP3 players<br />

(Taber-Doughty, 2005), <strong>and</strong> iPods for<br />

delivering numerous forms of instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

incorporating technology in school <strong>and</strong><br />

community settings (Taber-Doughty, Patt<strong>on</strong>,<br />

& Brennan, 2008; Van Laarhoven, Johns<strong>on</strong>,<br />

Van Laarhoven-Meyers, Grider, &<br />

Grider, 2009; Van Laarhoven & Van Laarhoven-Meyers,<br />

2006). All resulted in increases<br />

in skill acquisiti<strong>on</strong>, maintenance<br />

<strong>and</strong>/or generalizati<strong>on</strong>; thus, dem<strong>on</strong>strating<br />

the potential for the <strong>on</strong>going use of technology<br />

in instructi<strong>on</strong> for these populati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Two increasingly used strategies for teaching<br />

skills to students with autism spectrum<br />

disorder <strong>and</strong> moderate to severe intellectual<br />

disabilities incorporate video technology for<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> in the form of video prompting<br />

<strong>and</strong> video modeling. Video prompting combines<br />

visual <strong>and</strong> auditory prompts requiring<br />

students to view a single step of a video task<br />

sequence <strong>and</strong> complete that step before<br />

watching the next video clip of the subsequent<br />

step <strong>and</strong> performing that step (Cihak,<br />

Alberto, Taber-Doughty, & Gama, 2006;<br />

Krantz, MacDuff, Wadstrom, & McClannahan,<br />

1991; Taber-Doughty et al., 2008).<br />

Video prompting was successfully used to<br />

teach individuals with disabilities vocati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

skills in an employment setting (Van Laarhoven<br />

et al. 2009), to use an ATM <strong>and</strong> debit<br />

machine (Cihak et al; Mechling, Gast, &<br />

Barthold, 2003), daily living skills (Cannella-<br />

Mal<strong>on</strong>e, Sigafoos, O’Reilly, de la Cruz,<br />

Edrisinha, & Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, 2006; Van Laarhoven<br />

& Van Laarhoven-Meyers, 2006), grocery<br />

shopping skills (Hutchers<strong>on</strong>, Lang<strong>on</strong>e, Ay-<br />

res, & Clees, 2004; Mechling et al., 2002),<br />

<strong>and</strong> cooking skills (Graves, Collins, Schuster,<br />

& Kleinert, 2005). Similar success was<br />

also found with video modeling in which<br />

students perform a task in the same or alternative<br />

setting without additi<strong>on</strong>al prompting<br />

<strong>on</strong>ly after viewing the entire task sequence<br />

(Cannella-Mal<strong>on</strong>e et al.; Taber-Doughty et<br />

al.). What may vary with video modeling,<br />

other than the task, is the delay between<br />

viewing the video model <strong>and</strong> the actual performance<br />

of the task. Reported delays between<br />

viewing videos <strong>and</strong> performing tasks<br />

range from immediately after viewing (Charlop-Christy<br />

& Daneshvar, 2003; Geiger, Le-<br />

Blanc, Dill<strong>on</strong>, & Bates, 2010; Nikopoulos &<br />

Keenan, 2004) to at least an hour after viewing<br />

(Alberto, Cihak, & Gama, 2005;<br />

D’Ateno, Mangiapanello, & Taylor, 2003;<br />

Taber-Doughty et al.; Wert & Neisworth,<br />

2003) when teaching students with autism<br />

spectrum disorder a variety of social, play<br />

<strong>and</strong> drawing skills, <strong>and</strong> numerous functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

community skills (e.g., library skills, using<br />

an ATM) with students experiencing moderate<br />

<strong>and</strong> severe intellectual disabilities.<br />

While <strong>on</strong>going evidence is reported about<br />

the effectiveness of both video prompting<br />

<strong>and</strong> modeling for teaching students with autism<br />

spectrum disorder <strong>and</strong> moderate to severe<br />

intellectual disabilities, no studies<br />

could be found in which these strategies<br />

were used for teaching skills to students with<br />

mild intellectual disabilities. At a time when<br />

video technology is increasingly accessible<br />

<strong>and</strong> uncomplicated while c<strong>on</strong>currently socially<br />

desirable by peers (Taber-Doughty et<br />

al. 2008), research should seek to examine<br />

whether this technology is appropriate for<br />

use with students who experience mild intellectual<br />

disabilities. In additi<strong>on</strong>, the need<br />

exists to refocus research efforts in the area<br />

of functi<strong>on</strong>al skills training for students with<br />

mild intellectual disabilities (Bouck & Flanagan,<br />

in press). As such, the purpose of the<br />

present investigati<strong>on</strong> was to compare the<br />

effectiveness of video prompts <strong>and</strong> video<br />

modeling when used to teach three middle<br />

school students with mild intellectual disabilities<br />

to acquire cooking skills when preparing<br />

a variety of simple recipes.<br />

Video Modeling <strong>and</strong> Prompting / 501


TABLE 1<br />

Student Characteristics<br />

Student<br />

Method<br />

Participants<br />

Chr<strong>on</strong>ological<br />

Age Ethnicity IQ<br />

Brittany 12 Caucasian 72 a<br />

Rose 13 Caucasian 61 a<br />

Wes 12 African American 63 a<br />

a WISC-IV.<br />

b GAC<br />

c ABAS-II<br />

d Test Scores unavailable; score given is a teacher estimate<br />

e Woodcock-Johns<strong>on</strong><br />

Three sixth grade students with mild intellectual<br />

disabilities served as participants for this<br />

study. Brittany, Rose, <strong>and</strong> Wes were nominated<br />

by their teacher for participati<strong>on</strong> in the<br />

study based <strong>on</strong> the following: (a) willingness<br />

to participate, (b) level of cognitive functi<strong>on</strong>ing<br />

within the mild range of intellectual disabilities,<br />

(c) no sensory deficits, (d) limited, or<br />

no experience cooking, <strong>and</strong> (e) successful<br />

completi<strong>on</strong> of a pre-training program. Two of<br />

the participants were Caucasian <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>e was<br />

African American <strong>and</strong> all participants spent<br />

approximately 80% of their time in a special<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> setting for students with mild intellectual<br />

disabilities <strong>and</strong> 20% of their time in<br />

general educati<strong>on</strong>, largely for elective courses<br />

(i.e., physical educati<strong>on</strong>). All participants<br />

spoke English as their first language. Intelligence,<br />

adaptive behavior ratings, <strong>and</strong> reading<br />

<strong>and</strong> math levels were obtained from the<br />

teacher. Table 1 provides a summary for each<br />

participant.<br />

Brittany. Brittany was a 12 year-old sixthgrade<br />

female with a mild intellectual disability.<br />

Brittany had limited previous cooking experience<br />

at home <strong>and</strong> no previous cooking<br />

experience at school. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, Brittany<br />

reported she did not have previous experience<br />

using an iPod. Despite her limited cooking<br />

experience, Brittany expressed interest in<br />

using an iPod to learn how to make new recipes.<br />

Rose. Rose was a 13 year-old sixth-grade<br />

female with a mild intellectual disability. Rose<br />

reported she had limited previous cooking<br />

experience at home, <strong>and</strong> Rose’s special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

teacher indicated she did not have previous<br />

cooking experience at school. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally,<br />

Rose reported she did not have previous<br />

experience using an iPod, but she did have<br />

experience using another br<strong>and</strong> of MP3<br />

player <strong>and</strong> thought it was easy to use. Rose was<br />

also interested in learning how to cook <strong>and</strong><br />

making new recipes using videos although she<br />

lacked prior experience with these types of<br />

activities.<br />

Wes. Wes was a 12 year-old sixth-grade<br />

male with a mild intellectual disability. Wes<br />

reported his parents did not typically allow<br />

him to cook at home. Wes’ teacher indicated<br />

he did not have previous cooking experience<br />

at school. Wes also indicated he had no previous<br />

experience using an iPod, though he did<br />

state that he was good at “figuring out computers<br />

<strong>and</strong> a lot of stuff.” Similar to Brittany<br />

<strong>and</strong> Rose, Wes expressed an interest in using<br />

an iPod to learn how to cook.<br />

Setting<br />

Adaptive<br />

Behavior Reading Math<br />

44 b<br />

77 c<br />

88 b<br />

4.5 th grade d<br />

83 e<br />

87 e<br />

4.5 th grade d<br />

45 e<br />

70 e<br />

All cooking activities took place in the participants’<br />

special educati<strong>on</strong> classroom. Up<strong>on</strong> entering<br />

the classroom there was a small area<br />

(approximately <strong>on</strong>e-fourth of the classroom)<br />

with three sinks, a counter, cooking appliances<br />

(i.e., toaster oven, toaster, blender, <strong>and</strong><br />

microwave), <strong>and</strong> a refrigerator. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, a<br />

round table with six chairs was present for<br />

502 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


students <strong>and</strong> teachers to eat. Next to the refrigerator,<br />

near the rear of the classroom, was<br />

a table with three computers for student use.<br />

On the other side of the table, also near the<br />

rear of the classroom, was a desk for the paraeducators.<br />

In the middle of the classroom was<br />

a large instructi<strong>on</strong>al area where sixteen student<br />

desks faced the fr<strong>on</strong>t of the classroom. In<br />

the fr<strong>on</strong>t of the classroom next to the teacher’s<br />

desk was an interactive electr<strong>on</strong>ic whiteboard<br />

students used to complete several academic<br />

activities (e.g., math worksheets, daily<br />

oral language, etc.).<br />

Cooking activities took place in the morning<br />

between 8:30AM <strong>and</strong> 11:30AM. Depending<br />

<strong>on</strong> the class period, there were 12 to 15<br />

additi<strong>on</strong>al students in the classroom. While<br />

participating students completed cooking<br />

tasks, other students in the class were engaged<br />

in mathematics or Language Arts. These activities<br />

were typically completed in a group setting<br />

with the teacher or paraeducator lecturing<br />

to students <strong>and</strong> allowing students to raise<br />

their h<strong>and</strong>s <strong>and</strong> ask questi<strong>on</strong>s. C<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong><br />

am<strong>on</strong>gst students <strong>and</strong> teachers occurred often<br />

while target students engaged in cooking activities.<br />

Materials<br />

Recipes. All recipes students followed were<br />

from the Cooking to Learn books functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

curriculum from PCI Educati<strong>on</strong>al Publishing<br />

(Anders<strong>on</strong>, Coxs<strong>on</strong>, Lam<strong>on</strong>tagne, Buteyn, &<br />

Chapman, 2008; Anders<strong>on</strong>, Coxs<strong>on</strong>, Britt,<br />

Haugen-McLane, & Mullins, 1999; Coxs<strong>on</strong> &<br />

Anders<strong>on</strong>, 2001). Recipes were written at an<br />

independent level of reading, allowing students<br />

to read them without assistance. Recipes<br />

from the books were re-typed in Microsoft<br />

Word to allow for changes in the recipes (i.e.,<br />

if a recipe called for an oven, but was changed<br />

to a toaster oven due to the supplies available)<br />

<strong>and</strong> for c<strong>on</strong>sistency in how recipes were<br />

worded (i.e., to read, “Put 1/2 cup of ____,”<br />

instead of variati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> that step/wording<br />

such as “Add <strong>on</strong>e half cup of _____”). Recipes<br />

were also retyped to eliminate the preprinted<br />

checklist of ingredients <strong>and</strong> supplies at the top<br />

of each page. Recipes were varied, but all<br />

required completing seven or more task steps<br />

using multiple foods <strong>and</strong> cooking supplies.<br />

Cooking supplies. Cooking supplies were<br />

those typically available in a kitchen (e.g., a<br />

toaster oven, toaster, hot plate, <strong>and</strong> a microwave).<br />

Other cooking supplies used included<br />

pans, a cookie sheet, measuring cups <strong>and</strong><br />

spo<strong>on</strong>s, cooking <strong>and</strong> eating utensils, <strong>and</strong><br />

plates or bowls. Students were able to use<br />

these independently without instructi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Ingredients. Recipe ingredients were purchased<br />

prior to baseline <strong>and</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

For each recipe, all ingredients were<br />

placed <strong>on</strong> the worktable for students to select<br />

from when completing steps of each recipe.<br />

Comm<strong>on</strong> ingredients used included a variety<br />

of fruits, milk, crackers, <strong>and</strong> biscuit dough.<br />

Videos. Short videos ranging from <strong>on</strong>e<br />

minute <strong>and</strong> fifty-seven sec<strong>on</strong>ds to six minutes<br />

<strong>and</strong> thirty-eight sec<strong>on</strong>ds were created using<br />

iMovie (Apple Inc., 2010) <strong>and</strong> dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

how to complete each recipe from start to<br />

finish. Each video also c<strong>on</strong>tained simple audio<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>s in which task analysis steps were<br />

read prior to being dem<strong>on</strong>strated <strong>on</strong> the<br />

video. The third author physically <strong>and</strong> verbally<br />

modeled each step. All videos had similar pacing<br />

(i.e., how far apart each step was modeled)<br />

<strong>and</strong> view. For example, the video always<br />

showed the entire measuring cup being filled<br />

with an ingredient rather than just the specific<br />

line that to which the cup was being measured;<br />

or, the video zoomed in when going to<br />

a temperature or time setting. All videos were<br />

filmed in the classroom where participants<br />

performed tasks <strong>and</strong> used the same equipment<br />

<strong>and</strong> ingredients to be used by students<br />

for completing recipes.<br />

iPods. Three Apple 8-G iPod Nanos with<br />

color video capabilities were used in this<br />

study. The screen display was approximately<br />

<strong>on</strong>e <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>e-fourth inches wide <strong>and</strong> two<br />

inches in height. Below the screen was the<br />

c<strong>on</strong>trol wheel approximately <strong>on</strong>e inch in diameter<br />

c<strong>on</strong>taining the word “menu” at the<br />

top, play/pause at the bottom, <strong>and</strong> rewind<br />

<strong>and</strong> fast forward <strong>on</strong> the left <strong>and</strong> right sides<br />

respectively. At the center of the wheel was a<br />

black select butt<strong>on</strong> pressed when making a<br />

selecti<strong>on</strong> from the screen. Students turned <strong>on</strong><br />

the iPod by pressing <strong>and</strong> holding down any<br />

<strong>on</strong>e of the butt<strong>on</strong>s. In order to access videos,<br />

<strong>on</strong>ce the screen was lit, students pressed the<br />

menu butt<strong>on</strong>. Students moved their thumb<br />

around the c<strong>on</strong>trol wheel until the word “vid-<br />

Video Modeling <strong>and</strong> Prompting / 503


eos” was highlighted. Students then pressed<br />

the center select butt<strong>on</strong>. From the next list of<br />

items appearing <strong>on</strong> the screen, students used<br />

their thumb to scroll to the word “movies” <strong>and</strong><br />

pressed the center butt<strong>on</strong> to c<strong>on</strong>firm that selecti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

On the final screen, students scrolled<br />

down to the name of the correct recipe video<br />

<strong>and</strong> pressed the center select butt<strong>on</strong>. Once<br />

selected, they were able to press the play (or<br />

pause) butt<strong>on</strong> as needed. Students did not<br />

report any difficulty with being able to view<br />

the screen. During this study, all students navigated<br />

the iPods independently. Students<br />

watched videos <strong>and</strong> listened to the audio using<br />

earph<strong>on</strong>es; two students preferred behind-the-ear<br />

headph<strong>on</strong>es while <strong>on</strong>e preferred<br />

ear buds.<br />

Dependent <strong>and</strong> Independent Variables<br />

Video modeling <strong>and</strong> video prompting in c<strong>on</strong>juncti<strong>on</strong><br />

with a six level system of least<br />

prompts were two independent variables used<br />

to teach students to cook basic recipes. Pairs<br />

of recipes were matched based <strong>on</strong> number of<br />

steps, difficulty of completi<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> similarity<br />

of tasks involved. Students completed 12 total<br />

recipes using video modeling or video<br />

prompting during the interventi<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

When using video modeling students watched<br />

the full video of the task to be completed five<br />

minutes prior to being asked to cook the recipe.<br />

When video prompting was used, students<br />

watched individual task steps while cooking<br />

<strong>and</strong> paused the video after each step.<br />

When the system of least prompts was<br />

needed during video modeling <strong>and</strong> video<br />

prompting sessi<strong>on</strong>s, the following prompts<br />

were available: independent student performance<br />

(no prompt), verbal prompt, gesture,<br />

modeling, partial physical prompt, <strong>and</strong> full<br />

physical prompt. The dependent variable was<br />

the percent of task analysis steps for each recipe<br />

each student completed independently<br />

without prompting. The level of prompting<br />

needed for students to complete each step was<br />

also recorded.<br />

Design <strong>and</strong> Data Collecti<strong>on</strong><br />

An alternating treatment design (ATD) with a<br />

baseline <strong>and</strong> maintenance c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> was used<br />

to illustrate the effectiveness of the video mod-<br />

eling <strong>and</strong> video prompting systems for each<br />

student. This design was selected as it allowed<br />

investigators to rapidly compare the effects of<br />

both interventi<strong>on</strong>s to establish if <strong>on</strong>e was more<br />

effective than another when determining independent<br />

task performance while c<strong>on</strong>currently<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strating experimental c<strong>on</strong>trol<br />

(Kennedy, 2005). A flip of a coin was used to<br />

determine the order in which prompting systems<br />

were used with no more than two c<strong>on</strong>secutive<br />

administrati<strong>on</strong>s of a prompting system<br />

during interventi<strong>on</strong>. The maintenance<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> allowed investigators to c<strong>on</strong>firm student<br />

performance levels achieved during interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

using the system that resulted in<br />

greater independence.<br />

Each recipe was divided into discrete steps<br />

of a task analysis. Event recording was used to<br />

record the number of steps from each task<br />

analysis students were able to complete independently.<br />

Using each recipe step, a data<br />

sheet was created to allow researchers to record<br />

whether students completed each step<br />

independently or required a prompt <strong>and</strong> the<br />

level of prompt needed.<br />

Experimental Procedures<br />

Twenty-three different recipes were used in<br />

this study across c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s. Recipes were selected<br />

from the Cooking to Learn curriculum<br />

books (Coxs<strong>on</strong> & Anders<strong>on</strong>, 2001) <strong>and</strong> were<br />

divided <strong>and</strong> grouped together into three categories<br />

based <strong>on</strong> their number of task analysis<br />

steps. Specifically, the three groups included<br />

recipes c<strong>on</strong>taining 6–8 steps, 9–11 steps, <strong>and</strong><br />

12 or more steps. Three recipes were included<br />

in the study from the first group, 15 recipes<br />

from the sec<strong>on</strong>d group, <strong>and</strong> five recipes from<br />

the third.<br />

Recipes were r<strong>and</strong>omly selected during all<br />

phases of the study. Specifically, a total of 37<br />

recipes were selected <strong>and</strong> divided into the<br />

three groups based <strong>on</strong> their number of steps<br />

(i.e., 6–8, 9–11, <strong>and</strong> 12 or more). Six (16%)<br />

recipes c<strong>on</strong>tained 6–8 steps, 20 (54%) c<strong>on</strong>tained<br />

9–11 steps, <strong>and</strong> 11 (30%) c<strong>on</strong>tained 12<br />

or more steps. During interventi<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

recipes were r<strong>and</strong>omly selected <strong>and</strong> paired<br />

based <strong>on</strong> task difficulty, similarities, <strong>and</strong> number<br />

of steps. Overall, 6 recipe pairs or 12 total<br />

recipes were used during interventi<strong>on</strong>. Each<br />

recipe pair had a similar number of steps, type<br />

504 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


of food, cooking supplies needed, <strong>and</strong> preparati<strong>on</strong><br />

steps.<br />

Pretraining. Prior to data collecti<strong>on</strong>, students<br />

were introduced to the iPod Nanos <strong>and</strong><br />

taught how to use the device to watch videos.<br />

A sample video illustrating a simple task was<br />

created. To ensure each student was able to<br />

successfully use the iPod prior to beginning<br />

the study, each watched the video <strong>and</strong> completed<br />

the tasks (i.e., raised their right h<strong>and</strong> as<br />

the model <strong>on</strong> the video did, drew a blue star,<br />

drew a pink circle, etc.). Each student was<br />

required to complete the tasks with 80% accuracy<br />

before beginning baseline.<br />

Baseline. During this c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, students<br />

were given a paper copy of a recipe <strong>and</strong> instructed<br />

to make the assigned recipe. The students<br />

were observed while cooking <strong>and</strong> a<br />

member of the research team used the system<br />

of least prompts to correct the students, as<br />

needed. The percent of steps each student<br />

completed independently were recorded per<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>. Students c<strong>on</strong>tinued in the baseline<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> based <strong>on</strong> the order they were selected<br />

to begin the interventi<strong>on</strong>. Brittany began<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> first while Rose <strong>and</strong> Wes c<strong>on</strong>tinued<br />

baseline phase using traditi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>. Rose began interventi<strong>on</strong> sec<strong>on</strong>d;<br />

Wes c<strong>on</strong>tinued in the baseline phase using the<br />

traditi<strong>on</strong>al instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> was the last student<br />

to begin the interventi<strong>on</strong>. Students <strong>on</strong>ly began<br />

the interventi<strong>on</strong> phase <strong>on</strong>ce baseline stability<br />

was established.<br />

Interventi<strong>on</strong>. This c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sisted of<br />

two treatments: video modeling <strong>and</strong> video<br />

prompting. Six pairs (12 recipes total) of<br />

matched recipes were used <strong>and</strong> each student<br />

completed <strong>on</strong>e of the matched recipes using<br />

video modeling <strong>and</strong> the other recipe using<br />

video prompting. During video modeling sessi<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

students were individually presented<br />

with the iPod set to start <strong>on</strong> the selected video.<br />

Each was then instructed to watch the entire<br />

video at his/her desk. Once each student finished<br />

watching the video, he or she remained<br />

seated for 5 minutes (5-minute time delay)<br />

prior to beginning the cooking activity. At that<br />

time, students were asked to move to the cooking<br />

area. Once there, students were presented<br />

with a paper copy of the recipe <strong>and</strong> directed<br />

to begin cooking. Throughout each sessi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

the system of least prompts was used when the<br />

student required assistance with a task step.<br />

During video prompting sessi<strong>on</strong>s, each student<br />

immediately moved to the table in the<br />

kitchen area where he or she was given the<br />

iPod set to start at the selected video. Each was<br />

then instructed to pause the video after each<br />

step <strong>and</strong> complete that step before pressing,<br />

“Play,” to move <strong>on</strong> to the next step. Students<br />

were also instructed to rewind the video if a<br />

step needed to be viewed again before moving<br />

<strong>on</strong> to the next step. Finally, each student was<br />

provided a paper copy of the recipe. As during<br />

video modeling sessi<strong>on</strong>s, the system of least<br />

prompts was used to assist the students as they<br />

completed each cooking step.<br />

Follow-up. Two separate probes over a twoweek<br />

period were collected during this c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong><br />

in which students completed two additi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

matched recipes. The first probe<br />

recorded the student’s level of independence<br />

as he or she c<strong>on</strong>tinued to use his/her more<br />

effective interventi<strong>on</strong> used during the previous<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. The sec<strong>on</strong>d follow-up probe<br />

was c<strong>on</strong>ducted <strong>on</strong>e week later where neither<br />

video system was used. The purpose of this<br />

final probe was to determine if student performance<br />

would return to baseline levels. As<br />

in baseline <strong>and</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong>, the percent of<br />

steps completed independently by students<br />

were recorded <strong>and</strong> the system of least prompts<br />

was implemented as needed.<br />

Interobserver Agreement <strong>and</strong> Treatment Integrity<br />

Interobserver agreement data were collected<br />

by a trained sec<strong>on</strong>d observer for each student<br />

across all three c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s. The sec<strong>on</strong>d observer<br />

recorded whether the student completed<br />

steps independently or with prompts.<br />

Interobserver data were collected by the sec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

observer at the same time as the first<br />

observer. The percent agreement for steps<br />

completed independently by students was calculated<br />

by dividing the number of agreements<br />

by the total of agreements plus disagreements<br />

<strong>and</strong> multiplying those by 100. For Rose, data<br />

were recorded during 33% of the baseline<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s, 58% of the interventi<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>and</strong><br />

100% of the maintenance sessi<strong>on</strong>s. Agreement<br />

was 100% for baseline <strong>and</strong> maintenance<br />

<strong>and</strong> 97% for interventi<strong>on</strong>. For Brittany, data<br />

were recorded during 60% of the baseline<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s, 50% of the interventi<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>and</strong><br />

100% of the maintenance sessi<strong>on</strong>s. Agree-<br />

Video Modeling <strong>and</strong> Prompting / 505


ment was 100% for baseline <strong>and</strong> maintenance<br />

<strong>and</strong> was 96% for interventi<strong>on</strong>. For Wes, data<br />

were recorded during 63% of baseline, 67% of<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> 100% of maintenance sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Agreement was 100% during baseline<br />

<strong>and</strong> maintenance <strong>and</strong> 95% during interventi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

A checklist was developed to assess treatment<br />

integrity during the interventi<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong><br />

to ensure that the student began each<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong> by meeting with the researcher(s),<br />

watching the appropriate video depending <strong>on</strong><br />

the interventi<strong>on</strong> they were receiving for a<br />

given sessi<strong>on</strong> (this included having the students<br />

receive the time delay <strong>and</strong> prompts as<br />

needed), <strong>and</strong> completing the recipe. Treatment<br />

integrity was collected for 33% of the<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s for all three students <strong>and</strong> was 100%<br />

for all of the sessi<strong>on</strong>s for all three of the<br />

students<br />

Results<br />

Figure 1. Percentage of independent correct steps per sessi<strong>on</strong> (Brittany).<br />

Figures 1, 2 <strong>and</strong> 3 dem<strong>on</strong>strate the number of<br />

steps each student was able to complete independently<br />

using video modeling or video<br />

prompting while engaged in cooking tasks.<br />

Visual analysis revealed all students increased<br />

the number of steps they completed independently<br />

when using video prompting <strong>and</strong> modeling<br />

over baseline levels. Visual analysis indicated<br />

Brittany’s level of independence was<br />

higher when using video prompting, while<br />

Rose <strong>and</strong> Wes cooked more independently<br />

when using video modeling.<br />

Brittany. Figure 1 illustrates the percentage<br />

of steps Brittany completed independently<br />

while engaged in cooking tasks. She<br />

completed 58.5% of the steps independently<br />

during baseline 77.8% of during interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

using video prompting <strong>and</strong> 74.3% independently<br />

using video modeling. The difference<br />

between the percentages of steps completed<br />

using the two independent variables was 3.5%<br />

with video prompting resulting in slightly<br />

greater independent performance. A n<strong>on</strong>parametric<br />

analysis (st<strong>and</strong>ardized mean difference<br />

effect size) was used to verify these interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

findings <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>firmed the positive<br />

effect of video prompting over video modeling<br />

for Brittany (d 0.285, r 0.14). During<br />

the follow-up probe using video prompting,<br />

Brittany completed 100% of steps independently.<br />

When video prompting was then withdrawn,<br />

her level of cooking independence<br />

dropped to 90%.<br />

506 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


Figure 2. Percentage of independent correct steps per sessi<strong>on</strong> (Rose).<br />

Rose. Figure 2 illustrates the percentage of<br />

steps she completed independently while engaged<br />

in cooking tasks. Visual analysis indicates<br />

Rose completed tasks more independently<br />

when using video modeling. During<br />

baseline, she completed 52.0% of task analysis<br />

steps independently. However, this increased<br />

during interventi<strong>on</strong> where she completed<br />

78.5% of steps independently using video<br />

prompting <strong>and</strong> 87.0% using video modeling.<br />

To c<strong>on</strong>firm visual analysis findings of interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

data, the st<strong>and</strong>ard mean difference effect<br />

size was calculated verifying video modeling<br />

was slightly more effective than video prompting<br />

for Rose (d 0.587, r 0.28). During the<br />

follow-up probe, Rose c<strong>on</strong>tinued to increase<br />

her percentage of independent task performance<br />

by completing 91.0% of steps independently<br />

using video modeling. When this video<br />

system was withdrawn during the sec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

probe her level of cooking independence increased<br />

to 100%.<br />

Wes. Figure 3 illustrates the percentage<br />

of steps Wes completed independently while<br />

engaged in cooking tasks. Visual analysis indicates<br />

he completed more steps independently<br />

when using video modeling than<br />

when video prompting was used. During<br />

baseline, he completed 42.83% of task analysis<br />

steps independently. This increased to<br />

65.3% when using video prompting <strong>and</strong><br />

77.5% when using video modeling was used<br />

during interventi<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s. The difference<br />

between the percentages of steps completed<br />

using the strategies (e.g., video modeling<br />

<strong>and</strong> video prompting) was 12.2%. To c<strong>on</strong>firm<br />

these findings, a n<strong>on</strong>parametric analysis<br />

(st<strong>and</strong>ardized mean difference effect<br />

size) was c<strong>on</strong>ducted verifying that Wes completed<br />

tasks more independently when using<br />

video modeling (d 0.706, r 0.33).<br />

During the first follow-up probe using video<br />

modeling, Wes c<strong>on</strong>tinued to increase his<br />

level of independent task performance by<br />

completing 91.0% of task analysis steps independently.<br />

However, when this was withdrawn,<br />

his level of cooking independence<br />

dropped to 73%.<br />

Social Validity<br />

Each student was informally interviewed prior<br />

to <strong>and</strong> following the study in order to determine<br />

whether they felt learning to cook was<br />

Video Modeling <strong>and</strong> Prompting / 507


Figure 3. Percentage of independent correct steps per sessi<strong>on</strong> (Wes).<br />

important <strong>and</strong> if video modeling <strong>and</strong> prompting<br />

were effective strategies to use. In additi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

their teacher was also interviewed to c<strong>on</strong>firm<br />

the social validity of cooking skills <strong>and</strong><br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>al procedures used by students.<br />

Students unanimously agreed that learning to<br />

cook was an important skill to learn <strong>and</strong> video<br />

prompting <strong>and</strong> modeling were strategies that<br />

made tasks “easier to do after seeing it.” Students<br />

also reported the availability of the written<br />

recipe in additi<strong>on</strong> to the video prompts<br />

<strong>and</strong> models were important for successfully<br />

completing the cooking activities.<br />

The teacher reported her students loved<br />

using the video iPod technology but was c<strong>on</strong>cerned<br />

about using it herself due to her own<br />

lack of knowledge <strong>on</strong> how to operate the<br />

equipment. However, she indicated her intent<br />

to incorporate the video strategies into her<br />

“cooking curriculum for next year.” Additi<strong>on</strong>ally,<br />

she noted the potential benefits of using<br />

video prompting <strong>and</strong> modeling for students<br />

who were visual learners <strong>and</strong> indicated these<br />

strategies might be beneficial for teaching her<br />

students in other functi<strong>on</strong>al skill areas.<br />

Discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

Ensuring all students with disabilities receive<br />

access to the general educati<strong>on</strong> curriculum is<br />

not <strong>on</strong>ly a legal but ethical obligati<strong>on</strong> for all<br />

educators (Bechard, 2000; Hitchcock, Meyer,<br />

Rose, & Jacks<strong>on</strong>, 2002). Special educators<br />

must also assure students will have access to a<br />

curriculum that facilitates future aut<strong>on</strong>omous<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>ing in school, domestic, work, <strong>and</strong><br />

other community settings (Clark, Field, Patt<strong>on</strong>,<br />

Brolin, & Sitlingt<strong>on</strong>, 1994). As such, a<br />

clear need exists for educati<strong>on</strong> programs serving<br />

students with disabilities, including those<br />

with mild intellectual disabilities, to provide<br />

c<strong>on</strong>current access to the general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

curriculum <strong>and</strong> a functi<strong>on</strong>al curriculum in<br />

order to meet students’ academic <strong>and</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

skill needs. The present investigati<strong>on</strong><br />

compared the effectiveness of video prompting<br />

<strong>and</strong> video modeling delivered via iPod<br />

Nanos when teaching functi<strong>on</strong>al cooking<br />

skills to students with mild intellectual disabilities.<br />

The results indicated each student was<br />

successful in using video prompting <strong>and</strong> video<br />

modeling for independently completing novel<br />

508 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


ecipes <strong>and</strong> improving their accuracy over<br />

baseline levels.<br />

Increased independence was evident for<br />

each student between baseline <strong>and</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s. For students, baseline levels<br />

ranged from 42.8% to 58.5%. During interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

when video prompting was used, independent<br />

performance increased from<br />

65.3% to 78.5%. However, a somewhat higher<br />

percentage was found for students when video<br />

modeling was used with percentages of independence<br />

ranging from 74.3% to 87%. While<br />

video modeling resulted in slightly greater<br />

task performance, up<strong>on</strong> closer examinati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

data, slight differences in student performance<br />

were measured between the two video<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>al methods. For example, both Wes<br />

<strong>and</strong> Rose were able to complete cooking tasks<br />

more independently when using video modeling<br />

whereas Brittany’s performance was<br />

greater when using video prompting. Interestingly,<br />

while each student indicated their preferred<br />

video instructi<strong>on</strong>al system, <strong>on</strong>ly Rose<br />

performed better with her n<strong>on</strong>preferred system,<br />

video modeling. Previous studies examining<br />

student’s preferred instructi<strong>on</strong>al methods<br />

indicate some correlati<strong>on</strong> between<br />

preference <strong>and</strong> performance (Taber-<br />

Doughty, 2005; Taber-Doughty et al., 2008).<br />

However, this remains an area in need of further<br />

validati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Two separate follow-up probes were c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

to determine students’ <strong>on</strong>going level<br />

of cooking independence. The first probe was<br />

c<strong>on</strong>ducted to c<strong>on</strong>firm the effectiveness of the<br />

more effective interventi<strong>on</strong> used during the<br />

previous c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. The sec<strong>on</strong>d probe examined<br />

whether each student’s level of independence<br />

when cooking would return to baseline<br />

levels following withdrawal of the video system.<br />

All three students dem<strong>on</strong>strated a c<strong>on</strong>tinued<br />

level of independence as observed during<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> when using their more<br />

effective video system. When those systems<br />

were removed during the follow-up probe, the<br />

level of independent functi<strong>on</strong>ing decreased<br />

slightly for both Brittany <strong>and</strong> Wes while Rose<br />

increased in her performance level. Thus, students<br />

either improved in their cooking skills<br />

or some carry-over effect existed as a result of<br />

the similarity to the previous recipe completed<br />

when using the video system. Future<br />

research may seek to exp<strong>and</strong> this c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong><br />

over more sessi<strong>on</strong>s to c<strong>on</strong>firm the effects of<br />

the video systems <strong>on</strong> student performance. In<br />

additi<strong>on</strong>, a more accurate measure of student<br />

learning might compare student performance<br />

<strong>on</strong> similar recipes (e.g., similar number of<br />

steps, type of food, equipment needed, <strong>and</strong><br />

preparati<strong>on</strong> steps) previously completed using<br />

a video system as well as novel recipes requiring<br />

<strong>on</strong>ly a few similarities.<br />

While students in the present investigati<strong>on</strong><br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strated increased independence in<br />

cooking <strong>and</strong> following recipes when using<br />

video modeling or video prompting, <strong>on</strong>ly<br />

three students served as participants. Replicati<strong>on</strong><br />

is needed to c<strong>on</strong>firm these results when<br />

used by students who experience mild intellectual<br />

disabilities. In additi<strong>on</strong>, further studies<br />

are needed to validate the use of video<br />

prompting <strong>and</strong> video modeling by students<br />

who experience high <strong>and</strong> low incidence disabilities.<br />

While there is a growing body of<br />

literature dem<strong>on</strong>strating the effectiveness of<br />

these video strategies with students who experience<br />

low incidence disabilities (e.g., Cihak et<br />

al., 2006; Van Laarhoven et al., 2009) <strong>and</strong><br />

autism spectrum disorders (e.g., Charlop-<br />

Christy & Daneshvar, 2003; Charlop-Christy,<br />

Le, & Freeman, 2000), this research is still in<br />

its infancy. When used by students who experience<br />

mild intellectual disabilities, the present<br />

investigati<strong>on</strong> may represent the first to<br />

involve this student populati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Although students in the current study were<br />

generally successful in using video prompting<br />

<strong>and</strong> modeling, <strong>on</strong>e possible limitati<strong>on</strong> to their<br />

immediate success <strong>and</strong> independence may be<br />

attributed to the equipment used to deliver<br />

the videos. While lightweight <strong>and</strong> portable,<br />

the iPod Nanos c<strong>on</strong>tained an extremely small<br />

viewing screen. Students may have experienced<br />

limitati<strong>on</strong>s while completing cooking<br />

activities due to an inability to see video details.<br />

As such, future studies may need to introduce<br />

similar forms of portable equipment<br />

yet with larger screens for delivering video<br />

models <strong>and</strong> prompts. Another potential limitati<strong>on</strong><br />

involves the teacher’s knowledge <strong>and</strong><br />

comfort level with the technology being used<br />

to deliver the interventi<strong>on</strong>. During the present<br />

study, the teacher expressed enthusiasm<br />

about using video prompts <strong>and</strong> modeling with<br />

her students yet was hesitant about using the<br />

iPods dues to her own lack of knowledge in<br />

Video Modeling <strong>and</strong> Prompting / 509


how to program <strong>and</strong> operate the equipment.<br />

As such, future studies should examine how<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>s are selected <strong>and</strong> used based <strong>on</strong><br />

the teacher’s knowledge <strong>and</strong> comfort level. In<br />

additi<strong>on</strong>, a sec<strong>on</strong>d area of study might examine<br />

the amount of training a teacher receives<br />

in the use of technology <strong>and</strong> its subsequent<br />

use in the classroom.<br />

More empirical studies are needed examining<br />

strategies for teaching functi<strong>on</strong>al skills<br />

to students who experience mild intellectual<br />

disabilities. While acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

skills leads to a greater likelihood for future<br />

independence <strong>and</strong> success in school, home<br />

<strong>and</strong> community settings (Browder et al.,<br />

2004; Brown et al., 1979), the declining curricular<br />

focus for students with mild intellectual<br />

disabilities may result in individuals<br />

who struggle to complete basic life skills<br />

(Bouck, 2004b). This study represents <strong>on</strong>e<br />

recent attempt to address functi<strong>on</strong>al programming<br />

with students who experience<br />

mild intellectual disabilities while c<strong>on</strong>currently<br />

incorporating socially desirable technology<br />

during interventi<strong>on</strong>. Future studies<br />

should examine how to integrate new technologies<br />

in addressing skills for students<br />

who c<strong>on</strong>tinue to dem<strong>on</strong>strate a need for<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>al programming. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally,<br />

noted throughout this investigati<strong>on</strong> were<br />

numerous positive comments from peers<br />

who indicated their desire to use the iPod<br />

Nanos to assist them in completing their<br />

work bey<strong>on</strong>d the tasks targeted for interventi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Future studies may also examine the<br />

social validity associated with the various<br />

technologies used to deliver video prompts<br />

<strong>and</strong> models to students in an effort to find<br />

the most effective <strong>and</strong> those c<strong>on</strong>sidered<br />

most socially valid.<br />

Finally, investigators should examine<br />

whether or not the video systems are associated<br />

with the types of tasks targeted for interventi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

For example, does video prompting<br />

or video modeling work better with discrete<br />

trial tasks or those less precise? Can they be<br />

used for tasks with less clear outcomes such as<br />

social interacti<strong>on</strong>s where appropriate resp<strong>on</strong>ses<br />

may vary? Prospective studies might<br />

investigate whether a linkage exists between<br />

the video system used <strong>and</strong> type of task to be<br />

completed.<br />

References<br />

Agran, M., Blanchard, C., Wehmeyer, M., &<br />

Hughes, C. (2002). Increasing the problem-solving<br />

skills of students with developmental disabilities<br />

participating in general educati<strong>on</strong>. Remedial<br />

<strong>and</strong> Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 23, 279–288.<br />

Alberto, P. A., Cihak, D. F., & Gama, R. I. (2005).<br />

Use of static picture prompts versus video modeling<br />

during simulati<strong>on</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>. Research in <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 26, 327–339.<br />

Alwell, M., & Cobb, B. (2009). Functi<strong>on</strong>al life skills<br />

curricular interventi<strong>on</strong>s for youth with disabilities.<br />

Career Development for Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Individuals,<br />

32, 82–93.<br />

Anders<strong>on</strong>, C., Coxs<strong>on</strong>, L., Britt, M., Haugen-<br />

McLane, J., & Mullins, B. (1999). Cooking to learn:<br />

Integrating reading <strong>and</strong> writing activities. San Ant<strong>on</strong>io,<br />

TX: PCI Educati<strong>on</strong>al Publishing.<br />

Anders<strong>on</strong>, C., Coxs<strong>on</strong>, L., Lam<strong>on</strong>tagne, C., Buteyn,<br />

L., & Chapman, D. (2008). Cooking to learn 3:<br />

Recipes from around the world. San Ant<strong>on</strong>io, TX: PCI<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>al Publishing.<br />

Apple Inc. (2010). iMovie (versi<strong>on</strong> 8.0.6). Cupertino,<br />

CA: Apple Inc.<br />

Arnold-Reid, G. S., Schloss, P. J., & Alper, S. (1997).<br />

Teaching meal planning to youth with mental<br />

retardati<strong>on</strong> in natural settings. Remedial <strong>and</strong> Special<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>, 18, 166–173.<br />

Bechard, S. (2000, Oct.). Students with disabilities<br />

<strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ards-based reform. Policy Brief: Mid-C<strong>on</strong>tinent<br />

Research for Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Learning, Office of<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>al Research <strong>and</strong> Improvement, U.S. Department<br />

of Educati<strong>on</strong>, pp. 1–8.<br />

Belm<strong>on</strong>t, J. M. (1966). L<strong>on</strong>g-term memory in mental<br />

retardati<strong>on</strong>. In N. R. Ellis (Ed.), Internati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

review of research in mental retardati<strong>on</strong> (Vol. 7). New<br />

York: Academic.<br />

Billingsley, F. (1997, December). The problem <strong>and</strong><br />

place of functi<strong>on</strong>al skills in inclusive settings. In<br />

G. Singer (Chair), The role of functi<strong>on</strong>al skills <strong>and</strong><br />

behavior instructi<strong>on</strong>al methods in inclusive educati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

The Annual Meeting for Pers<strong>on</strong>s with Severe<br />

H<strong>and</strong>icaps. Bost<strong>on</strong>, MA.<br />

Billingsley, F. F., & Alberts<strong>on</strong>, L. R. (1999). Finding<br />

a future for functi<strong>on</strong>al skills. Journal of the Associati<strong>on</strong><br />

for Pers<strong>on</strong>s with Severe H<strong>and</strong>icaps, 24, 298–302.<br />

Blackorby, J., & Wagner, M. (1996). L<strong>on</strong>gitudinal<br />

post school outcomes of youth with disabilities:<br />

Findings from the nati<strong>on</strong>al l<strong>on</strong>gitudinal transiti<strong>on</strong><br />

study. Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, 62, 399–413.<br />

Bouck, E. C. (2004a). Exploring sec<strong>on</strong>dary special<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> for mild mental impairment: A program<br />

in search of its place. Remedial <strong>and</strong> Special<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>, 25, 367–382.<br />

Bouck, E. C. (2004b). The state of curriculum for<br />

sec<strong>on</strong>dary students with mild mental retardati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

510 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>,<br />

39, 169–176.<br />

Bouck, E. C. (2007). Lost in translati<strong>on</strong>: Educating<br />

sec<strong>on</strong>dary students with mild mental impairment.<br />

Journal of Disability Policy Studies, 18, 79–87.<br />

Bouck, E. C., Bassette, L., Taber-Doughty, T., Flanagan,<br />

S., & Szwed, K. (2009). Pentop computers as<br />

tools for teaching multiplicati<strong>on</strong> to students with<br />

mild intellectual disabilities. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training<br />

in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 44, 367–380.<br />

Bouck, E. C., & Flanagan, S. M. (2010). Functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

curriculum – Evidence-based educati<strong>on</strong>?: C<strong>on</strong>sidering<br />

sec<strong>on</strong>dary students with mild intellectual<br />

disabilities. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 45, 487–499.<br />

Browder, D., Flowers, C., Ahlgrim-Delzell, L., Karv<strong>on</strong>en,<br />

M., Spo<strong>on</strong>er, F., & Algozzine, R. (2004).<br />

The alignment of alternate assessment c<strong>on</strong>tent<br />

with academic <strong>and</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>al curricula. The Journal<br />

of Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 37, 211–223.<br />

Brown, L., Br<strong>on</strong>st<strong>on</strong>, M. B., Hamre-Nietupski, S.,<br />

Pumpian, I., Certo, N. & Gruenewald, L. (1979).<br />

A strategy for developing chr<strong>on</strong>ological age-appropriate<br />

<strong>and</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>al curricular c<strong>on</strong>tent for<br />

severely h<strong>and</strong>icapped adolescents <strong>and</strong> young<br />

adults. Journal of Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 13, 81–90.<br />

Cannella-Mal<strong>on</strong>e, H., Sigafoos, J., O’Reilly, M., de la<br />

Cruz, B., Edrisinha, C., & Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, G. E. (2006).<br />

Comparing video prompting to video modeling<br />

for teaching daily living skills to six adults with<br />

developmental disabilities. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training<br />

in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 41, 344–356.<br />

Charlop-Christy, M. H., & Daneshvar, S. (2003).<br />

Using video modeling to teach perspective taking<br />

to children with autism. Journal of Positive Behavior<br />

Interventi<strong>on</strong>s, 5, 12–21.<br />

Charlop-Christy, M. H., Le, L., & Freeman, K. A.<br />

(2000). A comparis<strong>on</strong> of video modeling with in<br />

vivo modeling for teaching children with autism.<br />

Journal of <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> Disorders, 30,<br />

537–552.<br />

Cihak, D., Alberto, P. A., Taber-Doughty, T., &<br />

Gama, R. I. (2006). A comparis<strong>on</strong> of static picture<br />

prompting <strong>and</strong> video prompting simulati<strong>on</strong> strategies<br />

using group instructi<strong>on</strong>al procedures. Focus<br />

<strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Other <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 21,<br />

89–99.<br />

Cihak, D. F., Kessler, K. B., & Alberto, P. A. (2007).<br />

Generalized use of a h<strong>and</strong>held prompting system.<br />

Research in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 28, 397–408.<br />

Clark, G. M., Field, S., Patt<strong>on</strong>, J. R., Brolin, D. E., &<br />

Sitlingt<strong>on</strong>, P. L. (1994). Life skills instructi<strong>on</strong>: A<br />

necessary comp<strong>on</strong>ent for all students with disabilities.<br />

A positi<strong>on</strong> statement of the <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> Career<br />

Development <strong>and</strong> Transiti<strong>on</strong>. Career Development<br />

for Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Individuals, 17, 125–124.<br />

Collins, B. C., Brans<strong>on</strong>, T. A., & Hall, M. (1995).<br />

Teaching generalized reading of cooking product<br />

labels to adolescents with mental disabilities<br />

through the use of key words taught by peer<br />

tutors. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 30, 65–76.<br />

Collins, C. B., & Stins<strong>on</strong>, D. M. (1995). Teaching<br />

generalized reading of product warning labels to<br />

adolescents with mental disabilities through the<br />

use of key words. Excepti<strong>on</strong>ality, 5, 163–181.<br />

Coxs<strong>on</strong>, L. & Anders<strong>on</strong>, C. (2001). Cooking to learn 2:<br />

Integrating reading <strong>and</strong> writing activities. San Ant<strong>on</strong>io,<br />

TX: PCI Educati<strong>on</strong>al Publishing.<br />

Cr<strong>on</strong>in, M. E. (1996). Life skills curricula for students<br />

with learning disabilities: A review of the<br />

literature. Journal of Learning <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 29, 53–<br />

68.<br />

D’Ateno, P., Mangiapanello, K., & Taylor, B. A.<br />

(2003). Using video modeling to teach complex<br />

play sequences to a preschooler with autism. Journal<br />

of Positive Behavior Interventi<strong>on</strong>s, 5, 5–11.<br />

Denny, P. J., & Test, D. W. (1995). The One-More-<br />

Than technique to teach m<strong>on</strong>ey counting to individuals<br />

with moderate mental retardati<strong>on</strong>: A<br />

systematic replicati<strong>on</strong>. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Treatment of<br />

Children, 18, 422–432.<br />

Dunn, L. M. (1973). An overview. In Lloyd M.<br />

Dunn, (Ed.), Excepti<strong>on</strong>al children in the schools: Special<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> in transiti<strong>on</strong> (2nd ed.). New York:<br />

Holt, Rinehart <strong>and</strong> Winst<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Edgar, E. (1987). Sec<strong>on</strong>dary programs in special<br />

educati<strong>on</strong>: Are many of them justifiable? Excepti<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Children, 53, 555–561.<br />

Edyburn, D. L. (1991). Fact retrieval by students<br />

with <strong>and</strong> without learning h<strong>and</strong>icaps using print<br />

<strong>and</strong> electr<strong>on</strong>ic encyclopedias. Journal of Special<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> Technology, 11, 75–90.<br />

Fazio, B. B., & Polsgrove, L. (1989). An evaluati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

the effects of training special educators to integrate<br />

microcomputer technology into match curricula.<br />

Journal of Special Educati<strong>on</strong> Technology,<br />

10(2), 5–13.<br />

Geiger, K. B., LeBlanc, L. A., Dill<strong>on</strong>, C. M., & Bates,<br />

S. L. (2010). An evaluati<strong>on</strong> of preference for<br />

video <strong>and</strong> in vivo modeling. Journal of Applied<br />

Behavior Analysis, 43, 279–283.<br />

Graves, T. B., Collins, B. C., Schuster, J. W., & Kleinert,<br />

H. (2005). Using video prompting to teach<br />

cooking skills to sec<strong>on</strong>dary students with moderate<br />

disabilities. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 40, 34–<str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>.<br />

Hitchcock, C., Meyer, A., Rose, D., & Jacks<strong>on</strong>, R.<br />

(2002). Providing new access to the general curriculum:<br />

Universal design for learning. Teaching<br />

Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, 35(2), 8–17.<br />

Hutchers<strong>on</strong>, K., Lang<strong>on</strong>e, J., Ayers, K. M., & Clees,<br />

T. (2004). Computer assisted instructi<strong>on</strong> to teach<br />

item selecti<strong>on</strong> in grocery stores: Assessment of<br />

acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong>. Journal of Special<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> Technology, 19, 33–42.<br />

Video Modeling <strong>and</strong> Prompting / 511


Individuals with <strong>Disabilities</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong> Improvement<br />

Act of 2003, 31 U.S.C. (2004).<br />

Kaye, H. S. (1997, July). Disability statistics abstract:<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> of children with disabilities, Number 19. San<br />

Francisco, CA: Disability Statistics Rehabilitati<strong>on</strong><br />

Research <strong>and</strong> Training Center, University of California,<br />

San Francisco.<br />

Kennedy, C. H. (2005). Single-case designs for educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

research. Bost<strong>on</strong>: Pears<strong>on</strong>/Allyn <strong>and</strong> Bac<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Kennedy, C. H., Itk<strong>on</strong>en, T. L., & Lindquist, K.<br />

(1994). Modality effects during equivalence class<br />

formati<strong>on</strong>s: An extensi<strong>on</strong> of sight word reading<br />

<strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>cept development. Journal of Applied Behavior<br />

Analysis, 27, 673–684.<br />

Kirk, S. A. (1972). Educating excepti<strong>on</strong>al children. Bost<strong>on</strong>:<br />

Hought<strong>on</strong> Mifflin.<br />

Krantz, P. J., MacDuff, G. S., Wadstrom, O., & Mc-<br />

Clannahan, L. E. (1991). Using video with developmentally<br />

disabled learners. In P. W. Dowrick<br />

(Ed.), A practical guide to using video in the behavioral<br />

sciences. New York: John Wiley & S<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, G. E., Dijkstra, A. W., O’Reilly, M. F.,<br />

Groeneweg, J., & Van den Hof, E. (2000). Frequent<br />

versus n<strong>on</strong>frequent verbal prompts delivered<br />

unobtrusively: Their impact <strong>on</strong> the task performance<br />

of adults with intellectual disability.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 35, 428–433.<br />

Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, G. E., Van den Hof, E., Boelens, H.,<br />

Rocha, N., & Seedhouse, P. (1998). A computerbased<br />

system providing pictorial instructi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong><br />

prompts to promote task performance in pers<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with severe developmental disabilities. Behavioral<br />

Interventi<strong>on</strong>s, 13, 111–122.<br />

Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, G. E., Van den Hof, E., Furniss, F.,<br />

O’Reilly, M. F., & Cunha, B. (1999). Evaluati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

a computer-aided system providing pictorial task<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> prompts to people with severe<br />

intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual Disability,<br />

43, 61–66.<br />

Lin, A., Podell, D. M., & Rein, N. (1991). The effects<br />

of CAI <strong>on</strong> word recogniti<strong>on</strong> in mildly mentally<br />

h<strong>and</strong>icapped <strong>and</strong> n<strong>on</strong>h<strong>and</strong>icapped learners. Journal<br />

of Special Educati<strong>on</strong> Technology, 11(1), 16–25.<br />

Luckass<strong>on</strong>, R., Borthwick-Duffy, S., Buntinx, W.,<br />

Buntinx, H. E., Coulter, D. L., Craig, E. M., et al.<br />

(2002). Definiti<strong>on</strong>s, classificati<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>and</strong> support systems<br />

(10 th ed.). Washingt<strong>on</strong>, DC: American Associati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>on</strong> Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Lynch, E. C., & Beare, P. L. (1990). The quality of<br />

IEP objectives <strong>and</strong> their relevance to instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

for students with mental retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> behavioral<br />

disorders. Remedial <strong>and</strong> Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 11,<br />

48–55.<br />

Margalit, M. (1995). Effects of social skills training<br />

for students with an intellectual disability. Internati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Journal of Disability, Development <strong>and</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

42, 75–85.<br />

Mechling, L. (2003). Effects of multimedia, computer-based<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> grocery shopping fluency.<br />

Journal of Special Educati<strong>on</strong> Technology, 19, 23–34.<br />

Mechling, L. (2005). The effect of instructor-created<br />

video programs to teach students with disabilities:<br />

A literature review. Journal of Special Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

Technology, 20, 25–36.<br />

Mechling, L. C., Gast, D. L., & Barthold, S. (2003).<br />

Multimedia computer-based instructi<strong>on</strong> to teach<br />

students with moderate intellectual disabilities to<br />

use a debit card to make purchases. Excepti<strong>on</strong>ality,<br />

11, 239–254.<br />

Mechling, L. C., Gast, D. L., & Lang<strong>on</strong>e, J. (2002).<br />

Computer-based video instructi<strong>on</strong> to teach pers<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with moderate intellectual disabilities to<br />

read grocery aisle signs <strong>and</strong> locate items. Journal of<br />

Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 35, 224–240.<br />

Newman, L. (2005). Family involvement in the educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

development of youth with disabilities. Menlo<br />

Park, CA. SRI Internati<strong>on</strong>al.<br />

Newman, L., Wagner, M., Cameto, R., & Knokey,<br />

A. M. (2009). The post-high school outcomes of youth<br />

with disabilities up to 4 years after high school. A report<br />

of findings from the Nati<strong>on</strong>al L<strong>on</strong>gitudinal Transiti<strong>on</strong><br />

Study-2 (NLTS2) (NCSER 2009–3017). Menlo Park,<br />

CA: SRI Internati<strong>on</strong>al.<br />

Nietupski, J., Hamre-Nietupski, S., Curtin, S., &<br />

Shrikanth, K. (1997). A review of curricular research<br />

in severe disabilities from 1976 to 1995 in<br />

six selected journals. Journal of Special Educati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

31, 36–55.<br />

Nikopoulos, C. K., & Keenan, M. (2004). Effects of<br />

video modeling <strong>on</strong> social initiati<strong>on</strong>s by children<br />

with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 37,<br />

93–96.<br />

No Child Left Behind Act of 2001, Pub. L. No.<br />

107–110, 115 Stat. 1425 (2002).<br />

Patt<strong>on</strong>, J. R., Polloway, E. A., & Smith, T. E. C.<br />

(2000). Educating students with mild mental retardati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Focus <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Other <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 15, 80–89.<br />

Patt<strong>on</strong>, J. R., Cr<strong>on</strong>in, M. E., & Jairrels, V. (1997).<br />

Curricular implicati<strong>on</strong>s of transiti<strong>on</strong>: Lifeskills as<br />

an integral part of transiti<strong>on</strong> educati<strong>on</strong>. Remedial<br />

<strong>and</strong> Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 18, 294–306.<br />

Polloway, E. A. (2004). A eulogy for MMI. DDD<br />

Express, 14, pp. 1, 8.<br />

Polloway, E. A. (2005). Mild retardati<strong>on</strong>: The status<br />

of a category of excepti<strong>on</strong>ality. In J. J. Hoover, &<br />

R. Hills (Eds.), 21 st century issues in special educati<strong>on</strong>:<br />

Meeting diverse needs (pp. 35–<str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>). Boulder:<br />

University of Colorado, BUENO Center.<br />

Polloway, E. A. (2006). Mild mental retardati<strong>on</strong>: A<br />

c<strong>on</strong>cept in search of clarity, a populati<strong>on</strong> in<br />

search of appropriate educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> supports, a<br />

professi<strong>on</strong> in search of advocacy. Excepti<strong>on</strong>ality,<br />

14, 183–190.<br />

Polloway, E. A., Patt<strong>on</strong>, J. R., Smith, T. E. C., & Buck,<br />

512 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


G. H. (1997). Mental retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> learning<br />

disabilities: C<strong>on</strong>ceptual <strong>and</strong> applied issues. Journal<br />

of Learning <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 30, 297–308, 345.<br />

Reschly, D. J. (2002). Change dynamics in special<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> assessment: Historical <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>temporary<br />

patterns. Peabody Journal of Educati<strong>on</strong>, 77, 117–<br />

136.<br />

Spitz, H. H. (1973). C<strong>on</strong>solidating facts into the<br />

schematized learning <strong>and</strong> memory of educable<br />

retardates. In N. R. Ellis (Eds.), Internati<strong>on</strong>al Review<br />

of Research in Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong>, Vol. 6 (pp.<br />

149–168). New York: Academic Press.<br />

Stephens, W. E. (1972). Equivalence formati<strong>on</strong> by<br />

retarded <strong>and</strong> n<strong>on</strong>retarded children at different<br />

mental ages. American Journal of Mental Deficiency,<br />

77, 311–313.<br />

Taber-Doughty, T. (2005). C<strong>on</strong>sidering student<br />

choice when selecting instructi<strong>on</strong>al strategies: A<br />

comparis<strong>on</strong> of three prompting systems. Research<br />

in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 26, 411–432.<br />

Taber-Doughty, T., Patt<strong>on</strong>, S. E., & Brennan, S.<br />

(2008). Simultaneous <strong>and</strong> delayed video modeling:<br />

An examinati<strong>on</strong> of system effectiveness <strong>and</strong><br />

student preferences. Journal of Special Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

Technology, 23(1), 2–18.<br />

Thomas, G. E. (1996). Teaching students with mental<br />

retardati<strong>on</strong>: A life goal curriculum planning guide.<br />

Columbus, OH: Merrill.<br />

Van Laarhoven, T., Johns<strong>on</strong>, J. W., Van Laarhoven-<br />

Myers, T., Grider, K. L., & Grider, K. M. (2009).<br />

The effectiveness of using a video iPod as a<br />

prompting device in employment settings. Journal<br />

of Behavioral Educati<strong>on</strong>, 18, 119–141.<br />

Van Laarhoven, T., & Van Laarhoven-Myers, T.<br />

(2006). Comparis<strong>on</strong> of three video-based instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

procedures for teaching daily living skills to<br />

pers<strong>on</strong>s with developmental disabilities. Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 41, 365–<br />

381.<br />

Wert, B. Y., & Neisworth, J. T. (2003). Effects of<br />

video self-modeling <strong>on</strong> sp<strong>on</strong>taneous requesting in<br />

children with autism. Journal of Positive Behavior<br />

Interventi<strong>on</strong>s, 5, 30–34.<br />

Zeaman, D., & House, B. J. (1963). The role of<br />

attenti<strong>on</strong> in retardate discriminati<strong>on</strong> learning. In<br />

N. R. Ellis (Ed.), H<strong>and</strong>book of mental deficiency:<br />

Psychological theory <strong>and</strong> research (pp. 159–223).<br />

Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Zeaman, D., & House, B. J. (1979). A review of<br />

attenti<strong>on</strong> theory. In N. R. Ellis (Ed.), H<strong>and</strong>book of<br />

mental deficiency: Psychological theory <strong>and</strong> research<br />

(pp. 63–120). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Received: 29 September 2010<br />

Initial Acceptance: 2 December 2010<br />

Final Acceptance: 22 January 2011<br />

Video Modeling <strong>and</strong> Prompting / 513


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 2011, <str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>(4), 514-527<br />

© <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Review of Video Prompting Studies with Pers<strong>on</strong>s with<br />

<strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Devender R. B<strong>and</strong>a<br />

Texas Tech University<br />

Rose Marie Matuszny<br />

Appalachian State University<br />

Maud S. Dogoe<br />

St. Cloud State University<br />

Abstract: We reviewed 18 video prompting studies that were c<strong>on</strong>ducted with pers<strong>on</strong>s with developmental<br />

disabilities. Results across the studies indicate that video prompting is a viable method for improving various<br />

domestic, vocati<strong>on</strong>al, <strong>and</strong> independent living skills. In additi<strong>on</strong>, video prompting strategies facilitated<br />

maintenance <strong>and</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong> of learned skills. Also, in several studies when teaching various skills, video<br />

promptings strategies were more effective than static pictures or video models al<strong>on</strong>e. We discuss the results <strong>and</strong><br />

make suggesti<strong>on</strong>s for future researchers <strong>and</strong> practiti<strong>on</strong>ers.<br />

Use of prompts is <strong>on</strong>e of the most important<br />

tools used in teaching students with disabilities,<br />

especially those with developmental disabilities<br />

(Wolery & Gast, 1984; Wolery, Gast,<br />

Kirk, & Schuster, 1988). Prompts are events<br />

that, when added to instructi<strong>on</strong>, increase the<br />

chances that the student will make a correct<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se (Collins, 2007; Wolery, Ault, &<br />

Doyle, 1992). Prompting techniques are designed<br />

to facilitate acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of skills, minimize<br />

errors, <strong>and</strong> ensure correct resp<strong>on</strong>ding.<br />

Prompts, therefore, help learners perform behaviors<br />

or skills they did not know <strong>and</strong> decrease<br />

the chances for errors. Prompts are<br />

either classified by the sensory modality (auditory,<br />

visual, <strong>and</strong> verbal) by which students<br />

receive the assistance or by the types of behaviors<br />

teachers engage in to provide assistance<br />

(Wolery et al.) <strong>and</strong> can be presented in any<br />

combinati<strong>on</strong> (Cooper, Her<strong>on</strong>, & Heward,<br />

2007). Visual prompts have been found to be<br />

effective for individuals with developmental<br />

disabilities because they are more permanent<br />

Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence c<strong>on</strong>cerning this article should<br />

be addressed to Devender B<strong>and</strong>a, Associate Professor<br />

of Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, Department of Educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Psychology <strong>and</strong> Leadership, College of Educati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

PO Box 41071, Texas Tech University,<br />

Lubbock, TX, 79409. Email: devender.b<strong>and</strong>a@<br />

ttu.edu<br />

<strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>crete than transient auditory prompts<br />

(Quill, 1997). Bryan <strong>and</strong> Gast (2000) suggested<br />

that visual prompts can be presented<br />

through a variety of means including photographs,<br />

texts, pictures/line drawings, <strong>and</strong> symbols.<br />

Over the years, visual prompts have been<br />

presented through the use of texts or static<br />

pictures. In recent years however, video-based<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>al procedures have been widely<br />

used to teach a range of adaptive skills <strong>and</strong><br />

behaviors to individuals with developmental<br />

disabilities. Two types of video-based methods<br />

have been used in the literature for pers<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with developmental disabilities: video modeling<br />

<strong>and</strong> video prompting. In video modeling,<br />

the learner watches the video of a model performing<br />

the entire target skill or task prior to<br />

being provided the opportunity to perform<br />

the target task (Cannella-Mal<strong>on</strong>e et al., 2006).<br />

Video prompting <strong>on</strong> the other h<strong>and</strong> involves<br />

the learner watching each step or task in the<br />

chain <strong>and</strong> performing the step before advancing<br />

to the next task in the chain (Sigafoos et<br />

al., 2007). There is indicati<strong>on</strong> from the literature<br />

that video prompting might be more effective<br />

for some pers<strong>on</strong>s with developmental<br />

disabilities who have difficulty watching<br />

lengthy videos, as it does not require the same<br />

cognitive load (i.e., remembering the sequence<br />

of steps in the target behavior) as is<br />

514 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


necessary in video modeling for such students<br />

(Sigafoos et al.). Furthermore, literature <strong>on</strong><br />

skill-based instructi<strong>on</strong> indicates that pers<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with developmental disabilities acquire skills<br />

when tasks are presented in small increments,<br />

multiple opportunities are provided to perform<br />

the steps, <strong>and</strong> when using various<br />

prompting <strong>and</strong> fading strategies (see Giangreco,<br />

2011; Snell, 2007). Thus, the video<br />

prompting strategy might be more useful for<br />

pers<strong>on</strong>s with moderate <strong>and</strong> severe developmental<br />

disabilities.<br />

Several literature reviews have been c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

<strong>on</strong> video modeling studies with pers<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with autism <strong>and</strong> other developmental disabilities<br />

(see Baker, Lang, & O’Reilly, 2009;<br />

Bellini & Akullian, 2007; Delano, 2007; Coy &<br />

Hermarisen, 2007; Rayner, Denholm, & Sigafoos,<br />

2009; Shukla-Mehta, Miller, & Callahan,<br />

2010). Results across reviews overwhelmingly<br />

indicate that video modeling strategies are<br />

useful in teaching social, communicati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong><br />

self-care skills for individuals with autism <strong>and</strong><br />

other developmental disabilities. Since the<br />

1990s there have been numerous studies c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

using the video prompting strategy to<br />

teach various skills to pers<strong>on</strong>s with developmental<br />

disabilities. However, no review has<br />

been found that specifically analyzed video<br />

prompting studies.<br />

It is therefore important that practiti<strong>on</strong>ers<br />

<strong>and</strong> researchers have the current knowledge<br />

base about the video prompting strategy.<br />

Thus, we c<strong>on</strong>ducted the review of video<br />

prompting studies that were c<strong>on</strong>ducted with<br />

pers<strong>on</strong>s with developmental disabilities. Specifically,<br />

we answered the following questi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

in this study: (a) was video prompting an effective<br />

method for teaching skills to pers<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with developmental disabilities?; (b) was the<br />

video prompting strategy more effective compared<br />

to other interventi<strong>on</strong>s?; (c) were the<br />

skills learned through video prompting maintained<br />

in the absence of interventi<strong>on</strong>?; (d)<br />

were the skills learned through video prompting<br />

generalized across settings, pers<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

<strong>and</strong>/or behaviors?; <strong>and</strong>, (e) what was the social<br />

validity of the video prompting strategy?<br />

Method<br />

We searched EBSCO databases which included<br />

PsychInfo, ERIC, Social Science Index,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Psychological Abstracts using the following<br />

terms: video <strong>and</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong>, video prompting,<br />

video modeling, video instructi<strong>on</strong>, developmental<br />

disabilities, intellectual disabilities, mental<br />

retardati<strong>on</strong>, autism, technology, teaching strategies,<br />

<strong>and</strong> multimedia instructi<strong>on</strong>. We selected studies<br />

that met the following criteria: (a) researchers<br />

implemented the video prompting interventi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

(b) studies included at least <strong>on</strong>e or more<br />

participants with developmental disabilities,<br />

<strong>and</strong> (c) published in a peer-reviewed journal<br />

between years 1990 <strong>and</strong> 2010. We also c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

an ancestral search for additi<strong>on</strong>al studies<br />

under the reference secti<strong>on</strong> of each study<br />

that we found. Overall, we selected 18 studies<br />

that met our criteria. We analyzed the selected<br />

studies <strong>on</strong> several variables including the demographics,<br />

target skills, designs, interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

effectiveness, maintenance, generalizati<strong>on</strong>, social<br />

validity, etc. Table 1 provides a detailed<br />

summary of the several variables that were<br />

analyzed.<br />

Results<br />

Participants<br />

The 18 studies included 68 participants. All<br />

participants were diagnosed with intellectual<br />

disability/mental retardati<strong>on</strong> ranging from<br />

mild to severe. A majority of participants were<br />

diagnosed with a moderate intellectual disability<br />

except for <strong>on</strong>e who was diagnosed with<br />

severe intellectual disability (see Grice &<br />

Blampied, 1994). In additi<strong>on</strong>, some participants<br />

were diagnosed with additi<strong>on</strong>al disabilities<br />

such as autism, ADHD, physical disabilities,<br />

Down Syndrome, Aspergers, behavior<br />

disorder, Tourette, seizures, <strong>and</strong> Williams Syndrome.<br />

Participants’ ages ranged from 8 years<br />

to 41 years, with an average age of 21.5 years.<br />

Settings<br />

Researchers have c<strong>on</strong>ducted studies in various<br />

settings, including: a special school for students<br />

with intellectual disabilities (Grice &<br />

Blampied, 1994); resource room/self-c<strong>on</strong>tained<br />

classrooms (Graves, Collins, Schuster,<br />

& Kleinert, 2005; Norman, Collins, & Schuster,<br />

2001); a c<strong>on</strong>ference room in a high school<br />

(Mechling, Gast, & Barthold, 2003); a home<br />

living room in a high school (Mechling, Gast,<br />

Video Prompting <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong> / 515


TABLE 1<br />

Video Prompting Studies C<strong>on</strong>ducted with Pers<strong>on</strong>s with <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Participants<br />

Effective/Not<br />

Effective<br />

Mode of<br />

Presentati<strong>on</strong><br />

Setting Target Skill Design<br />

Age Disability<br />

Author<br />

Computer Yes for all<br />

Multiple probe across<br />

subjects with<br />

alternating treatment<br />

Putting away<br />

grocery<br />

Table setting<br />

Kitchen of a vocati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

center<br />

Mild ID, AUT<br />

Mod. ID, AUT<br />

Mod. ID<br />

Asperger,<br />

Mod. ID<br />

Mild ID, AUT<br />

Mod. ID, AUT<br />

27 y<br />

28 y<br />

32 y<br />

36 y<br />

Cannela-Mal<strong>on</strong>e et<br />

al. (2006)<br />

36 y<br />

41 y<br />

Yes, as effective<br />

as using static<br />

picture<br />

5-ft, 8-in. screen<br />

projected from<br />

an Eps<strong>on</strong><br />

Powerlite S1<br />

Adapted alternating<br />

treatment<br />

Using debit card<br />

to withdraw<br />

m<strong>on</strong>ey from<br />

ATM to buy<br />

items<br />

Classroom <strong>and</strong> grocery<br />

store<br />

Mod. ID<br />

Mod. ID<br />

Mod. ID<br />

Mod. ID<br />

Mild. ID<br />

11 y<br />

12 y<br />

12 y<br />

12 y<br />

12 y<br />

11 y<br />

Cihak, et al.<br />

(2006)<br />

Mod. ID<br />

Computer 1 subject reached<br />

criteri<strong>on</strong> with VP<br />

al<strong>on</strong>e. All<br />

reached criteri<strong>on</strong><br />

with VP plus<br />

error correcti<strong>on</strong><br />

Setting table Multiple baseline<br />

across participants<br />

Dining area of<br />

vocati<strong>on</strong>al center<br />

AUT, Mod.<br />

MR<br />

Mod. MR<br />

AUT, Mild<br />

MR<br />

AUT, Mod.<br />

MR<br />

33 y<br />

Goods<strong>on</strong> et al.<br />

(2007)<br />

36 y<br />

36 y<br />

34 y<br />

VCR, TV Yes, 2 of 3 target<br />

skills<br />

Cooking Multiple probe across<br />

Behaviors<br />

Kitchen area of<br />

classroom<br />

Mod. ID<br />

Mod. ID<br />

Graves et al. (2005) 16 y<br />

18 y<br />

516 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011<br />

Mod. ID<br />

20 y<br />

Yes, 2<br />

participants with<br />

VP<br />

Third participant<br />

with VP least<br />

to most prompt<br />

Laundry skills Multiple baseline<br />

across participants<br />

Laundry room in a<br />

group home<br />

MR, AUT<br />

DD, ADHD<br />

MR, AUT,<br />

Behavior<br />

Disorder<br />

Horn et al. (2008) 29 y<br />

17 y<br />

25 y


TABLE 1—(C<strong>on</strong>tinued)<br />

Video Prompting Studies C<strong>on</strong>ducted with Pers<strong>on</strong>s with <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Participants<br />

Effective/Not<br />

Effective<br />

Mode of<br />

Presentati<strong>on</strong><br />

Setting Target Skill Design<br />

Age Disability<br />

Author<br />

Classroom Technology AB Televisi<strong>on</strong> Yes for All<br />

Mod. ID<br />

Sev. ID<br />

Mod. ID<br />

Mod. ID<br />

Mod. ID, Tourette<br />

Mod. ID, Phy. Dis<br />

Mod. ID, ADHD<br />

13 y<br />

13 y<br />

15 y<br />

18 y<br />

16y 1m<br />

17y 11m<br />

18y 7m<br />

Le Grice & Blampied<br />

(1994)<br />

Computer Yes for all<br />

Multiple probe across<br />

participants<br />

Purchasing<br />

using a debit<br />

C<strong>on</strong>ference room in a<br />

high school<br />

Mechling et al.<br />

(2003)<br />

card<br />

Yes for all<br />

Portable DVD<br />

player<br />

Dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> kitchen Cooking Multiple probe across<br />

tasks<br />

19 y<br />

20y 2m<br />

Mechling et al.<br />

(2008)<br />

22y 3m<br />

Yes<br />

Pers<strong>on</strong>al Digital<br />

Assistant (PDA)<br />

Home-living classroom cooking Multiple probe across<br />

skills<br />

16y 4m<br />

17y 4m<br />

Mechling et al.<br />

(2009)<br />

17y 10m<br />

Video prompting<br />

was more effective<br />

compared to static<br />

pictures for all 6<br />

participants<br />

Portable DVD<br />

player<br />

Adapted alternating<br />

treatment<br />

Cooking-related<br />

tasks<br />

Home living room in a<br />

high school<br />

Mod. ID<br />

Mod. ID, DS<br />

Mod. ID<br />

Mild ID, AUT<br />

Mod. ID, AUT<br />

Mod. ID, AUT<br />

Mod .AUT<br />

Mild. AUT<br />

Mod. AUT<br />

Mod. AUT<br />

Mod. AUT<br />

Mod. AUT<br />

Mod. ID,<br />

AD/HD, Seizure<br />

Mod. ID, DS<br />

Mod. ID<br />

Mod. ID<br />

Mod. ID<br />

Mod. ID, AD/HD<br />

17y 8m<br />

17y 10m<br />

21y 1m<br />

16y 3m<br />

15y 10m<br />

16y 8m<br />

18y 7m<br />

Mechling &<br />

Gustafs<strong>on</strong> (2008)<br />

Video prompting<br />

was more<br />

effective<br />

compared to<br />

static pictures for<br />

all 6 participants<br />

Portable DVD<br />

player<br />

Adapted alternating<br />

treatment<br />

Apartment Cooking-related<br />

tasks<br />

Mechling &<br />

Gustafs<strong>on</strong> (2009)<br />

20y 5m<br />

22y 1m<br />

21y 2m<br />

19y 3m<br />

22y 6m<br />

Video Prompting <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong> / 517


TABLE 1—(C<strong>on</strong>tinued)<br />

Video Prompting Studies C<strong>on</strong>ducted with Pers<strong>on</strong>s with <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Participants<br />

Effective/Not<br />

Effective<br />

Mode of<br />

Presentati<strong>on</strong><br />

Setting Target Skill Design<br />

Age Disability<br />

Author<br />

Video prompting<br />

was more effective<br />

compared to<br />

picture-based<br />

cookbooks for all<br />

4 participants<br />

Two of 3<br />

participants<br />

learned three tasks<br />

<strong>and</strong> the third <strong>on</strong>e<br />

two tasks.<br />

Portable DVD<br />

player<br />

Adapted alternating<br />

treatment<br />

Apartment Cooking-related<br />

tasks<br />

Mod. ID<br />

Mod. ID<br />

Mod. ID<br />

Mod. ID, William<br />

Syndrome<br />

20y 10m<br />

19y 9m<br />

19y 11m<br />

22y 6m<br />

Mechling & Stephens<br />

(2009)<br />

TV –VCR<br />

Combo<br />

Classroom Self-help skills Multiple-probe across<br />

behaviors <strong>and</strong><br />

participants<br />

Mod. ID DS<br />

Mild ID, DS<br />

Mod. ID, AUT<br />

Norman et al. (2001) 8y 1m<br />

9y 8m<br />

12y 3m<br />

Computer Yes for 2 of 3<br />

Delayed multiple<br />

probe<br />

A-B-A-follow-up<br />

Making a bag of<br />

popcorn in<br />

Microwave<br />

Kitchen of a vocati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

center<br />

Mod ID<br />

Mod ID<br />

Mod ID, AUT<br />

Yes for All<br />

Portable<br />

computer<br />

Dishwashing Multiple baseline<br />

across participants<br />

Kitchen of a vocati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

center<br />

Mild ID, AUT<br />

AUT<br />

Mod AUT<br />

Sigafoos et al. (2005) 34 y<br />

36 y<br />

36 y<br />

Sigafoos et al. (2007) 27 y<br />

28 y<br />

33 y<br />

Mod. ID, AUT<br />

Multiple probe M<strong>on</strong>itor Yes for all<br />

Exiting skills in<br />

case of fire<br />

Bedroom & hallway of<br />

a group home<br />

Mild ID<br />

—<br />

Mod. ID<br />

Mod. ID<br />

Video iPod Yes for all three<br />

tasks<br />

Ti<strong>on</strong>g et al. (1992) 21 y<br />

26 y<br />

30 y<br />

35 y<br />

Van Laarhoven et al.<br />

(2009)<br />

17 y Mod. ID Animal shelter Job-related tasks Multiple probe across<br />

(e.g., cleaning, tasks<br />

mopping,<br />

emptying<br />

garbage)<br />

18y Mod MR, AUT Home<br />

Domestic skills Adapted alternating<br />

17y to 19y Mod. MR classroom<br />

treatment<br />

17y to 19y Mod. MR<br />

518 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011<br />

Computer For 2 of 3<br />

participants video<br />

rehearsal plus<br />

video prompting<br />

more effective<br />

Van Laarhoven &<br />

Van Laarhoven-<br />

Myers (2006)<br />

AD/HD attenti<strong>on</strong> deficit/hyperactivity disorder; AUT <strong>Autism</strong>; DS Down Syndrome; ID intellectual disability; Mod Moderate, MR mental retardati<strong>on</strong>;<br />

Phy. Dis. physical disability.


& Seid, 2009; Mechling & Gustafs<strong>on</strong>, 2008); a<br />

resource room <strong>and</strong> grocery store (Cihak, Alberto,<br />

Taber-Doughty, & Gama, 2006); a community<br />

house (Ti<strong>on</strong>g, Blampied, & le Grice,<br />

1992); the laundry room in a group home<br />

(Horn et al., 2008); the kitchen of a vocati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

training center (Cannella-Mal<strong>on</strong>e et al., 2006;<br />

Goods<strong>on</strong>, Sigafoos, O’ Reilly, Cannella, &<br />

Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, 2007; Sigafoos et al., 2007; Sigafoos<br />

et al., 2005); an apartment kitchen (Mechling,<br />

Gast, & Fields, 2008; Mechling & Gustafs<strong>on</strong>,<br />

2009; Mechling & Stephens, 2009); <strong>and</strong> an<br />

animal shelter (Van Laarhoven, Johns<strong>on</strong>, Van<br />

Laarhoven-Myers, Grider, & Grider, 2009).<br />

Overall, most studies were c<strong>on</strong>ducted in<br />

kitchen or home living settings because they<br />

involved cooking-related tasks.<br />

Designs<br />

Researchers in the majority of the studies used<br />

a multiple baseline/multiple-probe design<br />

across behaviors/participants (Graves et al.,<br />

2005; Horn et al., 2008; Mechling et al., 2003;<br />

Mechling et al., 2008; Mechling et al., 2009;<br />

Norman et al., 2001; Ti<strong>on</strong>g et al., 1992; Van<br />

Laarhoven et al., 2009). However, in some<br />

studies a multiple-baseline design was combined<br />

with an alternating treatment or ABAB<br />

design: ABAB combined with multiple-baseline<br />

across participants (Goods<strong>on</strong> et al., 2007;<br />

Sigafoos et al., 2007; Sigafoos et al., 2005); a<br />

multiple-probe across participants with an alternating<br />

treatment design (Cannella-Mal<strong>on</strong>e<br />

et al., 2006). An adapted alternating treatments<br />

design was used in five studies (Cihak et<br />

al., 2006; Mechling & Gustafs<strong>on</strong>, 2008, 2009;<br />

Mechling & Stephens, 2009; Van Laarhoven &<br />

Van Laarhoven-Myers, 2006). In <strong>on</strong>e study an<br />

AB design was replicated across participants<br />

(Grice & Blampied, 1994).<br />

Targeted Skills<br />

Researchers in several studies (n 7) targeted<br />

food preparati<strong>on</strong> or cooking-related skills<br />

(Graves et al., 2005; Mechling et al., 2008;<br />

Mechling et al., 2009; Mechling & Gustafs<strong>on</strong>,<br />

2008, 2009; Mechling & Stephens, 2009; Sigafoos<br />

et al., 2005) <strong>and</strong> in <strong>on</strong>e study cooking<br />

tasks (such as microwaving pizza) were combined<br />

with other tasks such as washing a table<br />

<strong>and</strong> folding laundry (Van Laarhoven & Van<br />

Laarhoven-Myers, 2006). Investigators targeted<br />

self-help skills <strong>and</strong>/or other daily living<br />

skills in a number of studies, such as: purchasing<br />

<strong>and</strong> banking skills (Cihak et al., 2006;<br />

Mechling et al., 2003), table setting (Goods<strong>on</strong><br />

et al., 2007), setting the table <strong>and</strong> putting away<br />

groceries (Cannella-Mal<strong>on</strong>e et al., 2006), doing<br />

laundry (Horn et al., 2008), dish washing<br />

(Sigafoos et al., 2007), cleaning sunglasses,<br />

putting <strong>on</strong> a wristwatch, <strong>and</strong> zipping a jacket<br />

(Norman et al., 2001). In a few of the studies<br />

investigators targeted other skills, such as operating<br />

video <strong>and</strong> computer devices (Grice &<br />

Blampied, 1994); job-related tasks including<br />

cleaning a bathroom, mopping floors, taking<br />

out garbage, <strong>and</strong> cleaning kennels at an animal<br />

shelter (Van Laarhoven et al., 2009); <strong>and</strong><br />

safely exiting bedrooms in the event of fire<br />

(Ti<strong>on</strong>g et al., 1992). Overall, a majority of the<br />

tasks involved cooking <strong>and</strong> self-care/independent<br />

living skills.<br />

Video Prompting Material<br />

Types of video prompts. The video prompts<br />

were mainly c<strong>on</strong>sistent across the studies.<br />

Video prompts were prepared either from the<br />

participant’s viewpoint or spectator’s viewpoint.<br />

A majority of the videos were prepared<br />

from the spectators’ viewpoint with verbal or<br />

voice-over instructi<strong>on</strong>s (Goods<strong>on</strong> et al., 2007;<br />

Grice & Blampied, 1994; Mechling et al.,<br />

2009; Mechling & Gustafs<strong>on</strong>, 2008, 2009;<br />

Mechling & Stephens, 2009; Ti<strong>on</strong>g et al.,<br />

1992; Van Laarhoven et al., 2009). In <strong>on</strong>e<br />

study, the investigators menti<strong>on</strong>ed that the<br />

videos were prepared from the spectators’<br />

viewpoint (Van Laarhoven & Van Laarhoven-<br />

Myers, 2006); however, they did not menti<strong>on</strong><br />

whether any verbal instructi<strong>on</strong>s were used in<br />

the video prompt. Researchers in several<br />

other studies used video prompts from the<br />

participants’ viewpoint or with other combinati<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

including: participants’ viewpoint with<br />

voice-over instructi<strong>on</strong>s (Cannella-Mal<strong>on</strong>e et<br />

al., 2006; Graves et al., 2005; Sigafoos et al.,<br />

2007; Sigafoos et al., 2005), participants’ viewpoint<br />

with voice-over instructi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> text display<br />

(Norman et al., 2001), participant’s viewpoint<br />

(Horn et al., 2008), <strong>and</strong> participants’<br />

<strong>and</strong> spectators’ viewpoint with voice-over instructi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

(Mechling et al., 2008).<br />

Video Prompting <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong> / 519


Length of video clips. Researchers in 8 of 18<br />

studies described the length of video clips.<br />

Prompt lengths varied: 4 s (Cihak et al., 2006),<br />

4 to 12 s (Sigafoos et al., 2005), 4 to 30 s<br />

(Sigafoos et al., 2007), 9 to 13 s (Goods<strong>on</strong> et<br />

al., 2007), 10 to 15 s (Grice & Blampied,<br />

1994), 12 to 42 s (Cannella-Mal<strong>on</strong>e et al.,<br />

2006), <strong>and</strong> 12 to 25 s (Mechling & Gustafs<strong>on</strong>,<br />

2008, 2009). The average length could not be<br />

determined because <strong>on</strong>ly ranges were provided<br />

in these studies.<br />

Mode of Presentati<strong>on</strong><br />

A majority of the videos were presented <strong>on</strong><br />

laptop computers (Cannella-Mal<strong>on</strong>e et al.,<br />

2006; Goods<strong>on</strong> et al., 2007; Horn et al., 2008;<br />

Sigafoos et al., 2007; Sigafoos et al., 2005; Van<br />

Laarhoven & Van Laarhoven-Myers, 2006).<br />

However, in <strong>on</strong>e study a computer with a<br />

touch screen program was used (Mechling et<br />

al., 2003). Televisi<strong>on</strong>s were used to present<br />

the tasks in 4 of the 18 studies (Graves et al.,<br />

2005; Grice & Blampied, 1994; Norman et al.,<br />

2001; Ti<strong>on</strong>g et al., 1992) <strong>and</strong> in three other<br />

studies a portable DVD player was used<br />

(Mechling et al., 2008; Mechling & Gustafs<strong>on</strong>,<br />

2008; Mechling & Stephens, 2009). Researchers<br />

in two studies presented the videos <strong>on</strong><br />

small devices such as pers<strong>on</strong>al digital assistant<br />

(PDA; Mechling et al., 2009) or a video iPod<br />

(Van Laarhoven et al., 2009). Finally, the<br />

video prompts were presented <strong>on</strong> the screen<br />

in <strong>on</strong>e study (Cihak et al., 2006).<br />

Effectiveness of the Strategy<br />

Overwhelmingly, the video prompting strategy,<br />

sometimes combined with other prompting<br />

or error correcti<strong>on</strong> strategies, has enhanced<br />

various skills in the majority of<br />

participants (n 67; 99%) with developmental<br />

disabilities. However, <strong>on</strong>e of three participants<br />

in Sigafoos et al. (2005) did not acquire<br />

the targeted skill. Sigafoos <strong>and</strong> colleagues<br />

taught three adults with developmental disabilities<br />

to use a microwave oven to make<br />

popcorn. Results showed that two of three<br />

participants learned to make the popcorn <strong>and</strong><br />

maintained the skill in the follow-up probes.<br />

However, the third participant failed to reach<br />

the criteri<strong>on</strong>. The investigators indicated that<br />

the third participant showed lack of interest in<br />

making the popcorn <strong>and</strong> was <strong>on</strong> anti-depressant<br />

medicati<strong>on</strong>. Also, in another study Graves<br />

et al. (2005) reported that the three participants<br />

in their study learned two of three cooking<br />

tasks (i.e., stovetop, microwave, countertop<br />

cooking skills) with the video prompting<br />

strategy. The authors menti<strong>on</strong>ed that due to<br />

the end of the school year they did not have<br />

sufficient time to collect data for the third<br />

task.<br />

Video Prompting Combined with Other Strategies<br />

Researchers in several studies combined the<br />

video prompting with strategies such as c<strong>on</strong>stant<br />

time delay (CTD), video feedback,<br />

<strong>and</strong>/or error correcti<strong>on</strong> methods. For example,<br />

Goods<strong>on</strong> et al. (2007) used the video<br />

prompting strategy to train three adults with<br />

developmental disabilities to teach a domestic<br />

skill (table setting). The authors combined<br />

video prompting with an error correcti<strong>on</strong><br />

strategy. During the error correcti<strong>on</strong> strategy,<br />

the investigators replayed the step <strong>on</strong> the<br />

video. If the participant still could not perform<br />

the step of the task after viewing the<br />

video clip, the researchers dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

through in vivo (live model) modeling. Results<br />

indicated that the participants were able<br />

to learn the table setting tasks with 90% or<br />

more accuracy when video prompting was<br />

combined with error correcti<strong>on</strong> than with<br />

video prompting al<strong>on</strong>e. Similarly, Van Laarhoven<br />

et al. (2009) played videos <strong>on</strong> an iPod<br />

combined with an error correcti<strong>on</strong> strategy<br />

(video feedback) to teach three job-related<br />

tasks (i.e., cleaning a bathroom, mopping<br />

floors <strong>and</strong> taking out garbage, <strong>and</strong> cleaning<br />

kennels) to a 17-year-old pers<strong>on</strong> with a developmental<br />

disability. The participant was able<br />

to learn all three tasks quickly, required fewer<br />

prompts as the study progressed, <strong>and</strong> was able<br />

to operate the iPod independently.<br />

In quite a few studies least-to-most prompting<br />

methods were implemented. For example,<br />

Ti<strong>on</strong>g et al. (1992) trained four adults with<br />

intellectual disabilities in fire safety using the<br />

video prompting strategy. The authors also<br />

used various levels of prompting (least to most<br />

prompting) to train the participants in different<br />

scenarios in case of fire. Initially, the investigators<br />

read out a descripti<strong>on</strong> of a scenario<br />

<strong>and</strong> asked the participants “What would you<br />

520 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


do?” If the participant did not resp<strong>on</strong>d, the<br />

trainer provided a n<strong>on</strong>-specific verbal prompt.<br />

If the participant still did not resp<strong>on</strong>d to a<br />

verbal prompt, he was shown a video of the<br />

step then was asked to perform the step. Results<br />

indicated that all four participants acquired<br />

the tasks <strong>and</strong> were able to perform the<br />

tasks in transfer settings. Furthermore, the<br />

level <strong>and</strong> number of prompts decreased at the<br />

end of the interventi<strong>on</strong>. Similarly, Mechling et<br />

al. (2009) used the least-to-most prompting<br />

method (i.e., picture-prompt, picture auditory<br />

prompt, <strong>and</strong> video prompt voice over)<br />

to train three adults with autism in cooking<br />

skills using a pers<strong>on</strong>al digital assistant (PDA).<br />

Results showed that the PDAs with video, picture,<br />

<strong>and</strong> auditory prompts were effective as<br />

they served as a self-prompting method to<br />

improve participants’ cooking skills. Authors<br />

indicated that the self-prompting device may<br />

serve a dual purpose; an antecedent prompting<br />

as well as permanent prompting system.<br />

Also, Horn et al. (2008) taught three adults<br />

with developmental disabilities laundry skills.<br />

Two of three participants acquired the skills<br />

with the video prompting procedure al<strong>on</strong>e.<br />

However, <strong>on</strong>e of the participants acquired the<br />

skill when the investigators added least-tomost<br />

prompting al<strong>on</strong>g with video prompting.<br />

Results were idiosyncratic across participants.<br />

Likewise, Cihak et al. (2006) compared video<br />

prompting with static picture prompting with<br />

two groups of three students with intellectual<br />

disabilities to teach purchasing <strong>and</strong> banking<br />

skills. The investigators provided instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

in groups using the least-to-most prompt procedure.<br />

Results indicated that both the video<br />

prompting <strong>and</strong> the static picture prompting<br />

were equally effective across participants.<br />

Norman et al. (2001) used video modeling<br />

<strong>and</strong> video prompting combined with CTD to<br />

teach self-help skills to three children with<br />

intellectual disabilities. Initially, the entire<br />

task was presented to the participants through<br />

video modeling <strong>and</strong> later the participants<br />

were shown each step through video prompting<br />

combined with time delay. Results indicated<br />

that two participants learned all three<br />

tasks (cleaning sunglasses, putting <strong>on</strong> a wristwatch,<br />

<strong>and</strong> zipping a jacket) <strong>and</strong> the third<br />

participant learned two of three tasks (cleaning<br />

sunglasses <strong>and</strong> zipping a jacket). Similarly,<br />

Graves et al. (2005) initially showed the entire<br />

task of the video model <strong>and</strong> later showed<br />

video clips of each task combined with the<br />

CTD procedure with three students with developmental<br />

disabilities. Results indicated that<br />

all three participants successfully learned two<br />

of three cooking tasks. Likewise, Mechling et<br />

al. (2003) used video-based instructi<strong>on</strong> (video<br />

modeling, video prompting, <strong>and</strong> still photographs)<br />

to teach debit card use with three<br />

adults with moderate intellectual disabilities.<br />

The authors combined video prompting with<br />

CTD. All three participants acquired the skills<br />

<strong>and</strong> learned to use the debit card in the community<br />

<strong>and</strong> novel stores.<br />

Video Prompting <strong>and</strong> Fading<br />

Prompt dependency is <strong>on</strong>e of the main c<strong>on</strong>cerns<br />

when teaching pers<strong>on</strong>s with developmental<br />

disabilities. Thus, decreasing prompts<br />

is an important part of instructi<strong>on</strong> to enhance<br />

independence. Results show that researchers<br />

in several studies have attempted to reduce<br />

prompts or directly withdrew the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

to see the effects of the video prompting strategy.<br />

In a study, Sigafoos et al. (2007) explicitly<br />

faded video prompting when teaching three<br />

adults with developmental disabilities dish<br />

washing skills. Initially, the authors showed<br />

the video clips individually <strong>and</strong> asked the participants<br />

to perform the task. Subsequently,<br />

more steps were added to each video clip until<br />

all of the steps of the task had been combined<br />

into a single video clip. At the end of the<br />

study, video prompting was removed. Results<br />

indicated that the participants were able to<br />

learn the task <strong>and</strong> maintain at 1, 2, <strong>and</strong> 3<br />

m<strong>on</strong>ths. In another study, Sigafoos et al.<br />

(2005) taught three adults with developmental<br />

disabilities to make popcorn in the microwave<br />

with the video prompting strategy. Participants<br />

were able to retain the skills<br />

following the withdrawal of the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

which showed that fading was not required. In<br />

another study that involved domestic skills<br />

(washing a table, microwaving pizza, <strong>and</strong> folding<br />

laundry), Van Laarhoven <strong>and</strong> Van Laarhoven-Myers<br />

(2006) showed that the students<br />

were able to fade the video prompts <strong>on</strong> their<br />

own <strong>and</strong> move to picture prompts. Finally,<br />

Mechling et al. (2008) investigated the effects<br />

of the video prompting strategy in which the<br />

participants operated a portable DVD player<br />

Video Prompting <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong> / 521


to learn three cooking tasks. Results indicated<br />

that all participants learned to operate the<br />

DVD player <strong>and</strong> were able to correctly perform<br />

the steps of the cooking tasks.<br />

Video Prompting versus Other Interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

Researchers in 6 of 18 studies compared video<br />

prompting with other picture/video-based interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

(Cannella-Mal<strong>on</strong>e et al., 2006;<br />

Cihak et al., 2006; Mechling & Gustafs<strong>on</strong>,<br />

2008, 2009; Mechling & Stephens, 2009; Van<br />

Laarhoven & Van Laarhoven-Myers, 2006).<br />

For example, Van Laarhoven <strong>and</strong> Van Larhoven-Myers<br />

compared three video-based<br />

strategies (video rehearsal, video rehearsal<br />

plus photos, <strong>and</strong> video rehearsal <strong>and</strong> in-vivo<br />

video prompting) to teach daily living skills<br />

with three young adults with developmental<br />

disabilities. Results showed that the video rehearsal<br />

combined with video prompting was<br />

more effective with two participants when<br />

compared to the two other c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s (video<br />

modeling <strong>and</strong> video modeling plus photos).<br />

For <strong>on</strong>e participant, the video modeling plus<br />

photo c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> was effective to increase task<br />

acquisiti<strong>on</strong>. In another study, Canella-Mal<strong>on</strong>e<br />

<strong>and</strong> colleagues compared video modeling <strong>and</strong><br />

video prompting strategies with six adults with<br />

developmental disabilities. The investigators<br />

trained the adults to set the table <strong>and</strong> put away<br />

groceries. Results indicated that video<br />

prompting was more effective than video<br />

modeling in teaching both tasks; table setting<br />

<strong>and</strong> putting away groceries.<br />

Researchers in four studies compared video<br />

prompting versus static pictures (Cihak et al.,<br />

2006; Mechling & Gustafs<strong>on</strong>, 2008, 2009;<br />

Mechling & Stephens, 2009). Mechling <strong>and</strong><br />

Gustafs<strong>on</strong> trained six adults with autism in<br />

cooking-related tasks. The investigators compared<br />

the video prompting <strong>and</strong> static pictures<br />

using an adapted alternating treatment design.<br />

Results indicated that all participants acquired<br />

the skills faster in the video prompting<br />

method compared to the static pictures during<br />

the interventi<strong>on</strong> phase of the study. Also,<br />

in another study Mechling <strong>and</strong> Gustafs<strong>on</strong><br />

compared the video prompting <strong>and</strong> static picture<br />

prompting while training six adults in<br />

cooking-related tasks. Results showed that the<br />

video prompting strategy was more effective<br />

than the static picture prompting across all of<br />

the participants. Likewise, Mechling <strong>and</strong> Stephens<br />

compared the video instructi<strong>on</strong> as a<br />

self-prompting strategy <strong>and</strong> static picturebased<br />

cookbooks to train four students with<br />

moderate intellectual disabilities in cookingrelated<br />

tasks. Results indicated that the video<br />

self-prompting strategy was more effective<br />

than the picture-based cookbook across all<br />

four participants. However, in <strong>on</strong>e study the<br />

video prompting <strong>and</strong> static pictures were<br />

equally effective with the participants. Cihak<br />

et al. investigated the effects of the video<br />

prompting versus static picture prompting<br />

with two groups of three students with intellectual<br />

disabilities to teach purchasing <strong>and</strong><br />

banking skills (withdrawing m<strong>on</strong>ey from ATM<br />

<strong>and</strong> purchasing). The authors used group instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

procedures (model <strong>and</strong> test) <strong>and</strong><br />

also provided community based instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

using a least-to-most prompt procedure. Results<br />

indicated that both the video prompting<br />

<strong>and</strong> the static picture prompting were equally<br />

effective. Overall, results of 5 of 6 studies<br />

showed that video prompting methods were<br />

more effective than the static picture interventi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Maintenance <strong>and</strong> Generalizati<strong>on</strong> Data<br />

Investigators in 7 of 18 studies reported maintenance<br />

data but did not report the generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

data (Cihak et al., 2006; Graves et al.,<br />

2005; Horn et al., 2008; Mechling et al., 2009;<br />

Sigafoos et al., 2007; Sigafoos et al., 2005; Van<br />

Laarhoven et al., 2009). Participants in Cihak<br />

et al. study maintained the purchasing skills<br />

learned through video prompting <strong>and</strong> static<br />

picture prompting strategies. Similarly, Sigfoos<br />

et al. collected maintenance data in their<br />

study which was c<strong>on</strong>ducted with three adults<br />

with developmental disabilities to teach them<br />

to microwave popcorn. Two of three participants<br />

successfully learned the skill <strong>and</strong> maintained<br />

it at 2, 6, <strong>and</strong> 10 weeks after the interventi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

In another study, Sigafoos et al.<br />

trained three adults with developmental disabilities<br />

to do dish washing <strong>and</strong> maintenance<br />

data showed that they were able to perform<br />

the skill at 1, 2, <strong>and</strong> 3 m<strong>on</strong>th probes. Also,<br />

Mechling et al. assessed for maintenance of<br />

cooking skills using video-based instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

delivered <strong>on</strong> PDA with three individuals with<br />

autism. All three students maintained the skill<br />

522 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


following the interventi<strong>on</strong>. Likewise, Graves et<br />

al. collected maintenance data with the participants<br />

after they reached criteri<strong>on</strong> during the<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>. Results indicated that all participants<br />

learned the cooking tasks <strong>and</strong> maintained<br />

at least <strong>on</strong>e or two tasks two weeks<br />

following the interventi<strong>on</strong>. Results from Horn<br />

et al. also indicated that two of three participants<br />

in the study maintained the learned<br />

skills (laundry) two weeks following the interventi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Van Laarhoven et al. trained an adult<br />

with developmental disability using video iPod<br />

as a prompting design to complete three job<br />

related tasks (cleaning bathroom, mopping<br />

floors <strong>and</strong> taking out garbage, cleaning kennels)<br />

at an animal shelter. The participant<br />

quickly acquired the skills <strong>and</strong> required few<br />

prompts. Maintenance data indicated that the<br />

participant was able to perform the task without<br />

video iPod prompt at 10 weeks following<br />

the interventi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Investigators in <strong>on</strong>e study reported generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

data but no maintenance data (Grice<br />

& Blampied, 1994). Le Grice <strong>and</strong> Blampied<br />

trained four individuals with intellectual disabilities<br />

to operate video equipment <strong>and</strong> the<br />

computer using the video prompting strategy.<br />

All four participants learned the skill <strong>and</strong> generalized<br />

to novel video players <strong>and</strong> computer<br />

equipment.<br />

Investigators in 4 of 18 studies reported<br />

both maintenance <strong>and</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong> data<br />

(Mechling et al., 2003; Norman et al., 2001;<br />

Ti<strong>on</strong>g et al., 1992; Van Laarhoven & Van Laarhoven-Myers,<br />

2006). Van Laarhoven <strong>and</strong> Van<br />

Larhoven-Myers collected post interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong> data. The three participants<br />

were able to perform the steps (microwave<br />

pizza) in novel instructi<strong>on</strong>al settings <strong>and</strong><br />

post interventi<strong>on</strong> data indicated that participants<br />

were able to maintain the tasks. Also,<br />

Mechling et al. trained three adults with intellectual<br />

disabilities to make purchases using a<br />

debit card. Maintenance data were collected<br />

for two of three participants 1 week <strong>and</strong> 6<br />

weeks following instructi<strong>on</strong>. Maintenance<br />

data showed both participants were able to<br />

make purchases. In additi<strong>on</strong>, all three participants<br />

generalized the debit card use in community<br />

<strong>and</strong> novel stores. Similarly, Norman et<br />

al. taught self-help skills to three children with<br />

developmental disabilities (cleaning sunglasses,<br />

putting <strong>on</strong> a wristwatch, <strong>and</strong> zipping a<br />

jacket) using the video modeling <strong>and</strong> video<br />

prompting strategies combined with CTD. Results<br />

showed that all three participants<br />

learned the skill <strong>and</strong> maintained the skill;<br />

however, maintenance probes ranged between<br />

1 to 13 weeks across three participants.<br />

For the task of cleaning glasses, the investigators<br />

assessed for generalizati<strong>on</strong> across pers<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with two participants <strong>and</strong> also assessed for<br />

generalizati<strong>on</strong> of putting <strong>on</strong> wristwatch with<br />

<strong>on</strong>e of the participants across material. No<br />

generalizati<strong>on</strong> probes were c<strong>on</strong>ducted with<br />

the third participant. Finally, Ti<strong>on</strong>g et al. assessed<br />

both generalizati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> maintenance<br />

of fire safety skills with four adults with intellectual<br />

disabilities using the video prompting<br />

strategy. Results indicated that the participants<br />

were able to transfer the training to a<br />

novel setting <strong>and</strong> maintain the skills 4–5<br />

weeks post interventi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Social Validity<br />

Social validity was assessed in 4 of 18 studies<br />

(Mechling et al., 2009; Ti<strong>on</strong>g et al., 1992; Van<br />

Laarhoven et al., 2009; Van Laarhoven & Van<br />

Laarhoven-Myers, 2006). In a study, Ti<strong>on</strong>g et<br />

al. trained participants with intellectual disabilities<br />

to escape from the bedroom in the<br />

event of fire. The authors used a questi<strong>on</strong>naire<br />

to assess the satisfacti<strong>on</strong> of the staff <strong>and</strong><br />

the participants. Results indicated that the<br />

staff expressed medium level of satisfacti<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> the participants expressed high level of<br />

satisfacti<strong>on</strong>. However, the staff also expressed<br />

c<strong>on</strong>cerns about the video quality <strong>and</strong> indicated<br />

that the training program was boring<br />

<strong>and</strong> two of the participants indicated that they<br />

were more c<strong>on</strong>cerned about the fire safety.<br />

Similarly, Van Laarhoven et al. c<strong>on</strong>ducted social<br />

validity assessments through informal interviews<br />

with the study participant, his<br />

mother, <strong>and</strong> the employers about the video<br />

iPod <strong>and</strong> video feed back interventi<strong>on</strong>. The<br />

participant indicated that he liked the video<br />

iPod <strong>and</strong> he felt that the device helped him to<br />

do tasks independently. In additi<strong>on</strong>, the<br />

mother <strong>and</strong> the employers expressed satisfacti<strong>on</strong><br />

with the results <strong>and</strong> were impressed with<br />

the effectiveness of the video iPod. The employers<br />

indicated that they thought the video<br />

iPod would benefit other employers when<br />

training new employees in their setting. Also,<br />

Video Prompting <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong> / 523


Van Laarhoven <strong>and</strong> Van Laarhoven-Myers assessed<br />

the social validity of interventi<strong>on</strong> with<br />

parents who indicated that they were satisfied<br />

with the way the participants acquired the<br />

steps with the strategy. In additi<strong>on</strong>, two participants<br />

stated that they enjoyed watching the<br />

videos <strong>on</strong> the computer <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>e participant<br />

stated that she would have preferred watching<br />

<strong>on</strong> the televisi<strong>on</strong>. Finally, Mechling <strong>and</strong> colleagues<br />

assessed social validity by asking the<br />

participants about their preference by presenting<br />

a portable DVD player, PDA, <strong>and</strong> picture<br />

cookbook. The study was c<strong>on</strong>ducted using<br />

PDAs to deliver video-based instructi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Two participants preferred the portable DVD<br />

player <strong>and</strong> the third participant preferred the<br />

PDA.<br />

Discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

Results of this review show that video prompting<br />

is a viable interventi<strong>on</strong> for teaching individuals<br />

with a range of developmental disabilities<br />

for various domestic, life, vocati<strong>on</strong>al, <strong>and</strong><br />

independent living skills. Results of several<br />

studies also show that the video prompting<br />

strategy is more effective when compared to<br />

static picture use. Furthermore, participants<br />

in several studies maintained the skills learned<br />

through video prompting <strong>and</strong> generalized the<br />

learned skills to novel settings or behaviors.<br />

The video prompting strategy is a robust<br />

method for enhancing several skills in pers<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with developmental disabilities. There may be<br />

several possible reas<strong>on</strong>s for its success. The<br />

method is based <strong>on</strong> task analysis, prompting,<br />

repetiti<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> feedback which are fundamental<br />

principles of teaching <strong>and</strong> are likely to<br />

increase skill acquisiti<strong>on</strong> in pers<strong>on</strong>s with developmental<br />

disabilities (Giangreco, 2011;<br />

Snell, 2007). In additi<strong>on</strong>, video prompting involves<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sistency through video clips which<br />

can be repeatedly presented to participants at<br />

an individual pace until they are proficient.<br />

Furthermore, video clip informati<strong>on</strong> can be<br />

presented to learners in multiple modes including<br />

auditory, visual, <strong>and</strong> animated cues<br />

(Mechling et al., 2008). Research also supports<br />

that repeated practice is likely to enhance<br />

skill acquisiti<strong>on</strong> (Ayres & Lang<strong>on</strong>e,<br />

2005; Giangreco, 2011) <strong>and</strong> because many<br />

studies involved participants with autism, it is<br />

likely that pers<strong>on</strong>s with autism, are especially<br />

likely to resp<strong>on</strong>d to visual mode of presentati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

(Quill, 1997).<br />

This review indicates that video prompting<br />

was predominantly used with cooking-related,<br />

vocati<strong>on</strong>al, <strong>and</strong>/or domestic skills with young<br />

adults or adults with developmental disabilities.<br />

Although few studies have attempted other<br />

skills, such as purchasing <strong>and</strong> safety, n<strong>on</strong>e of the<br />

studies attempted to teach social skills or selfhelp<br />

skills such as dressing, grooming, brushing,<br />

shaving, etc. It is possible that the complex nature<br />

of social skills requires a combinati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

variables (e.g., initiate, resp<strong>on</strong>d, terminate c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

share) <strong>and</strong> that students may be better<br />

able to resp<strong>on</strong>d to other interventi<strong>on</strong>s such<br />

as direct teaching, peer modeling, <strong>and</strong> video<br />

modeling. However, the questi<strong>on</strong> for future researchers<br />

that remains is whether video prompts<br />

can be used to teach social or self-help skills in<br />

pers<strong>on</strong>s with developmental disabilities. In additi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

more studies are needed with preschoolage<br />

<strong>and</strong>/or young children with developmental<br />

disabilities.<br />

Researchers in several studies combined<br />

video prompting with CTD <strong>and</strong> used least-tomost<br />

prompting <strong>and</strong>/or video feedback as error<br />

correcti<strong>on</strong> methods to teach tasks. Research<br />

supports the use of such strategies to<br />

teach chained tasks in pers<strong>on</strong>s with developmental<br />

disabilities (see Dogoe & B<strong>and</strong>a, 2009;<br />

Wolery et al., 1992). Several studies also show<br />

that video prompting al<strong>on</strong>g with least-to-most<br />

prompting was effective in teaching various<br />

skills. Future researchers should investigate if<br />

other prompting methods, such as graduated<br />

guidance with video prompting, improve skills<br />

in pers<strong>on</strong>s with developmental disabilities (see<br />

Sigafoos et al., 2005). In additi<strong>on</strong>, it remains<br />

to be verified whether video feedback is an<br />

effective way to correct participants’ errors<br />

during video instructi<strong>on</strong> (Van Laarhoven et<br />

al., 2009).<br />

Investigators in several studies used multiple<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>s such as video prompting<br />

combined with video modeling, video<br />

prompts with voice-over instructi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong>/or<br />

text display, static pictures, etc. Although<br />

package interventi<strong>on</strong>s seem to be effective<br />

with the participants, the individual effectiveness<br />

of each variable is difficult to assess in<br />

many of the studies. Future researchers<br />

should investigate the individual effects of<br />

each interventi<strong>on</strong> such as video prompts with<br />

524 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


<strong>and</strong> without voice over instructi<strong>on</strong>s or video<br />

prompting with or without text display, etc.<br />

(see Mechling et al., 2003).<br />

Results show that few studies have attempted<br />

to fade the video prompts. An exemplary<br />

model developed by Sigafoos et al.<br />

(2007) can be replicated. In additi<strong>on</strong>, it<br />

should be worth examining whether video<br />

prompts can be faded to picture prompts <strong>and</strong>,<br />

later, to verbal prompts which are natural.<br />

Also, researchers in few studies successfully<br />

trained the participants in using h<strong>and</strong>-held<br />

devices such as PDA or video iPOD to deliver<br />

video prompts (Mechling et al., 2009; Van<br />

Laarhoven et al., 2009). Training pers<strong>on</strong>s with<br />

developmental disabilities to operate video devices<br />

<strong>on</strong> their own is likely to decrease the<br />

need for prompts <strong>and</strong> likely to improve independent<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>ing.<br />

This study indicates that <strong>on</strong>ly 5 of 18 studies<br />

have assessed for generalizati<strong>on</strong>. The importance<br />

of generalizati<strong>on</strong> is very essential when<br />

skills are taught to pers<strong>on</strong>s with developmental<br />

disabilities because they have difficulties<br />

generalizing tasks across behaviors, settings,<br />

<strong>and</strong>/or pers<strong>on</strong>s. Thus, it is important for future<br />

researchers to incorporate generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

training using various strategies (e.g., multiple<br />

exemplars, using natural c<strong>on</strong>texts, varying<br />

stimuli) <strong>and</strong> to assess for generalizati<strong>on</strong> when<br />

video prompting studies are c<strong>on</strong>ducted.<br />

Although video prompting is an appealing<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>, in <strong>on</strong>ly 4 of 18 studies was social<br />

validity assessed. Thus, it is difficult to c<strong>on</strong>clude<br />

whether video prompting interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

are easy to develop, easy to implement, <strong>and</strong><br />

feasible in applied settings. Similar c<strong>on</strong>cerns<br />

appear in other studies (see Grice & Blampied,<br />

1994; Van Laarhoven et al., 2009). It is<br />

essential for practiti<strong>on</strong>ers to select a functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

skill, task analyze the skill, video tape<br />

the sequence, edit, c<strong>on</strong>sider whether to use<br />

prompts, operate a video device (e.g., DVD,<br />

iPod, PDA), collect data, m<strong>on</strong>itor, <strong>and</strong> assess<br />

for maintenance <strong>and</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong>. Also,<br />

some tasks may involve too many steps <strong>and</strong> it<br />

may be cumbersome to present each step to<br />

the participant as a video prompt (e.g., having<br />

30 or more steps in a complex cooking skill).<br />

Future investigators should include such social<br />

validity comp<strong>on</strong>ents of video prompting<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>s or the strategy may not sustain<br />

in practiti<strong>on</strong>er community.<br />

C<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

This review supports the findings that pers<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with developmental disabilities can learn various<br />

domestic <strong>and</strong> independent living skills<br />

through the video prompting strategy. Video<br />

prompts appear to be more effective when<br />

compared to picture prompts. The strategy is<br />

also effective in the maintenance <strong>and</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

of learned skills. Because of the robust<br />

research findings, it is important for future<br />

researchers to c<strong>on</strong>tinually assess social<br />

validity <strong>and</strong> make it more accessible for teachers<br />

<strong>and</strong> practiti<strong>on</strong>ers working with pers<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with developmental disabilities.<br />

References<br />

Ayres, K. M., & Lang<strong>on</strong>e, J. (2005). Interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> with video for students with autism: A<br />

review of the literature. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in<br />

<strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 40, 183–196.<br />

Baker, S. D., Lang, R., & O’Reilly, M. (2009). Review<br />

of video modeling with students with emoti<strong>on</strong>al<br />

<strong>and</strong> behavioral disorders. Educati<strong>on</strong> & Treatment<br />

of Children, 32, 403–420.<br />

Bellini, S., & Akullian, J. (2007). A meta-analysis of<br />

video modeling <strong>and</strong> video self-modeling interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for children <strong>and</strong> adolescents with autism<br />

spectrum disorders. Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, 73, 264–<br />

287.<br />

Bryan, L. C., & Gast, D. L. (2000). Teaching <strong>on</strong>-task<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>-schedule behaviors to high-functi<strong>on</strong>ing<br />

children with autism via picture activity schedules.<br />

Journal of <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> Disorders, 30,<br />

553–567.<br />

Cannella-Mal<strong>on</strong>e, H., Sigafoos, J., O’Reilly, M., de la<br />

Cruz, B., Edrisinha, C., & Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, G. E. (2006).<br />

Comparing video prompting to video modeling<br />

for teaching daily living skills to six adults with<br />

developmental disabilities. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training<br />

in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 41, 344–356.<br />

Cihak, D., Alberto, P. A., Taber-Doughty, T., &<br />

Gama, R. I. (2006). A comparis<strong>on</strong> of static picture<br />

prompting <strong>and</strong> video prompting simulati<strong>on</strong> strategies<br />

using group instructi<strong>on</strong>al procedures. Focus<br />

<strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> & Other <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 21,<br />

89–99.<br />

Collins, B. C. (2007). Moderate <strong>and</strong> severe disabilities: A<br />

foundati<strong>on</strong>al approach. Upper Saddle River, NJ:<br />

Pears<strong>on</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong> Inc.<br />

Cooper, J. O., Her<strong>on</strong>, T. E., & Heward, W. L.<br />

(2007). Applied behavior analysis (2nd ed.). Upper<br />

Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.<br />

Delano, M. E. (2007). Video modeling interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for individuals with autism. Remedial & Special<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>, 28, 33–42.<br />

Video Prompting <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong> / 525


Dogoe, M., & B<strong>and</strong>a, D. R. (2009). Review of recent<br />

research using c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay to teach<br />

chained tasks to pers<strong>on</strong>s with developmental disabilities.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 44, 177–186.<br />

Giangreco, M. F. (2011). Educating students with<br />

severe disabilities: Foundati<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>cepts <strong>and</strong><br />

practices. In M. E. Snell & F. Brown (Eds.), Instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

of students with severe disabilities (7th ed.,<br />

pp. 1–30). Bost<strong>on</strong>: Pears<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Goods<strong>on</strong>, J., Sigafoos, J., O’ Reilly, M., Cannella, H.,<br />

& Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, G. E. (2007). Evaluati<strong>on</strong> of a videobased<br />

error correcti<strong>on</strong> procedure for teaching a<br />

domestic skill to individuals with developmental<br />

disabilities. Research in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>,<br />

28, 458–<str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>7.<br />

Graves, T. B., Collins, B. C., Schuster, J. W., & Kleinert,<br />

H. (2005). Using video prompting to teach<br />

cooking skills to sec<strong>on</strong>dary students with moderate<br />

disabilities. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 40, 34–<str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>.<br />

Horn, J. A., Miltenberger, R. G., Weil, T., Mowery, J.,<br />

C<strong>on</strong>n, M., & Sams, L. (2008). Teaching laundry<br />

skills to individuals with developmental disabilities<br />

using video prompting. Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal of<br />

Behavioral C<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> & Therapy, 4, 279–286.<br />

Le Grice, B., & Blampied, N. M. (1994). Training<br />

pupils with intellectual disability to operate educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

technology using video prompting. Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

& Training in Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong> & <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 29, 321–330.<br />

McCoy, K., & Hermarisen, E. (2007). Video modeling<br />

for individuals with autism: A review of model<br />

types <strong>and</strong> effects. Educati<strong>on</strong> & Treatment of Children,<br />

30, 183–213.<br />

Mechling, L. C., Gast, D. L., & Barthold, S. (2003).<br />

Multimedia computer-based instructi<strong>on</strong> to teach<br />

students with moderate intellectual disabilities to<br />

use a debit card to make purchases. Excepti<strong>on</strong>ality,<br />

11, 239–254.<br />

Mechling, L. C., Gast, D. L., & Fields, E. A. (2008).<br />

Evaluati<strong>on</strong> of a portable DVD player <strong>and</strong> system<br />

of least prompts to self-prompt cooking task completi<strong>on</strong><br />

by young adults with moderate intellectual<br />

disabilities. Journal of Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 42,<br />

179–190.<br />

Mechling, L. C., Gast, D. L., & Seid, N. H. (2009).<br />

Using a pers<strong>on</strong>al digital assistant to increase independent<br />

task completi<strong>on</strong> by students with autism<br />

spectrum disoders. Journal of <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

Disorders, 39, 1420–1434.<br />

Mechling, L. C., & Gustafs<strong>on</strong>, M. (2008). Comparis<strong>on</strong><br />

of static picture <strong>and</strong> video prompting <strong>on</strong> the<br />

performance of cooking-related tasks by students<br />

with autism. Journal of Special Educati<strong>on</strong> Technology,<br />

23, 31–45.<br />

Mechling, L. C., & Gustafs<strong>on</strong>, M. (2009). Comparis<strong>on</strong><br />

of the effects of static picture <strong>and</strong> video<br />

prompting <strong>on</strong> completi<strong>on</strong> of cooking related<br />

tasks by students with moderate intellectual disabilities.<br />

Excepti<strong>on</strong>ality, 17, 103–116.<br />

Mechling, L. C., & Stephens, E. (2009). Comparis<strong>on</strong><br />

of self-prompting of cooking skills via picturebased<br />

cookbooks <strong>and</strong> video recipes. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Training in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 44, 218–236.<br />

Norman, J. M., Collins, B. C., & Schuster, J. W.<br />

(2001). Using an instructi<strong>on</strong>al package including<br />

video technology to teach self-help skills to elementary<br />

students with mental disabilities. Journal<br />

of Special Educati<strong>on</strong> Technology, 16(3), 5–18.<br />

Quill, K. A. (1997). Instructi<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong>s for<br />

young children with autism: The rati<strong>on</strong>ale for<br />

visually cued instructi<strong>on</strong>. Journal of <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>Developmental</strong> Disorders, 27, 697–714.<br />

Rayner, C., Denholm, C., & Sigafoos, J. (2009). Video-based<br />

Interventi<strong>on</strong> for individuals with autism:<br />

Key questi<strong>on</strong>s that remain unanswered. Research<br />

in <strong>Autism</strong> Spectrum Disorders, 3, 291–303.<br />

Shukla-Mehta, S., Miller, T., & Callahan, K. J.<br />

(2010). Evaluating the effectiveness of video instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>on</strong> social <strong>and</strong> communicati<strong>on</strong> skills<br />

training for children with autism spectrum disorders:<br />

A review of the literature. Focus <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> &<br />

Other <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 25, 23–36.<br />

Sigafoos, J., O’Reilly, M., Cannella, H., Edrisinha,<br />

C., de la Cruz, B., Upadhyaya, M., et al. (2007).<br />

Evaluati<strong>on</strong> of a video prompting <strong>and</strong> fading procedure<br />

for teaching dish washing skills to adults<br />

with developmental disabilities. Journal of Behavioral<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>, 16, 93–109.<br />

Sigafoos, J., O’Reilly, M., Cannella, H., Upadhyaya,<br />

M., Edrisinha, C., Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, G. E., et al. (2005).<br />

Computer-presented video prompting for teaching<br />

microwave oven use to three adults with developmental<br />

disabilities. Journal of Behavioral Educati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

14, 189–201.<br />

Snell, M. E. (2007). Advances in instructi<strong>on</strong>. In S. L.<br />

Odom, R. H. Horner, M. E. Snell & J. Blacher<br />

(Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book of developmental disabilities (pp.<br />

249–268). NY: The Guilford Press.<br />

Ti<strong>on</strong>g, S. J., Blampied, N. M., & le Grice, B. (1992).<br />

Training community-living, intellectually h<strong>and</strong>icapped<br />

people in fire safety using video prompting.<br />

Behaviour Change, 9(2), 65–72.<br />

Van Laarhoven, T., Johns<strong>on</strong>, J. W., Van Laarhoven-<br />

Myers, T., Grider, K. L., & Grider, K. M. (2009).<br />

The effectiveness of using a video iPod as a<br />

prompting device in employment settings. Journal<br />

of Behavioral Educati<strong>on</strong>, 18, 119–141.<br />

Van Laarhoven, T., & Van Laarhoven-Myers, T.<br />

(2006). Comparis<strong>on</strong> of three video-based instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

procedures for teaching daily living skills to<br />

pers<strong>on</strong>s with developmental disabilities. Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 41, 365–<br />

381.<br />

Wolery, M., Ault, M. J., & Doyle, P. M. (1992).<br />

526 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


Teaching students with moderate to severe disabilities:<br />

Use of resp<strong>on</strong>se prompting strategies. White Plains, NY:<br />

L<strong>on</strong>gman Publishing Group.<br />

Wolery, M., & Gast, D. L. (1984). Effective <strong>and</strong> efficient<br />

procedures for the transfer of stimulus c<strong>on</strong>trol. Topics<br />

in Early Childhood Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 4, 52–77.<br />

Wolery, M., Gast, D. L., Kirk, K., & Schuster, J.<br />

(1988). Fading extra stimulus prompts with autistic<br />

children using time delay. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Treatment<br />

of Children, 11, 29–44.<br />

Received: 7 September 2010<br />

Initial Acceptance: 5 November 2010<br />

Final Acceptance: 15 December 2010<br />

Video Prompting <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong> / 527


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 2011, <str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>(4), 528-543<br />

© <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Simultaneous Prompting: An Instructi<strong>on</strong>al Strategy for Skill<br />

Acquisiti<strong>on</strong><br />

Rebecca E. Waugh, Paul A. Alberto, <strong>and</strong> Laura D. Fredrick<br />

Georgia State University<br />

Abstract: Errorless learning is an instructi<strong>on</strong>al approach designed to eliminate <strong>and</strong>/or reduce the number of<br />

errors students produce in traditi<strong>on</strong>al trial-<strong>and</strong>-error approaches (Mueller, Palkovic, & Maynard, 2007).<br />

Various resp<strong>on</strong>se prompting strategies are employed to produce errorless learning. Simultaneous prompting is an<br />

errorless learning strategy that has a growing body of literature to support its use spanning two decades. This<br />

paper provides a comprehensive review of the literature including (a) skills targeted for instructi<strong>on</strong>, (b)<br />

populati<strong>on</strong>s targeted for instructi<strong>on</strong>, (c) strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses of simultaneous prompting, <strong>and</strong> (d) future<br />

areas of research.<br />

Errorless learning is an instructi<strong>on</strong>al approach<br />

designed to reduce the number of<br />

errors students make in traditi<strong>on</strong>al trial-<strong>and</strong>error<br />

approaches (Mueller, Palkovic, & Maynard,<br />

2007). During errorless learning procedures<br />

stimulus c<strong>on</strong>trol is transferred from the<br />

c<strong>on</strong>trolling prompt to the discriminative stimulus.<br />

This transfer is achieved through the use<br />

of resp<strong>on</strong>se prompting strategies. Resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

prompting strategies c<strong>on</strong>sist of additi<strong>on</strong>al informati<strong>on</strong><br />

which results in the correct resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

being emitted (Wolery, Ault, & Doyle,<br />

1992). The underlying purpose of errorless<br />

learning is the transfer of stimulus c<strong>on</strong>trol<br />

from a resp<strong>on</strong>se prompt to the natural stimulus.<br />

Wolery <strong>and</strong> Gast (1984) identified four<br />

comm<strong>on</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se prompting strategies that<br />

comm<strong>on</strong>ly are employed to transfer stimulus<br />

c<strong>on</strong>trol: (a) most-to-least prompts, (b) least-tomost<br />

prompts, (c) graduated guidance, <strong>and</strong><br />

(d) time delay. Most-to-least prompts c<strong>on</strong>sists<br />

of employing the most intrusive prompt<br />

needed to assist the student in emitting the<br />

correct resp<strong>on</strong>se <strong>and</strong> fading the intensity of<br />

the prompt until the student is correctly resp<strong>on</strong>ding<br />

to the discriminative stimulus independently.<br />

Least-to-most prompts provide the<br />

Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence c<strong>on</strong>cerning this article should<br />

be addressed to Rebecca E. Waugh, Georgia State<br />

University, Department of Educati<strong>on</strong>al Psychology<br />

<strong>and</strong> Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, PO Box 3979, Atlanta, GA<br />

30302-3979. Email: rwaugh1@gsu.edu<br />

student with an opportunity to resp<strong>on</strong>d independently<br />

to the discriminative stimulus. If<br />

the student resp<strong>on</strong>ds incorrectly a prompt is<br />

provided which gradually increases in intensity<br />

until the student resp<strong>on</strong>ds correctly to the<br />

discriminative stimulus. “Graduated guidance<br />

is a technique combining physical guidance<br />

<strong>and</strong> fading in which the physical guidance is<br />

systematically <strong>and</strong> gradually reduced <strong>and</strong> then<br />

faded completely” (Foxx, 1982, p. 129). Graduate<br />

guidance relies heavily <strong>on</strong> the teacher’s<br />

judgment whether or not a prompt is required<br />

or the degree of prompt required. There are<br />

two forms of graduated guidance. During <strong>on</strong>e<br />

form a teacher shadows a student’s movement<br />

when teaching a task to provide guidance during<br />

each step or to remove the physical<br />

prompt. During a sec<strong>on</strong>d form of graduated<br />

guidance the teacher may provide c<strong>on</strong>stant<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tact but gradually <strong>and</strong> systematically reduce<br />

the intrusiveness <strong>and</strong> placement of the<br />

prompt (Foxx, 1981; Wolery & Gast, 1984).<br />

Time delay is the fourth comm<strong>on</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

prompting strategy which results in near errorless<br />

learning by transferring stimulus c<strong>on</strong>trol<br />

from a c<strong>on</strong>trolling prompt to the discriminative<br />

stimulus by inserting a delay between<br />

the presentati<strong>on</strong> of the discriminative stimulus<br />

<strong>and</strong> the c<strong>on</strong>trolling prompt (Snell & Gast,<br />

1981; Touchette, 1971). Two forms of time<br />

delay are reported in the literature, progressive<br />

time delay (PTD) <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay<br />

(CTD). During PTD a systematically increased<br />

528 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


delay is inserted between the presentati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

the discriminative stimulus <strong>and</strong> the c<strong>on</strong>trolling<br />

prompt (Cooper, Her<strong>on</strong>, & Heward,<br />

2007). In c<strong>on</strong>trast, CTD c<strong>on</strong>sists of <strong>on</strong>ly two<br />

prompting c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s, a zero-sec<strong>on</strong>d delay<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> a three- or five-sec<strong>on</strong>d delay<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. During the zero-sec<strong>on</strong>d delay c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

the stimulus <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>trolling prompt<br />

are delivered c<strong>on</strong>currently. During the threeor<br />

five-sec<strong>on</strong>d delay c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> the stimulus is<br />

presented with the specified delay inserted<br />

prior to the delivery of the c<strong>on</strong>trolling prompt<br />

to allow for independent resp<strong>on</strong>ding. Acquisiti<strong>on</strong><br />

during both PTD <strong>and</strong> CTD is measured<br />

by correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses during the delayed trials<br />

in which the student resp<strong>on</strong>ds to the stimulus<br />

prior to the presentati<strong>on</strong> of the c<strong>on</strong>trolling<br />

prompt.<br />

Purpose<br />

The purpose of this paper is to review the<br />

research literature <strong>on</strong> simultaneous prompting,<br />

a fifth prompting strategy that results in<br />

near errorless learning. This review includes<br />

skills <strong>and</strong> individuals taught using simultaneous<br />

prompting <strong>and</strong> strengths <strong>and</strong> weaknesses<br />

of simultaneous prompting as identified in<br />

the literature.<br />

Simultaneous Prompting<br />

Simultaneous prompting is a resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

prompting strategy that results in near errorless<br />

learning. During this procedure the instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

cue <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>trolling prompt are<br />

presented c<strong>on</strong>currently or simultaneously<br />

with probes c<strong>on</strong>ducted prior to the instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong> to measure skill acquisiti<strong>on</strong><br />

(Gibs<strong>on</strong> & Schuster, 1992; Schuster, Griffen,<br />

& Wolery, 1992). Simultaneous prompting<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sists of three comp<strong>on</strong>ents (a) baseline or<br />

full probe sessi<strong>on</strong>s, (b) assessment or daily<br />

probe sessi<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>and</strong> (c) instructi<strong>on</strong>al sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

During baseline/full probe sessi<strong>on</strong>s, data are<br />

collected <strong>on</strong> the students’ identificati<strong>on</strong> or<br />

completi<strong>on</strong> of all stimuli within the program.<br />

Baseline/full probe sessi<strong>on</strong>s are presented<br />

prior to the beginning of instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> typically<br />

following mastery of a set of stimuli prior<br />

to presentati<strong>on</strong> of the next set of stimuli. Full<br />

probe sessi<strong>on</strong>s may serve as baseline c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

as well as maintenance c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s. As-<br />

sessment/daily probe sessi<strong>on</strong>s which measure<br />

acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of the stimuli targeted for instructi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

are presented prior to each instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>. Assessment/daily probe sessi<strong>on</strong>s provide<br />

for independent resp<strong>on</strong>ding opportunities<br />

for the students. Instructi<strong>on</strong>al sessi<strong>on</strong>s are<br />

c<strong>on</strong>ducted following assessment/daily probe<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s each day. During instructi<strong>on</strong>al sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

the stimulus <strong>and</strong> the c<strong>on</strong>trolling prompt<br />

are presented c<strong>on</strong>currently.<br />

Method<br />

An electr<strong>on</strong>ic search of ERIC was c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

with simultaneous prompting used as the keyword.<br />

Articles also were identified through the<br />

reference lists of research articles <strong>and</strong> a published<br />

review of the literature <strong>on</strong> simultaneous<br />

prompting (Morse & Schuster, 2004). Articles<br />

were included if (a) they employed simultaneous<br />

prompting in an experimental design <strong>and</strong><br />

(b) were published in a peer review journal. A<br />

total of 35 peer reviewed articles <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>e<br />

review of the literature were identified. Investigati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

that were not included in the initial<br />

review of the literature are included in Tables<br />

1 <strong>and</strong> 2.<br />

Demographic Variables<br />

Participants. A total of 35 published studies<br />

spanning eighteen years (1992–2010) <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>on</strong>e review of the literature <strong>on</strong> simultaneous<br />

prompting are included. In an initial review of<br />

the literature Morse <strong>and</strong> Schuster (2004)<br />

identified 18 published studies which examined<br />

simultaneous prompting including 74<br />

participants. Since the initial review of the<br />

literature an additi<strong>on</strong>al 17 studies have been<br />

identified with an additi<strong>on</strong>al 62 participants<br />

for a total of 35 published studies <strong>and</strong> 136<br />

participants.<br />

Simultaneous prompting has been employed<br />

predominately with students in elementary<br />

school (Akmanoglu & Batu, 2004;<br />

Batu, 2008; Birkan, 2005; Griffen, Schuster, &<br />

Morse, 1998; Kurt & Tekin-Iftar, 2008; Parrott,<br />

Schuster, Collins, & Gassaway, 2000; Schuster<br />

& Griffen, 1993; Schuster, Griffen, & Wolery,<br />

1992; Singlet<strong>on</strong>, Schuster, & Ault, 1995; Tekin<br />

& Kircaali-Iftar, 2002; Tekin-Iftar, 2008; Tekin-<br />

Iftar, Kurt, & Acar, 2008; Waugh, Fredrick, &<br />

Alberto, 2009) but also has been implemented<br />

Simultaneous Prompting / 529


TABLE 1<br />

Summary of Demographic <strong>and</strong> Procedural Variables<br />

Independent Variable<br />

(C<strong>on</strong>trolling Prompt) Error Rates<br />

Authors Participants (Target Participants) Setting Pupil: Teacher Ratio Dependent Variable<br />

Instructi<strong>on</strong> 10.4%<br />

Probe 45%<br />

Modeling prompt paired<br />

with verbal prompt<br />

Preschool<br />

Expressive identificati<strong>on</strong><br />

of relatives<br />

2 with <strong>Autism</strong>; 5.5 years old Classroom for children<br />

with <strong>Autism</strong> in a<br />

university unit for<br />

children with<br />

development delays<br />

Akmanoglu-Uludag & Batu<br />

(2005)<br />

Instructi<strong>on</strong> (1:1)<br />

Verbal 38.5%,<br />

Motor 0%; (4:1)<br />

Verbal 49%,<br />

Motor 0%<br />

Probe (1:1) Verbal,<br />

Motor, <strong>and</strong> Instructive<br />

Feedback 41.6%; (4:<br />

1) Verbal 25%,<br />

Motor 24.2%,<br />

Instructive Feedback <br />

30%<br />

Instructi<strong>on</strong> Everyday<br />

probes 0–3.5%<br />

Every 4th day probes <br />

0–4.3%<br />

Probes First 8 days <br />

50% First 2 every 4th day<br />

28.1%<br />

Full physical prompt to<br />

perform play acti<strong>on</strong><br />

paired with a verbal<br />

model of language<br />

(1) acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

training trials of target<br />

pretend play expressive<br />

vocabulary skills (2)<br />

pretend play motor<br />

skill (3) instructive<br />

feedback - expressive<br />

vocabulary skills<br />

(1:1)<br />

Public preschool<br />

Classroom (1:1 <strong>and</strong><br />

4:1)<br />

4 with pervasive developmental<br />

disorder <strong>and</strong> developmental<br />

disabilities; 3–4 years old;<br />

Para delivered instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

Colozzi, Ward, & Crotty<br />

(2008)<br />

Verbal model of correct<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

Classroom (1:1) 14 transportati<strong>on</strong> words<br />

<strong>and</strong> 11 line drawings<br />

(2 words per c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>;<br />

3 c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s <br />

Everyday probes, Every<br />

fourth day probes <strong>and</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>trol)<br />

Reichow & Wolery (2009) 1 with speech language<br />

impairment, 1 English<br />

Language Learner, 1 with<br />

typical development, 1 at-risk<br />

for school failure; 4–5 years<br />

old<br />

Instructi<strong>on</strong> 34–64%<br />

Probe not reported<br />

Not Reported<br />

Modeling plus verbal<br />

prompt<br />

Verbal directi<strong>on</strong> paired with<br />

modeling<br />

Instructi<strong>on</strong> 0%<br />

Probes 26–35%<br />

Verbal Model of correct<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

Instructi<strong>on</strong> Not<br />

reported Probes (CTD<br />

1–16%) (SP 2–15%)<br />

CTD vs Simultaneous<br />

Prompting (Intermittent<br />

probes)<br />

Elementary<br />

Akmanoglu & Batu (2004) 3 with <strong>Autism</strong>; 6–17 years old Classroom at university Receptive identificati<strong>on</strong><br />

school (1:1)<br />

of numerals 1–9<br />

Batu (2008) 4 with <strong>Developmental</strong> Delays Home-based instructi<strong>on</strong> (1) Caregiver<br />

(IQ 41–50); 6-9 years old;<br />

implementati<strong>on</strong> (2)<br />

Caregiver-delivered<br />

Acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of home-<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

living skills (e.g.,<br />

wearing socks, making<br />

bed, etc)<br />

Birkan (2005) 1 MID, 2 MoID; 6–13 years old Classroom at research 3 discrete tasks (sight<br />

university (1:1)<br />

words, receptive<br />

identificati<strong>on</strong> of digits,<br />

telling time<br />

Kurt & Tekin-Iftar (2008) 4 with <strong>Autism</strong>; 6–8 years old School - Classroom, Turning <strong>on</strong> CD player<br />

cafeteria, free-play<br />

<strong>and</strong> taking a digital<br />

area, <strong>and</strong> hall<br />

picture<br />

530 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


TABLE 1—(C<strong>on</strong>tinued)<br />

Summary of Demographic <strong>and</strong> Procedural Variables<br />

Independent Variable<br />

(C<strong>on</strong>trolling Prompt) Error Rates<br />

Participants (Target<br />

Participants) Setting Pupil: Teacher Ratio Dependent Variable<br />

Authors<br />

Instructi<strong>on</strong> 0% Probes<br />

14–19%<br />

Model with verbal<br />

descripti<strong>on</strong><br />

11 - 15 step task analysis<br />

for purchasing<br />

Community settings<br />

(grocery store, pastry<br />

shop, <strong>and</strong> dry cleaning<br />

store)<br />

Instructi<strong>on</strong> 0.3%<br />

Probes 7.5%<br />

Verbal Model of correct<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

Tekin-Iftar (2008) 3 with <strong>Autism</strong>, 1 with<br />

MoID; 7–12 years old;<br />

Parent-delivered<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

Tekin-Iftar, Kurt, & Acar (2008) 2 with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>;<br />

7-8 years old<br />

Instructi<strong>on</strong> not reported<br />

Probes not reported<br />

Verbal model of correct<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

3 students with MoID;<br />

9–11 years old<br />

Waugh, Fredrick, & Alberto<br />

(2009)<br />

Not reported<br />

Verbal Model of correct<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

5 with MoID (IQ 40–<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>); 12-15 years old<br />

Alberto, Waugh, & Fredrick<br />

(2010)<br />

Instructi<strong>on</strong> 2.14%<br />

Probes 6.57%<br />

Verbal prompt paired with a<br />

model<br />

3 with MID, 2 with<br />

MoID; 11–14 years<br />

Gursel, Tekin-Iftar, & Bozkurt<br />

(2006)<br />

Verbal prompt paired with a Not reported<br />

model<br />

Verbal of correct resp<strong>on</strong>se Instructi<strong>on</strong> 8.6%– 17.6%<br />

Probes 15.2%–26.4%<br />

University Unit (1:1) Tool identificati<strong>on</strong> with<br />

instructive feedback;<br />

intermittent probe<br />

C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

Self-c<strong>on</strong>tained classroom Correct reading of sight-<br />

in public school (1:1) words, letter-sound<br />

corresp<strong>on</strong>dences, <strong>and</strong><br />

blending skills<br />

Middle School<br />

Self-c<strong>on</strong>tained classroom Verbal reading <strong>and</strong><br />

in public school (2:1 motoric dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> 3:1)<br />

of comprehensi<strong>on</strong> of<br />

individual sight words<br />

<strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>nected text<br />

Classroom (2:1);<br />

Discrete skills -<br />

heterogeneous dyadic identificati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

grouping<br />

provinces, rivers, <strong>and</strong><br />

border countries <strong>on</strong><br />

Turkish map <strong>and</strong><br />

expressive identificati<strong>on</strong><br />

of math symbols<br />

Self-c<strong>on</strong>tained classroom 12-step task analysis for<br />

(1:1)<br />

subtracti<strong>on</strong> with decimals<br />

Classroom (1:1) Recall of multiplicati<strong>on</strong><br />

Facts<br />

Not reported<br />

CTD vs Simultaneous<br />

Prompting<br />

Different discrete tasks -<br />

(1) expressively read<br />

words from general<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> vocabulary<br />

lists (2) Verbally define<br />

key vocabulary words<br />

Acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of community<br />

signs with instructive<br />

feedback<br />

Special educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

classrooms (large<br />

group)<br />

Rao & Kane (2009) 2 with Educable Mental<br />

Impairement<br />

Rao & Mallow (2009) 2 with cognitive<br />

impairments (IQ <br />

49 & 62)<br />

Riesen et al. (2003) 1 with autism, 2 with<br />

multiple disabilities<br />

(IQ 50–55), 1 with<br />

MID (IQ 58–70); 2<br />

paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>als<br />

Instructi<strong>on</strong> 1.1%<br />

Probes 2.21%<br />

Verbal Model of correct<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

Counselor’s Office in a<br />

public school (tutor<br />

dyads)<br />

Tekin-Iftar (2003) 4 typical peers, 4 with<br />

developmental<br />

disabilities; 10-13<br />

years old<br />

Instructi<strong>on</strong> 0%<br />

Probes 29%<br />

Verbal Model of correct<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

Expressive identificati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

first aid materials <strong>and</strong><br />

instructive feedback<br />

Classroom in a public<br />

school (1:1)<br />

Tekin-Iftar, Acar, & Kurt (2003) 3 with MID; 13–14 years<br />

old<br />

Simultaneous Prompting / 531


TABLE 2<br />

Summary of Outcome Variables<br />

Authors Design Results Maintenance Generalizati<strong>on</strong> Social Validity<br />

Parent Questi<strong>on</strong>naire<br />

Across materials, settings,<br />

<strong>and</strong> trainers<br />

Preschool<br />

1, 2, <strong>and</strong> 4 weeks<br />

following mastery<br />

criteri<strong>on</strong><br />

Does not specify when<br />

collected<br />

Multiple Probe All students acquired<br />

targeted skills<br />

Akmanoglu-Uludag & Batu<br />

(2005)<br />

Questi<strong>on</strong>naire –<br />

Parents, Preschool<br />

Teachers <strong>and</strong><br />

Paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>als<br />

Across people, setting,<br />

<strong>and</strong> materials<br />

Colozzi et al. (2008) Multiple Probe Simultaneous prompting<br />

effective - met criteri<strong>on</strong>.<br />

No significant<br />

differences in probe<br />

errors for verbal <strong>and</strong><br />

motor resp<strong>on</strong>ses<br />

between 1:1 <strong>and</strong> small<br />

Not reported Not reported Not reported<br />

group<br />

3 of 4 participants<br />

acquired target stimuli<br />

under both<br />

simultaneous prompting<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s; 1 student<br />

acquired target stimuli<br />

in the every 4th day<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly<br />

Reichow & Wolery (2009) Adapted Alternating<br />

Treatments<br />

Across materials Parent Questi<strong>on</strong>naire<br />

Across trainers Semi-structured<br />

interview<br />

Across setting <strong>and</strong><br />

Not reported<br />

materials<br />

Not reported 16 instructors <strong>and</strong><br />

professors<br />

completed<br />

Questi<strong>on</strong>naire<br />

Across settings Mother <strong>and</strong> Student<br />

Questi<strong>on</strong>naire<br />

Multiple exemplars Not reported<br />

Elementary<br />

Akmanoglu & Batu (2004) Multiple Probe All students met Criteri<strong>on</strong> 1, 2, <strong>and</strong> 4 weeks<br />

following<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

mastery<br />

Batu (2008) Multiple Probe All students met Criteri<strong>on</strong> 1 <strong>and</strong> 3 weeks<br />

following<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

mastery<br />

Birkan (2005) Multiple Probe All students met Criteri<strong>on</strong> 7, 18, <strong>and</strong> 25 days after<br />

criteri<strong>on</strong><br />

Kurt & Tekin-Iftar (2008) Adapted Alternating Both equally effective with 1, 2, <strong>and</strong> 4 weeks<br />

Treatments<br />

mixed efficiency data<br />

following<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

mastery<br />

Tekin-Iftar (2008) Multiple Probe All students met criteri<strong>on</strong> 2 <strong>and</strong> 5 weeks<br />

following<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

mastery<br />

Tekin-Iftar, Kurt, & Acar Multiple Probe All students met criteri<strong>on</strong> 1, 2, <strong>and</strong> 4 weeks<br />

(2007)<br />

following<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

mastery<br />

532 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


TABLE 2—(C<strong>on</strong>tinued)<br />

Summary of Outcome Variables<br />

Authors Design Results Maintenance Generalizati<strong>on</strong> Social Validity<br />

Preschool<br />

Waugh et al. (2009) Changing Criteri<strong>on</strong> All 3 students met<br />

Measured during the Across materials Teacher Interview<br />

criteri<strong>on</strong> through<br />

study due to<br />

Blending Set 2, <strong>on</strong>e<br />

summer break; not<br />

student met criteri<strong>on</strong><br />

measured followed<br />

through Blending Set 5<br />

mastery of all phases<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>e student met<br />

of instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

criteri<strong>on</strong> through<br />

Blending Set 3<br />

Middle School<br />

Alberto et al (2010) Changing Criteri<strong>on</strong> All students met criteri<strong>on</strong> Not measured Across materials Teacher<br />

embedded in a<br />

Questi<strong>on</strong>naire<br />

Multiple Baseline<br />

across groups<br />

Gursel et al. (2006) Multiple Probe All students met criteri<strong>on</strong> 2 <strong>and</strong> 6 weeks<br />

Across people <strong>and</strong><br />

Not reported<br />

following<br />

materials<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

mastery<br />

Rao & Kane (2009) Multiple Probe Both students met<br />

10 days following<br />

Across settings, materials, Not reported<br />

criteri<strong>on</strong><br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

<strong>and</strong> professi<strong>on</strong>als<br />

mastery<br />

Rao & Mallow (2009) Multiple Probe Both students met<br />

1 <strong>and</strong> 3 weeks<br />

Across format, setting,<br />

Not reported<br />

criteri<strong>on</strong><br />

following<br />

<strong>and</strong> pers<strong>on</strong>nel<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

mastery<br />

Riesen et al. (2003) Adapted Alternating 3 students reached<br />

Not reported Not reported Not reported<br />

Treatments<br />

criteri<strong>on</strong> under both<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s, 1 student<br />

reached criteri<strong>on</strong> under<br />

simultaneous prompting<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly<br />

Tekin-Iftar (2003) Multiple Probe All met criteri<strong>on</strong> 1 week following<br />

Across people Not reported<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

mastery<br />

Tekin-Iftar, Acar, & Kurt Multiple Probe All met criteri<strong>on</strong> 1, 2, <strong>and</strong> 4 weeks<br />

Not reported Not reported<br />

(2003)<br />

following<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

mastery<br />

Simultaneous Prompting / 533


with students in preschool (Akmanogu-<br />

Uludag & Batu, 2005; Colozzi, Ward, & Crotty,<br />

2008; Dogan & Tekin-Iftar, 2002; Gibs<strong>on</strong> &<br />

Schuster, 1992; MacFarl<strong>and</strong>-Smith, Schuster,<br />

& Stevens, 1993; Reichow & Wolery, 2009;<br />

Sewell, Collins, Hemmeter, & Schuster, 1998),<br />

middle school (Alberto, Waugh, & Fredrick,<br />

2010; Fickel, Schuster, & Collins, 1998; Gursel,<br />

Tekin-Iftar, & Bozkurt, 2006; Rao & Kane,<br />

2009; Rao & Mallow, 2009; Riesen, McD<strong>on</strong>nell,<br />

Johns<strong>on</strong>, Polychr<strong>on</strong>is, & James<strong>on</strong>, 2003;<br />

Tekin-Iftar, 2003; Tekin-Iftar, Acar, & Kurt,<br />

2003), <strong>and</strong> high school (Fetko, Schuster, Harley,<br />

& Collins, 1999; Johns<strong>on</strong>, Schuster, & Bell,<br />

1996; Parker & Schuster, 2002; Singlet<strong>on</strong>,<br />

Schuster, Morse, & Collins, 1999), <strong>and</strong> with<br />

adults (Maciag, Schuster, Collins, & Cooper,<br />

2000; Palmer, Collins, & Schuster, 1999). The<br />

procedure has been employed in 19 studies<br />

with a total of 51 participants with moderate<br />

intellectual disabilities (MoID) (Alberto et al.,<br />

2010; Batu, 2008; Birkan, 2005; Dogan &<br />

Tekin-Iftar, 2002; Fickel et al., 1998; Griffen et<br />

al., 1998; Gursel et al., 2006; Maciag, et al.,<br />

2000; Parker & Schuster, 2002; Parrott, et al.,<br />

2000; Rao & Mallow, 2009; Riesen et al., 2003;<br />

Schuster & Griffen, 1993; Schuster et al., 1992;<br />

Singlet<strong>on</strong> et al., 1995; Singlet<strong>on</strong> et al., 1999;<br />

Tekin & Kircaali-Iftar, 2002; Tekin-Iftar, 2008;<br />

Waugh et al., 2009). The procedure also has<br />

been employed in 13 studies with a total of 23<br />

participants with mild intellectual disabilities<br />

(MID) (Birkan, 2005; Dogan & Tekin-Iftar,<br />

2002; Fickel et al., 1998; Gursel et al., 2006;<br />

Johns<strong>on</strong> et al., 1996; Palmer et al.,1999;<br />

Parker & Schuster, 2002; Rao & Kane, 2009;<br />

Rao & Mallow, 2009; Riesen et al., 2003; Tekin<br />

& Kircaali-Iftar, 2002; Tekin-Iftar, 2003; Tekin-<br />

Iftar et al., 2003), 7 studies with a total of 18<br />

participants with autism (Akmanoglu & Batu,<br />

2004; Akmanoglu-Uludag & Batu, 2005;<br />

Colozzi et al., 2008; Kurt & Tekin-Iftar, 2008;<br />

Parrott et al., 2000; Riesen et al., 2003; Tekin-<br />

Iftar, 2008), 5 studies with a total of 10 participants<br />

with typical development (Fickel et al.,<br />

1998; Gibs<strong>on</strong> & Schuster, 1992; Parker &<br />

Schuster, 2002; Reichow & Wolery, 2009;<br />

Tekin-Iftar et al., 2003), 4 studies with a total<br />

of 11 participants with severe intellectual disabilities<br />

(SID) (Colozzi et al., 2008; Fetko et<br />

al.,1999; Maciag et al., 2000; Parrott et al.,<br />

2000), 4 studies with 12 participants with developmental<br />

delays (Gibs<strong>on</strong> & Schuster, 1992;<br />

MacFarl<strong>and</strong>-Smith et al., 1992; Sewell et al.,<br />

1998; Wolery et al., 1993), <strong>on</strong>e study with a<br />

total of three participants with learning disabilities<br />

(Johns<strong>on</strong> et al., 1996), <strong>on</strong>e study with<br />

a participant with spina bifida (Gibs<strong>on</strong> &<br />

Schuster, 1992), <strong>on</strong>e study with a participant<br />

classified as a slow learner (Tekin-Iftar, 2003),<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>e study which included a student with a<br />

speech-language impairment, a student who<br />

was classified as an English Language Learner,<br />

<strong>and</strong> a student identified as at-risk for school<br />

failure (Reichow & Wolery, 2009).<br />

In the same way that simultaneous prompting<br />

has been employed with a variety of participants,<br />

a variety of individuals have implemented<br />

the procedure. While this procedure<br />

predominately has been implemented by<br />

classroom teachers (Griffen et al., 1998; Gursel<br />

et al., 2008; Waugh et al., 2009), it also has<br />

been implemented by paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>als<br />

(Colozzi et al., 2008; Riesen et al., 2003), parents<br />

(Tekin-Iftar, 2008), caregivers (Batu,<br />

2008), sibling tutors (Tekin & Kircaali-Iftar,<br />

2002), <strong>and</strong> peer tutors (Tekin-Iftar, 2003). Simultaneous<br />

prompting is executed with a<br />

high level of procedural fidelity, ranging from<br />

84100% across all implementers.<br />

Instructi<strong>on</strong>al Grouping<br />

The majority of studies which have employed<br />

simultaneous prompting have used individual<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>al formats (Akmanoglu & Batu,<br />

2004; Akamanoglu-Uludag & Batu, 2005;<br />

Batu, 2008; Birkan, 2005; Dogan & Tekin-<br />

Iftar, 2002; Fetko et al., 1999; Gibs<strong>on</strong> & Schuster,<br />

1992; Griffen et al., 1998; Parrott et al.,<br />

2000; Rao & Kane, 2009; Rao & Mallow, 2009;<br />

Reichow et al., 2009; Riesen et al., 2003; Schuster<br />

et al., 1992; Singlet<strong>on</strong> et al., 1999; Tekin &<br />

Kircaali-Iftar, 2002; Tekin & Iftar, 2003; Tekin-<br />

Iftar et al, 2003; Tekin-Iftar et al, 2008). Six<br />

studies have implemented the instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

strategy in a group format, ranging from a 2:1<br />

format to an 11:1 format. Singlet<strong>on</strong> et al.<br />

(1995) were the first to examine simultaneous<br />

prompting in a group format using dyads. The<br />

researchers found that simultaneous prompting<br />

could be implemented effectively in dyads<br />

to teach basic discrete identificati<strong>on</strong> of community<br />

signs to students with MoID. Maciag et<br />

al. (2000) further examined the use of simultaneous<br />

prompting in teaching a chained vo-<br />

534 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


cati<strong>on</strong>al task in a dyadic group format to<br />

adults with SID. Gursel et al. (2006) also examined<br />

a heterogeneous dyadic group format<br />

in teaching discrete skills to students with developmental<br />

disabilities. Fickel et al. (1998)<br />

<strong>and</strong> Parker <strong>and</strong> Schuster (2002) further exp<strong>and</strong>ed<br />

the literature <strong>on</strong> simultaneous<br />

prompting in a group format by teaching a<br />

heterogeneous group of students discrete<br />

skills. Students were taught different tasks using<br />

different stimuli in a group format of 4:1<br />

(Fickel et al., 1998) <strong>and</strong> 5:1 (Parker & Schuster,<br />

2002). Johns<strong>on</strong> et al. (1996) c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>al sessi<strong>on</strong>s in the largest group format<br />

of 11:1 in teaching high school students<br />

with mild disabilities. Across all studies, simultaneous<br />

prompting implemented in both individual<br />

<strong>and</strong> group formats has been effective<br />

in teaching targeted skills.<br />

Only <strong>on</strong>e study directly compared the effects<br />

of simultaneous prompting in individual<br />

<strong>and</strong> group formats (Colozzi et al., 2008).<br />

Colozzi <strong>and</strong> colleagues compared the effectiveness<br />

of simultaneous prompting in individual<br />

format (1:1) <strong>and</strong> a group format (4:1) in<br />

teaching four students with autism pretend<br />

play vocabulary <strong>and</strong> motor skills. While group<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> required more instructi<strong>on</strong>al sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<strong>and</strong> resulted in more instructi<strong>on</strong>al errors<br />

there were no significant differences in probe<br />

errors across the two instructi<strong>on</strong>al formats.<br />

Although group instructi<strong>on</strong> required more instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s to mastery, the implementati<strong>on</strong><br />

of group instructi<strong>on</strong> may allow for the<br />

acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of additi<strong>on</strong>al skills through the use<br />

of n<strong>on</strong>targeted instructi<strong>on</strong>al feedback <strong>and</strong> observati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

learning.<br />

Observati<strong>on</strong>al Learning <strong>and</strong> Instructive Feedback<br />

Observati<strong>on</strong>al learning c<strong>on</strong>sists of learning<br />

through observing others engaging in an activity<br />

or being taught a specific activity. In<br />

order for observati<strong>on</strong>al learning to occur students<br />

must dem<strong>on</strong>strate imitative behaviors<br />

(Wolery et al., 1992). Some students with<br />

moderate to severe intellectual disabilities<br />

who dem<strong>on</strong>strate imitative behaviors can acquire<br />

n<strong>on</strong>targeted skills through observati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

learning. Several studies which employed<br />

simultaneous prompting in a group<br />

format have examined the acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of n<strong>on</strong>targeted<br />

informati<strong>on</strong> through observati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

learning (Fickel et al., 1998; Gursel et al.,<br />

2006; Parker & Schuster, 2002; Singlet<strong>on</strong> et<br />

al., 1999). Fickel et al. found that students<br />

acquired 66% to 100% of their peer’s target<br />

stimuli through observati<strong>on</strong>al learning. Similarly,<br />

Gursel et al. found students acquiring<br />

33% to 100% of their peer’s target stimuli<br />

through observati<strong>on</strong>al learning. Parker <strong>and</strong><br />

Schuster <strong>and</strong> Singlet<strong>on</strong> et al. measured observati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

learning of target stimuli as well as<br />

instructive feedback.<br />

Instructive feedback c<strong>on</strong>sists of additi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

informati<strong>on</strong> that provides the student with<br />

supplementary details about the target stimulus<br />

(Tekin-Iftar et al., 2008). Instructive feedback<br />

has been used widely in the teaching of<br />

target skills using simultaneous prompting<br />

(Colozzi et al., 2008; Griffen et al., 1998; Gursel<br />

et al., 2006; Parker & Schuster, 2002; Singlet<strong>on</strong><br />

et al., 1999; Tekin-Iftar, 2003; Tekin-<br />

Iftar et al., 2003; Tekin-Iftar et al., 2008).<br />

While observati<strong>on</strong>al learning requires a group<br />

format, instructive feedback can be implemented<br />

<strong>and</strong> measured in both individual <strong>and</strong><br />

group formats. Wolery, Holcombe, Werts, <strong>and</strong><br />

Cipoll<strong>on</strong>i (1993) provided instructive feedback<br />

to teach classificati<strong>on</strong> of food <strong>and</strong> drink<br />

items while teaching receptive identificati<strong>on</strong><br />

of rebus symbols of specific food <strong>and</strong> drink<br />

items to preschool students with developmental<br />

disabilities. Students were provided with<br />

informati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>cerning the classificati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

when (e.g., We eat cereal for breakfast) <strong>and</strong><br />

how (e.g., Juice is a drink). Two of the five<br />

students correctly classified all the target stimuli<br />

<strong>and</strong> the remaining three students correctly<br />

classified some of the target stimuli. Gursel et<br />

al. (2006) taught a heterogeneous group of<br />

middle school students with MID <strong>and</strong> MoID a<br />

variety of discrete skills ranging from map<br />

skills to mathematical symbol identificati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Instructive feedback included additi<strong>on</strong>al geographical<br />

informati<strong>on</strong>. Students acquired 33<br />

to 100% of the instructive feedback. Parker<br />

<strong>and</strong> Schuster taught a variety of discrete skills<br />

to two high school students with typical development<br />

<strong>and</strong> two students with MID/MoID.<br />

Three of four of the student acquired some of<br />

their targeted instructive feedback (range 25–<br />

83% accuracy) <strong>and</strong> some of their group members<br />

targeted instructive feedback (range<br />

9–38% accuracy). Singlet<strong>on</strong> et al. reported<br />

similar findings with elementary-aged students<br />

Simultaneous Prompting / 535


with MoID acquiring some of their peer’s target<br />

stimuli (47–54%) <strong>and</strong> instructive feedback<br />

(61–81%) through observati<strong>on</strong>al learning.<br />

Targeted Skills<br />

Simultaneous prompting has been used to<br />

teach a variety of discrete <strong>and</strong> chained skills.<br />

Skills taught using simultaneous prompting<br />

include literacy skills (Birkan, 2005; Gibs<strong>on</strong> &<br />

Schuster, 1992; Griffen et al., 1998; Johns<strong>on</strong>,<br />

et al., 1996; Parker & Schuster, 2002; Reichow<br />

& Wolery, 2009; Riesen et al., 2003; Schuster<br />

et al., 1992; Singlet<strong>on</strong> et al., 1995; Singlet<strong>on</strong> et<br />

al., 1999; Tekin-Iftar, 2003; Waugh et al.,<br />

2009), math skills (Akmanoglu & Batu, 2004;<br />

Birkan, 2005; Fickel et al., 1998; Gursel et al.,<br />

2006; Rao & Kane, 2009; Rao & Mallow, 2009),<br />

communicati<strong>on</strong> skills (Akmanoglu-Uludag &<br />

Batu, 2005; Dogan & Tekin-Iftar, 2002; Fickel<br />

et al., 1998; Tekin & Kircaali-Iftar, 2002;<br />

Tekin-Iftar et al., 2003; Tekin-Iftar, 2008; Wolery<br />

et al., 1993), daily living skills (Batu, 2008;<br />

Fetko et al., 1999; Parrott et al., 2000; Schuster<br />

& Griffen, 1993; Sewell et al., 1998; Tekin-<br />

Iftar, 2008), leisure skills (Colozzi et al., 2008;<br />

Kurt & Tekin-Iftar, 2008), <strong>and</strong> vocati<strong>on</strong>al skills<br />

(Maciag et al., 2000).<br />

Literacy skills. The most comm<strong>on</strong> skill<br />

taught employing simultaneous prompting is<br />

literacy instructi<strong>on</strong>. Of the 35 studies c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

employing simultaneous prompting,<br />

16 studies examined some comp<strong>on</strong>ent of literacy<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> with the majority of those<br />

studies focused <strong>on</strong> sight-word instructi<strong>on</strong>. The<br />

words targeted for instructi<strong>on</strong> include grocery<br />

words (Parker & Schuster, 2002; Schuster et<br />

al., 1992; Singlet<strong>on</strong> et al., 1999), envir<strong>on</strong>mental<br />

words (Griffen et al., 1998), academic vocabulary<br />

words (Johns<strong>on</strong> et al., 1996; Riesen et<br />

al., 2003), occupati<strong>on</strong>al words (Parker &<br />

Schuster, 2002), community words <strong>and</strong>/or<br />

signs (Singlet<strong>on</strong> et al., 1995; Tekin-Iftar,<br />

2003), thematic words (Reichow & Wolery,<br />

2009), <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>trolled vocabulary (Alberto et<br />

al., 2010; Birkan, 2005; Gibs<strong>on</strong> & Schuster,<br />

1992; Waugh et al., 2009). Simultaneous<br />

prompting was employed with a total of 50<br />

participants ranging from typically developing<br />

students (Reichow & Wolery, 2009) to students<br />

with MoID (e.g., Waugh et al.) <strong>and</strong> was<br />

effective in teaching sight words to 49 of 50<br />

participants. While most studies taught sight<br />

words in isolati<strong>on</strong>, two studies exp<strong>and</strong>ed up<strong>on</strong><br />

the individual approach to sight-word instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

to include reading of c<strong>on</strong>nected (Alberto<br />

et al., 2010) <strong>and</strong> exp<strong>and</strong>ing to ph<strong>on</strong>ics instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

(Waugh et al.). Alberto et al. systematically<br />

taught five students with MoID to read<br />

individual sight words composed of various<br />

parts of speech. Students also were taught to<br />

read the individual sight words in various<br />

forms of c<strong>on</strong>nected text <strong>and</strong> dem<strong>on</strong>strate<br />

comprehensi<strong>on</strong> of what was read. All five students<br />

read the sight words in both individual<br />

<strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>nected text formats <strong>and</strong> were able to<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strate comprehensi<strong>on</strong>. Waugh et al.<br />

also exp<strong>and</strong>ed <strong>on</strong> the use of simultaneous<br />

prompting to teach sight words to students<br />

with MoID by first teaching three elementary<br />

students with MoID to read targeted sight<br />

words <strong>and</strong> then teaching corresp<strong>on</strong>ding ph<strong>on</strong>ics<br />

skills. The students were taught to read<br />

four sight words using simultaneous prompting.<br />

Once students reached mastery <strong>on</strong> the<br />

four sight words, they were taught the corresp<strong>on</strong>ding<br />

letter-sound corresp<strong>on</strong>dences for<br />

the graphemes in each word. Students were<br />

then taught the skill of blending to read the<br />

previously taught sight words. The students<br />

successfully acquired the sight words <strong>and</strong> various<br />

numbers of the blending words. The students<br />

were able to read some but not all generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

words.<br />

Math skills. Of the 35 studies which implemented<br />

simultaneous prompting, 6 of the<br />

studies addressed math skills. Of these six<br />

studies, five taught discrete skills, such as number<br />

identificati<strong>on</strong> (Akmanoglu & Batu, 2005;<br />

Birkan, 2005), math symbol identificati<strong>on</strong><br />

(Gursel et al., 2006), multiplicati<strong>on</strong> facts identificati<strong>on</strong><br />

(Rao & Mallow, 2009), additi<strong>on</strong> facts<br />

identificati<strong>on</strong> (Fickel et al., 1998), <strong>and</strong> telling<br />

time (Birkan, 2005). Only <strong>on</strong>e study examined<br />

the use of simultaneous prompting to<br />

teach the chained math skill of subtracti<strong>on</strong><br />

with decimals (Rao & Kane, 2009). Using simultaneous<br />

prompting Rao <strong>and</strong> Kane taught<br />

the chained academic skills of subtracti<strong>on</strong> to<br />

two students (reported IQ scores 47–50). Students<br />

mastered subtracti<strong>on</strong> with regrouping<br />

in 25 or fewer sessi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> maintained <strong>and</strong><br />

generalized the math skills. Simultaneous<br />

prompting was employed with a total of 11<br />

participants <strong>and</strong> was effective in teaching<br />

math skills to all of the participants.<br />

536 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


Communicati<strong>on</strong> skills. Simultaneous prompting<br />

has been used to teach communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

skills in 7 of the 35 published studies. Communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

skills taught include expressive<br />

naming of relatives for preschool students<br />

with autism (Akmnaoglu-Uludag & Batu,<br />

2004), receptive identificati<strong>on</strong> of occupati<strong>on</strong><br />

picture cards for two preschool students with<br />

MoID <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>e preschool student with MID<br />

(Dogan & Tekin-Iftar, 1998), manual sign producti<strong>on</strong><br />

of six communicati<strong>on</strong> symbols for<br />

three middle school students with MID/MoID<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>e student without disabilities (Fickel et<br />

al., 1998), receptive identificati<strong>on</strong> of animals<br />

for three elementary students with MID/<br />

MoID (Tekin & Kircaali-Iftar, 2002), expressive<br />

identificati<strong>on</strong> of first aid materials for<br />

three middle school students with MID<br />

(Tekin-Iftar et al., 2003), <strong>and</strong> expressive identificati<strong>on</strong><br />

of tools for two elementary students<br />

with intellectual disabilities (Tekin-Iftar et al.,<br />

2008). The <strong>on</strong>e receptive skill taught was identificati<strong>on</strong><br />

of rebus symbols by five preschool<br />

students with developmental disabilities (Wolery<br />

et al., 1993). Across these seven studies<br />

simultaneous prompting was effective in<br />

teaching 21 of 23 participants with the remaining<br />

two participants not reaching mastery<br />

criteria but dem<strong>on</strong>strating an increase in<br />

performance over baseline.<br />

Daily living skills. Of the 35 studies examining<br />

simultaneous prompting, 6 studies examined<br />

the acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of daily skills (Batu,<br />

2008; Fetko et al., 1999; Parrott et al., 2000;<br />

Schuster & Griffen, 1993; Sewell et al., 1998;<br />

Tekin-Iftar, 2008). Simultaneous prompting<br />

was employed to teach home living skills, such<br />

as setting the table, preparing s<strong>and</strong>wiches,<br />

hanging clothes, folding clothes, etc (Batu,<br />

2008), making juice (Schuster & Griffen,<br />

1993) dressing skills (Sewell et al., 1998),<br />

opening a key lock (Fetko et al., 1999), h<strong>and</strong><br />

washing skills (Parrott et al., 2000), <strong>and</strong> purchasing<br />

skills (Tekin-Iftar, 2008). This strategy<br />

was successful in teaching 20 of the 23 participants.<br />

The use of simultaneous prompting to<br />

teach daily living skills was implemented predominately<br />

by classroom teachers. Tekin-Iftar<br />

(2008) was the first to examine the effectiveness<br />

of implementati<strong>on</strong> of the procedure in a<br />

natural setting by a parent. Four students with<br />

developmental delays were taught purchasing<br />

skills in the natural setting (i.e., grocery store,<br />

pastry shop, <strong>and</strong> dry cleaning store). The students<br />

acquired the targeted purchasing skills<br />

<strong>and</strong> were able to generalize those skills to<br />

purchasing of items in different locati<strong>on</strong>s. The<br />

parents effectively delivered simultaneous<br />

prompting at an average of 91% accuracy.<br />

Batu (2008) further examined the implementati<strong>on</strong><br />

of caregiver-delivered simultaneous<br />

prompting to teach home skills (e.g., setting<br />

the table, preparing food, hanging clothes,<br />

etc) to four elementary students with developmental<br />

delays. All four students acquired the<br />

targeted stimuli <strong>and</strong> maintained the skills over<br />

time. Students were able to generalize the<br />

skills across individuals in the naturalistic setting.<br />

This study provided initial support for<br />

the implementati<strong>on</strong> of simultaneous prompting<br />

with caregivers of students with disabilities.<br />

Across all students <strong>and</strong> caregivers, reliability<br />

data were reported at a range of 87%–<br />

100% accuracy. These studies also support the<br />

ease with which simultaneous prompting can<br />

be implemented reliably.<br />

Leisure skills. Colozzi et al. (2008) <strong>and</strong><br />

Kurt <strong>and</strong> Tekin-Iftar (2008) examined the effects<br />

of simultaneous prompting in teaching<br />

leisure/play skills to students with autism.<br />

Colozzi et al. analyzed the effects of simultaneous<br />

prompting in teaching pretend play<br />

skills to preschool students with autism in<br />

both individual <strong>and</strong> group instructi<strong>on</strong>al formats.<br />

Students were taught vocabulary <strong>and</strong><br />

motor skills to represent the pretend play activity.<br />

All students acquired the targeted skills<br />

<strong>and</strong> maintained the skills at 100% accuracy,<br />

<strong>and</strong> individual instructi<strong>on</strong> was more efficient,<br />

requiring fewer instructi<strong>on</strong>al sessi<strong>on</strong>s than<br />

group instructi<strong>on</strong>. However, group instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

allowed for the acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of observati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

learning resp<strong>on</strong>ses. Kurt <strong>and</strong> Tekin-<br />

Iftar compared the resp<strong>on</strong>se prompting<br />

strategies of CTD <strong>and</strong> simultaneous prompting<br />

in teaching four students with autism to<br />

engage in two leisure skills of turning <strong>on</strong> a<br />

compact disc player <strong>and</strong> taking a digital picture.<br />

Both procedures were effective in teaching<br />

the targeted leisure skills to students with<br />

autism. Efficiency data produced mixed results<br />

as in previous studies with two students<br />

requiring the leisure skills in fewer sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with CTD <strong>and</strong> two students requiring fewer<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s with simultaneous prompting.<br />

Simultaneous Prompting / 537


Vocati<strong>on</strong>al task. To date <strong>on</strong>e study has examined<br />

the effectiveness of simultaneous<br />

prompting in teaching a vocati<strong>on</strong>al task. Ten<br />

adults with MoID <strong>and</strong> SID were taught to assemble<br />

boxes at a sheltered work site in<br />

groups of two (Maciag et al., 2000). Simultaneous<br />

prompting was effective for teaching 4<br />

of the 5 dyads. The remaining dyad was unable<br />

to complete the task to criteri<strong>on</strong> due to<br />

time c<strong>on</strong>straints. The employees acquired the<br />

targeted skill within a maximum of twenty<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> maintained the skill fifteen weeks<br />

after instructi<strong>on</strong> at a range of 73–93% accuracy.<br />

Comparis<strong>on</strong> of Instructi<strong>on</strong>al Strategies<br />

In order to determine the effectiveness <strong>and</strong><br />

efficiency of simultaneous prompting, researchers<br />

have compared simultaneous<br />

prompting to other resp<strong>on</strong>se prompting strategies.<br />

Simultaneous prompting has been compared<br />

to CTD (Kurt & Tekin-Iftar, 2008; Riesen<br />

et al., 2003; Schuster et al., 1992; Tekin &<br />

Kircaali-Iftar, 2002) <strong>and</strong> antecedent-prompt<br />

<strong>and</strong> test procedure (Singlet<strong>on</strong> et al., 1999).<br />

Simultaneous prompting is c<strong>on</strong>sidered an adaptati<strong>on</strong><br />

of these two differing resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

prompting procedures (Schuster et al., 1992).<br />

In CTD, simultaneous prompting is comparable<br />

to the zero-sec<strong>on</strong>d delay interval (Schuster<br />

et al., 1992). However, simultaneous prompting<br />

does not transiti<strong>on</strong> to increased delayed<br />

intervals as in CTD.<br />

During the antecedent-prompt <strong>and</strong> test procedure<br />

the teacher presents the stimulus <strong>and</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>trolling prompt together <strong>and</strong> then provides<br />

an opportunity for the student to resp<strong>on</strong>d<br />

independently to the stimulus during<br />

probe or test trials (Wolery, Ault, & Doyle,<br />

1992). In the antecedent-prompt <strong>and</strong> test procedure<br />

trials in which the stimulus <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>trolling<br />

prompt are presented together always<br />

occur prior to probe trials (Wolery et al.). In<br />

c<strong>on</strong>trast, during simultaneous prompting<br />

probes are c<strong>on</strong>ducted prior to instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

C<strong>on</strong>stant time delay. Schuster et al. (1992)<br />

first examined the effectiveness of simultaneous<br />

prompting by comparing the procedure<br />

to CTD in teaching four elementary students<br />

with MoID to read grocery words. While both<br />

procedures were effective in teaching sight<br />

words to students with MOID, simultaneous<br />

prompting required fewer instructi<strong>on</strong>al sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<strong>and</strong> less instructi<strong>on</strong>al time <strong>and</strong> resulted<br />

in fewer errors. It should be noted that the<br />

reducti<strong>on</strong> in instructi<strong>on</strong>al time with simultaneous<br />

prompting was minimal for three of the<br />

four students ranging from 30-sec<strong>on</strong>ds to<br />

3-minutes <strong>and</strong> substantial for <strong>on</strong>e student (11minutes).<br />

Maintenance data for the procedure<br />

was mixed with two students producing<br />

better maintenance with words taught with<br />

CTD <strong>and</strong> two students producing better maintenance<br />

with words taught with simultaneous<br />

prompting. This study provided initial support<br />

for the use of simultaneous prompting in<br />

teaching students with MoID.<br />

Riesen et al. (2003) further compared the<br />

effectiveness <strong>and</strong> efficiency of CTD <strong>and</strong> simultaneous<br />

prompting in teaching two junior<br />

high school students to read academic words<br />

<strong>and</strong> two junior high school students to define<br />

academic vocabulary words within an embedded-instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

format. Three students<br />

reached criteri<strong>on</strong> under both c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

while <strong>on</strong>e student reached criteri<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly in<br />

the simultaneous prompting c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. This<br />

study further validated the use of simultaneous<br />

prompting as an effective instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

strategy for teaching literacy skills to students<br />

with disabilities.<br />

Tekin <strong>and</strong> Kircaali-Iftar (2003) examined<br />

the effects of simultaneous prompting <strong>and</strong><br />

CTD in teaching students with MID <strong>and</strong> MoID<br />

to receptively identify animals. Three students<br />

with MID/MoID were taught by sibling tutors<br />

to identify animals receptively. Both procedures<br />

were implemented with a high level of<br />

fidelity by sibling tutors. Both procedures<br />

were effective in teaching receptive identificati<strong>on</strong><br />

of animals with no difference in maintenance<br />

data across the two procedures. Efficiency<br />

data were inc<strong>on</strong>clusive with CTD more<br />

efficient in the number of sessi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> number<br />

of trials to criteri<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> simultaneous<br />

prompting more efficient in the number of<br />

errors <strong>and</strong> total training time to criteri<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Kurt <strong>and</strong> Tekin-Iftar (2008) compared the<br />

effects of simultaneous prompting <strong>and</strong> CTD<br />

in teaching the leisure skills of turning <strong>on</strong> a<br />

compact disc player <strong>and</strong> taking a digital picture<br />

to four boys with autism. Both procedures<br />

were equally effective in the acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

maintenance of the targeted skills. Efficiency<br />

538 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


data were inc<strong>on</strong>clusive with CTD more efficient<br />

for two students <strong>and</strong> simultaneous<br />

prompting more efficient for two students.<br />

Across the four studies that have compared<br />

simultaneous prompting to CTD, the data<br />

have showed minimal differences between the<br />

two strategies with both strategies dem<strong>on</strong>strating<br />

effectiveness in teaching discrete skills <strong>and</strong><br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strating mixed results in efficiency with<br />

simultaneous prompting more efficient for<br />

some students <strong>and</strong> CTD more efficient for<br />

some students.<br />

Antecedent-prompt <strong>and</strong> test procedure. Singlet<strong>on</strong><br />

et al. (1999) compared the effectiveness of<br />

simultaneous prompting <strong>and</strong> the antecedentprompt<br />

<strong>and</strong> test procedure in teaching four<br />

students with MoID to read grocery words.<br />

Both procedures were effective. However, efficiency<br />

data supported the antecedentprompt<br />

<strong>and</strong> test procedure over simultaneous<br />

prompting. The antecedent-prompt <strong>and</strong> test<br />

procedure required fewer sessi<strong>on</strong>s, less probe<br />

time, <strong>and</strong> resulted in fewer probe errors to<br />

criteri<strong>on</strong>. Despite the data supporting the antecedent-prompt<br />

<strong>and</strong> test procedure, maintenance<br />

data supported simultaneous prompting<br />

with students maintaining a higher<br />

percentage of words taught in the simultaneous<br />

prompting c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. These data indicate<br />

an important difference between simultaneous<br />

prompting <strong>and</strong> the antecedent-prompt<br />

<strong>and</strong> test procedure. During the antecedentprompt<br />

<strong>and</strong> test procedure probes are c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

following instructi<strong>on</strong> thereby indirectly<br />

measuring transfer of skills to shortterm<br />

memory. However, simultaneous<br />

prompting c<strong>on</strong>ducts probes prior to instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

each day measuring transfer of skills to<br />

l<strong>on</strong>g-term memory.<br />

Strengths <strong>and</strong> Weaknesses of Simultaneous<br />

Prompting<br />

Simultaneous prompting may provide certain<br />

advantages over other resp<strong>on</strong>se prompting<br />

strategies for various reas<strong>on</strong>s. First, simultaneous<br />

prompting does not require changes in<br />

teacher behavior as in CTD (Schuster et al.,<br />

1992), system of least prompts, most prompts,<br />

<strong>and</strong> graduated guidance. Each instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong> is completed in the same sequence<br />

until mastery is reached, decreasing the likelihood<br />

that teachers will emit procedural er-<br />

rors. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, simultaneous prompting does<br />

not require differential reinforcement because<br />

<strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e correct resp<strong>on</strong>se is reinforced<br />

(Schuster et al.). Third, unlike CTD in which<br />

students must exhibit a wait resp<strong>on</strong>se, simultaneous<br />

prompting eliminates the need for<br />

this resp<strong>on</strong>se (Schuster et al.). Simultaneous<br />

prompting also reduces the need to keep direct<br />

data during instructi<strong>on</strong>al sessi<strong>on</strong>s because<br />

transfer of stimulus c<strong>on</strong>trol is measured during<br />

probes. Avoiding the need to keep data<br />

during instructi<strong>on</strong>al sessi<strong>on</strong>s may be preferred<br />

by teachers when c<strong>on</strong>ducting group instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

because it eliminates the problems associated<br />

with keeping track of multiple students’<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses <strong>and</strong> maintaining student attenti<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> focus.<br />

Across 35 peer-reviewed studies, simultaneous<br />

prompting has an effectiveness rate of<br />

93%, with 126 out of 136 participants reaching<br />

criteri<strong>on</strong> during instructi<strong>on</strong> with simultaneous<br />

prompting. Ten participants across the<br />

35 studies did not reach criteri<strong>on</strong>. Rati<strong>on</strong>ale<br />

for not reaching criteri<strong>on</strong> is often noted as<br />

time c<strong>on</strong>straints associated with the end of the<br />

school year. Although the number of participants<br />

who did not reach criteri<strong>on</strong> is minimal<br />

<strong>and</strong> all students dem<strong>on</strong>strated an increase in<br />

performance over baseline, the literature does<br />

reveal some problems associated with simultaneous<br />

prompting. The goal of errorless learning<br />

procedures is to ensure that students do<br />

not have opportunities to make errors or practice<br />

incorrect resp<strong>on</strong>ses. While instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s attempt to c<strong>on</strong>trol the producti<strong>on</strong> of<br />

errors by providing a c<strong>on</strong>trolling prompt c<strong>on</strong>currently<br />

with the discriminative stimulus, errors<br />

can often be emitted during probe sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

when students have an opportunity to<br />

independently resp<strong>on</strong>d to the discriminative<br />

stimulus. As such, error rates vary greatly between<br />

daily probes (4–54% of trials) <strong>and</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s (0–5% of trials) (Morse &<br />

Schuster, 2004). A sec<strong>on</strong>d obstacle noted c<strong>on</strong>cerning<br />

simultaneous prompting is the need<br />

to c<strong>on</strong>duct probe sessi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> thereby impact<br />

efficiency (Schuster et al., 1992). Alternate<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se prompting strategies allow students<br />

to resp<strong>on</strong>d independently during instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

trials, however, in order for students to<br />

have an opportunity to resp<strong>on</strong>d independently<br />

to a stimulus during simultaneous<br />

prompting, a probe sessi<strong>on</strong> must be c<strong>on</strong>duct-<br />

Simultaneous Prompting / 539


ed; thereby adding to the amount of time<br />

required to fully employ the strategy. Despite<br />

the fact that probe time is often minimal, it is<br />

in additi<strong>on</strong> to instructi<strong>on</strong>al time.<br />

Future Research Areas<br />

There are currently four main areas for future<br />

research <strong>on</strong> simultaneous prompting. The<br />

first is to exp<strong>and</strong> the procedure to examine its<br />

effectiveness with individuals with profound<br />

intellectual disabilities (Morse & Schuster,<br />

2004). To date, no studies have investigated<br />

the effectiveness of the procedure with individuals<br />

with profound intellectual disabilities<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly a few studies have been c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

with individuals with severe intellectual disabilities.<br />

Sec<strong>on</strong>d, researchers have recommended<br />

that future investigati<strong>on</strong>s examine<br />

the effects of previous learning histories <strong>on</strong><br />

the effects of simultaneous prompting (Singlet<strong>on</strong><br />

et al., 1995). Does previous experience<br />

with errorless learning strategies impact acquisiti<strong>on</strong><br />

rates?<br />

The third <strong>and</strong> fourth recommendati<strong>on</strong>s are<br />

designed to examine methods for reducing<br />

the number of errors students emit during<br />

probe sessi<strong>on</strong>s in order to increase the degree<br />

of errorless learning associated with simultaneous<br />

prompting. The third recommendati<strong>on</strong><br />

is to provide error correcti<strong>on</strong> during daily/<br />

assessment probes (Birkan, 2005; Colozzi et<br />

al., 2008; Dogan & Tekin-Iftar, 2002; Fickel et<br />

al., 1998; Tekin-Iftar, 2003; Tekin-Iftar et al.,<br />

2003). Traditi<strong>on</strong>al procedures during daily/<br />

assessment probes are to provide verbal reinforcement<br />

for correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses <strong>and</strong> to ignore<br />

incorrect or no resp<strong>on</strong>ses. To date five studies<br />

have provided error correcti<strong>on</strong> during daily/<br />

assessment probes (Alberto, Waugh, &<br />

Fredrick, 2010; Johns<strong>on</strong> et al., 1996; Parker &<br />

Schuster, 2002; Tekin-Iftar, 2003; Waugh et<br />

al., 2009) <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>e study has directly compared<br />

the effects of traditi<strong>on</strong>al simultaneous<br />

prompting <strong>and</strong> simultaneous prompting with<br />

error correcti<strong>on</strong> during daily/assessment<br />

probes (Johns<strong>on</strong> et al., 1996). Johns<strong>on</strong> et al.<br />

c<strong>on</strong>ducted a direct comparis<strong>on</strong> of simultaneous<br />

prompting with error correcti<strong>on</strong> during<br />

daily probes <strong>and</strong> simultaneous prompting<br />

without error correcti<strong>on</strong> during daily/assessment<br />

probes in teaching science vocabulary<br />

words to five high school students with learn-<br />

ing disabilities <strong>and</strong> mild intellectual disabilities.<br />

Both c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s were effective for teaching<br />

science vocabulary. Compared to sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

in which no error correcti<strong>on</strong> was provided<br />

fewer sessi<strong>on</strong>s to criteri<strong>on</strong> were required <strong>and</strong><br />

fewer errors were emitted when error correcti<strong>on</strong><br />

was provided during daily/assessment<br />

probes. Social validity indicated that students<br />

preferred when they were provided with error<br />

correcti<strong>on</strong> during daily/assessment probes.<br />

Four other studies have included error correcti<strong>on</strong><br />

during daily/assessment probes but have<br />

not directly examined the impact of error correcti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

While simultaneous prompting with<br />

error correcti<strong>on</strong> may be more efficient in the<br />

acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of targeted stimuli, this procedural<br />

modificati<strong>on</strong> has been examined <strong>on</strong>ly with a<br />

limited number of participants <strong>and</strong> in a limited<br />

disability area. Further research should<br />

be c<strong>on</strong>ducted with individuals with various<br />

disabilities to determine if daily/assessment<br />

probes with error correcti<strong>on</strong> are more efficient<br />

than without error correcti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

The fourth recommendati<strong>on</strong> for future research<br />

is to provide intermittent probes versus<br />

daily/assessment probes (Birkan, 2005;<br />

Dogan & Tekin-Iftar, 2002; Fickel et al., 1998;<br />

Gibs<strong>on</strong> & Schuster, 1992; Griffen et al., 1998;<br />

Johns<strong>on</strong> et al., 1996; Maciag et al., 2000;<br />

Parker & Schuster; 2002; Tekin-Iftar et al.,<br />

2008; Wolery et al., 1993). Intermittent probes<br />

are probes c<strong>on</strong>ducted prior to every sec<strong>on</strong>d or<br />

third sessi<strong>on</strong> of instructi<strong>on</strong> instead of prior to<br />

each sessi<strong>on</strong>. By c<strong>on</strong>ducting probes prior to<br />

every sec<strong>on</strong>d or third sessi<strong>on</strong> of instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

students are allowed fewer opportunities to<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>d independently to the stimulus <strong>and</strong><br />

possibly make fewer errors. To date two studies<br />

have employed intermittent probes<br />

(Reichow & Wolery, 2009; Tekin-Iftar et al.,<br />

2008). Tekin-Iftar et al. employed intermittent<br />

probes to examine the effects of simultaneous<br />

prompting in teaching object identificati<strong>on</strong> to<br />

two students with intellectual disabilities (level<br />

of functi<strong>on</strong>ing not reported). Researchers<br />

c<strong>on</strong>ducted probes prior to every third instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>. Tekin-Iftar et al. report that<br />

employing intermittent probes did not reduce<br />

the number of errors emitted during probe<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s, although a direct comparis<strong>on</strong> was<br />

not made. Without a direct comparis<strong>on</strong>, it is<br />

unclear if these students would have produced<br />

lower error rates with intermittent versus daily<br />

540 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


probes. Reichow <strong>and</strong> Wolery recently c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

a direct comparis<strong>on</strong> of daily versus<br />

intermittent probes during simultaneous<br />

prompting. The researchers taught four preschool<br />

students to read vehicle transportati<strong>on</strong><br />

words (i.e., car, bus, truck, etc). The students<br />

included <strong>on</strong>e student with speech language<br />

impairment, <strong>on</strong>e student who was an English<br />

Language Learner, <strong>on</strong>e typically developing<br />

student, <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>e student identified as at-risk<br />

for school failure. Reichow <strong>and</strong> Wolery provided<br />

no error correcti<strong>on</strong> during probe sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

All four students reached mastery during<br />

intermittent probe c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s with three<br />

of the four students reaching mastery during<br />

the daily probe c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s. Efficiency data<br />

were mixed with the <strong>on</strong>e student who did not<br />

reach mastery in the daily probe c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

<strong>on</strong>e student who reached mastery in fewer<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s during intermittent probes, <strong>on</strong>e student<br />

who required the same number of sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

across both c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>e student<br />

who required fewer sessi<strong>on</strong>s during daily<br />

probe c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s. While the researchers did<br />

not report direct percentages of error rates<br />

across probe <strong>and</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>al sessi<strong>on</strong>s, they<br />

did provide initial data to support intermittent<br />

probes. During the first eight sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

during daily probes 50% of student trials resulted<br />

in errors versus the first two sessi<strong>on</strong>s of<br />

the intermittent probe c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> which resulted<br />

in errors in 28.1% of student trials.<br />

However, due to the limited number of participants<br />

<strong>and</strong> the lack of details c<strong>on</strong>cerning<br />

characteristics of the participants (i.e., IQ<br />

scores, etc), further research should be c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

to determine if intermittent probes<br />

produce more efficient student learning when<br />

employing simultaneous prompting.<br />

Simultaneous prompting is an errorless<br />

learning strategy with a research base to support<br />

its use to teach a variety of skills across<br />

various groups of ability levels. Despite the<br />

research base to support its usage, c<strong>on</strong>tinued<br />

research is needed to further examine alternatives<br />

to increase its efficiency <strong>and</strong> examine<br />

its usage with students with profound intellectual<br />

disabilities.<br />

References<br />

Akmanoglu, N., & Batu, S. (2004). Teaching pointing<br />

to numerals to individuals with autism using<br />

simultaneous prompting. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training<br />

in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 39, 326–336.<br />

Akmanoglu-Uludag, N., & Batu, S. (2005). Teaching<br />

naming relatives to individuals with autism using<br />

simultaneous prompting. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training<br />

in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 40, 401–410.<br />

Alberto, P. A., Waugh, R. E., & Fredrick, L. D.<br />

(2010). Teaching the reading of c<strong>on</strong>nected text<br />

through sight-word instructi<strong>on</strong> to students with<br />

moderate intellectual disabilities. Research in <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 31, 1<str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>7–1474.<br />

Batu, S. (2008). Caregiver-delivered home-based instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

using simultaneous prompting for<br />

teaching home skills to individuals with developmental<br />

disabilities. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 43, 541–555.<br />

Birkan, B. (2005). Using simultaneous prompting<br />

for teaching various discrete tasks to students with<br />

mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 40, 68–79.<br />

Colozzi, G. A., Ward, L. W., & Crotty, K. E. (2008).<br />

Comparis<strong>on</strong> of simultaneous prompting procedure<br />

in 1:1 <strong>and</strong> small group instructi<strong>on</strong> to teach<br />

play skills to preschool students with pervasive<br />

developmental disorder <strong>and</strong> developmental disabilities.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 43, 226–248.<br />

Cooper, J. O., Her<strong>on</strong>, T. E., & Heward, W. L.<br />

(2007). Applied behavior analysis.(2 nd ed.). Upper<br />

Saddle River, NJ: Merrill.<br />

Dogan, O. S., & Tekin-Iftar, E. (2002). The effects of<br />

simultaneous prompting <strong>on</strong> teaching receptively<br />

identifying occupati<strong>on</strong>s from picture cards. Research<br />

in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 23, 237–252.<br />

Fetko, K. S., Schuster, J. W., Harley, D. A., & Collins,<br />

B. C. (1999). Using simultaneous prompting to<br />

teach a chained vocati<strong>on</strong>al task to young adults<br />

with severe intellectual disabilities. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Training in Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 34, 318–329.<br />

Fickel, K. M., Schuster, J. W., & Collins, B. C. (1998).<br />

Teaching different tasks using different stimuli in<br />

a heterogeneous small group. Journal of Behavioral<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>, 8, 219–244.<br />

Foxx, R. (1981). Effective behavioral programming:<br />

Graduated guidance <strong>and</strong> backward chaining. Champaign,<br />

IL: Research Press.<br />

Foxx, R. M. (1982). Increasing behaviors of severely<br />

retarded <strong>and</strong> autistic pers<strong>on</strong>s. Champaign, IL: Research<br />

Press.<br />

Gibs<strong>on</strong>, A. N., & Schuster, J. W. (1992). The use of<br />

simultaneous prompting for teaching expressive<br />

word recogniti<strong>on</strong> to preschool children. Topics in<br />

Early Childhood Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 12, 247–267.<br />

Griffen, A. K., Schuster, J. W., & Morse, T. E. (1998).<br />

The acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of instructive feedback: A comparis<strong>on</strong><br />

of c<strong>on</strong>tinuous versus intermittent presentati<strong>on</strong><br />

schedules. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in Mental<br />

Simultaneous Prompting / 541


Retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 33, 42–<br />

61.<br />

Gursel, O., Tekin-Iftar, E., & Bozkurt, F. (2006).<br />

Effectiveness of simultaneous prompting in small<br />

group: The opportunity of acquiring n<strong>on</strong>target<br />

skills through observati<strong>on</strong>al learning <strong>and</strong> instructive<br />

feedback. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 41, 225–243.<br />

Johns<strong>on</strong>, P., Schuster, J., & Bell, J. K. (1996). Comparis<strong>on</strong><br />

of simultaneous prompting with <strong>and</strong> without<br />

error correcti<strong>on</strong> in teaching science vocabulary<br />

words to high school students with mild<br />

disabilities. Journal of Behavioral Educati<strong>on</strong>, 6, 437–<br />

458.<br />

Kurt, O., & Tekin-Iftar, E. (2008). A comparis<strong>on</strong> of<br />

c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay <strong>and</strong> simultaneous prompting<br />

within embedded instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> teaching leisure<br />

skills to children with autism. Topics in Early Childhood<br />

Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 28, 53–64.<br />

MacFarl<strong>and</strong>-Smith, J., Schuster, J. W., & Stevens,<br />

K. B. (1993). Using simultaneous prompting to<br />

teach expressive object identificati<strong>on</strong> to preschoolers<br />

with developmental delays. Journal of<br />

Early Interventi<strong>on</strong>, 17, 50–60.<br />

Maciag, K. G., Schuster, J. W., Collins, B. C., &<br />

Cooper, J. T. (2000). Training adults with moderate<br />

<strong>and</strong> severe mental retardati<strong>on</strong> in a vocati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

skill using simultaneous prompting procedure.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 35, 306-316.<br />

Mueller, M. M., Palkovic, C. M., & Maynard, C. S.<br />

(2007). Errorless learning: Review <strong>and</strong> practical<br />

applicati<strong>on</strong> for teaching children with pervasive<br />

developmental disabilities. Psychology in the Schools,<br />

44, 691–700.<br />

Morse, T. E., & Schuster, J. W. (2004). Simultaneous<br />

prompting: A review of the literature. Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 39, 153–<br />

168.<br />

Palmer, T., Collins, B. C., & Schuster, J. W. (1999).<br />

The use of simultaneous prompting procedure to<br />

teach receptive manual sign identificati<strong>on</strong> to<br />

adults with disabilities. Journal of <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Physical <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 11, 179–191.<br />

Parker, M. A., & Schuster, J. W. (2002). Effectiveness<br />

of simultaneous prompting <strong>on</strong> the acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of<br />

observati<strong>on</strong>al <strong>and</strong> instructive feedback stimuli<br />

when teaching a heterogeneous group of high<br />

school students. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in Mental<br />

Retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 37, 89–<br />

104.<br />

Parrott, K. A., Schuster, J. W., Collins, B. C., &<br />

Gassaway, L. J. (2000). Simultaneous prompting<br />

<strong>and</strong> instructive feedback when teaching chained<br />

tasks. Journal of Behavioral Educati<strong>on</strong>, 10, 3–19.<br />

Rao, S., & Kane, M. T. (2009). Teaching students<br />

with cognitive impairment a chained mathematical<br />

task of decimal subtracti<strong>on</strong> using simultane-<br />

ous prompting. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 44, 244–256.<br />

Rao, S., & Mallow, L. (2009). Using simultaneous<br />

prompting procedure to promote recall of multiplicati<strong>on</strong><br />

facts by middle school students with<br />

cognitive impairment. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in<br />

<strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 44, 80–90.<br />

Reichow, B., & Wolery, M. (2009). Comparis<strong>on</strong> of<br />

everyday <strong>and</strong> every-fourth-day probe sessi<strong>on</strong>s with<br />

the simultaneous prompting procedure. Topics in<br />

Early Childhood Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 29(2), 79–89.<br />

Riesen, T., McD<strong>on</strong>nell, J., Johns<strong>on</strong>, J. W., Polychr<strong>on</strong>is,<br />

S., & James<strong>on</strong>, M. (2003). A comparis<strong>on</strong> of<br />

c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay <strong>and</strong> simultaneous prompting<br />

within embedded instructi<strong>on</strong> in general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

classes with students with moderate to severe<br />

disabilities. Journal of Behavioral Educati<strong>on</strong>, 12,<br />

241–259.<br />

Schuster, J. W., & Griffen, A. K. (1993). Teaching a<br />

chained task with a simultaneous prompting procedure.<br />

Journal of Behavioral Educati<strong>on</strong>, 3, 299–<br />

315.<br />

Schuster, J. W., Griffen, A. K., & Wolery, M. (1992).<br />

Comparis<strong>on</strong> of simultaneous prompting <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>stant<br />

time delay procedures in teaching sight<br />

words to elementary students with moderate mental<br />

retardati<strong>on</strong>. Journal of Behavioral Educati<strong>on</strong>, 2,<br />

305–325.<br />

Sewell, T. J., Collins, B. C., Hemmeter, M. L., &<br />

Schuster, J. W. (1998). Using simultaneous<br />

prompting within an activity-based format to<br />

teach dressing skills to preschoolers with developmental<br />

delays. Journal of Early Interventi<strong>on</strong>, 21,<br />

132–145.<br />

Singlet<strong>on</strong>, K. C., Schuster, J. W., & Ault, M. J.<br />

(1995). Simultaneous prompting in a small group<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>al arrangement. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training<br />

in Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>,<br />

30, 218–230.<br />

Singlet<strong>on</strong>, D. K., Schuster, J. W., Morse, T. E., &<br />

Collins, B. C. (1999). A comparis<strong>on</strong> of antecedent<br />

prompt <strong>and</strong> test <strong>and</strong> simultaneous prompting<br />

procedures in teaching grocery words to adolescents<br />

with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Training in Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 34, 182–199.<br />

Snell, M. E., & Gast, D. L. (1981). Applying time<br />

delay procedure to the instructi<strong>on</strong> of severely<br />

h<strong>and</strong>icapped. Journal of the Associati<strong>on</strong> of the Severely<br />

H<strong>and</strong>icapped, 6(3), 6–14.<br />

Tekin, E., & Kircaali-Iftar, G. (2002). Comparis<strong>on</strong> of<br />

the effectiveness <strong>and</strong> efficiency of two resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

prompting procedures delivered by sibling tutors.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 37, 283–299.<br />

Tekin-Iftar, E. (2003). Effectiveness of peer delivered<br />

simultaneous prompting <strong>on</strong> teaching community<br />

signs to students with developmental dis-<br />

542 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


abilities. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 38, 77–94.<br />

Tekin-Iftar, E. (2008). Parent-delivered communitybased<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> with simultaneous prompting<br />

for teaching community skills to children with<br />

developmental disabilities. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training<br />

in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 43, 249–265.<br />

Tekin-Iftar, E., Acar, G., & Kurt, O. (2003). The<br />

effects of simultaneous prompting <strong>on</strong> teaching<br />

expressive identificati<strong>on</strong> of objects: An instructive<br />

feedback study. Internati<strong>on</strong>al Journal of <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

Disability <strong>and</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong>, 50, 149–167.<br />

Tekin-Iftar, E., Kurt, O., & Acar, G. (2008). Enhancing<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>al efficiency through generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> instructive feedback: A single-subject<br />

study with children with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. Internati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Journal of Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 23, 147–158.<br />

Touchette, P. (1971). Transfer of stimulus c<strong>on</strong>trol:<br />

Measuring the moment of transfer. Journal of Experimental<br />

Analysis of Behavior, 15, 347–354.<br />

Waugh, R. E., Fredrick, L. D., & Alberto, P. A.<br />

(2009). Using simultaneous prompting to teach<br />

sounds <strong>and</strong> blending skills to students with moderate<br />

intellectual disabilities. Research in <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 30, 1435–1447.<br />

Wolery, M., Ault, M. J., & Doyle, P. M. (1992).<br />

Teaching students with moderate to severe disabilities:<br />

Use of resp<strong>on</strong>se prompting strategies. New York, NY:<br />

L<strong>on</strong>gman.<br />

Wolery, M., & Gast, D. L. (1984). Effective <strong>and</strong><br />

efficient procedures for the transfer of stimulus<br />

c<strong>on</strong>trol. Topics in Early Childhood Special Educati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

4, 52–77.<br />

Wolery, M., Holcombe, A., Werts, M. G., & Cipoll<strong>on</strong>i,<br />

R. M. (1993). Effects of simultaneous<br />

prompting <strong>and</strong> instructive feedback. Early Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Development, 4, 20–31.<br />

Received: 27 July 2010<br />

Initial Acceptance: 25 September 2010<br />

Final Acceptance: 16 December 2010<br />

Simultaneous Prompting / 543


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 2011, <str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>(4), 544–555<br />

© <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Teaching M<strong>on</strong>ey Computati<strong>on</strong> Skills to High School Students<br />

with Mild Intellectual <strong>Disabilities</strong> via the TouchMath©<br />

Program: A Multi-Sensory Approach<br />

Hugh E. Waters <strong>and</strong> Richard T. Bo<strong>on</strong><br />

The University of Georgia<br />

Abstract: This study investigated the effects of the TouchMath© program (Bullock, Pierce, & McClellan, 1989)<br />

to teach students with mild intellectual disabilities to subtract 3-digit m<strong>on</strong>ey computati<strong>on</strong>al problems with<br />

regrouping. Three students with mild intellectual disabilities in high school received instructi<strong>on</strong> in a special<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> mathematics self-c<strong>on</strong>tained classroom. A multiple-probe across participants design (Alberto &<br />

Troutman, 2009) was used to evaluate the effectiveness of the TouchMath© program using the “touch-points”<br />

strategy to facilitate the student’s mathematics performance. The results revealed the TouchMath© program<br />

improved all three of the students’ ability to subtract 3-digit mathematics operati<strong>on</strong>s using m<strong>on</strong>ey applicati<strong>on</strong>s;<br />

however, maintenance results were mixed, as the students exhibited difficulty with maintaining the necessary<br />

skills <strong>on</strong>ce the interventi<strong>on</strong> was withdrawn. Limitati<strong>on</strong>s, recommendati<strong>on</strong>s for classroom teachers <strong>and</strong> future<br />

research directi<strong>on</strong>s are presented.<br />

With the growing trend of providing proven,<br />

scientifically-validated practices in the classroom,<br />

teaching students’ mathematics instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

at the sec<strong>on</strong>dary-level, especially those<br />

with intellectual disabilities, can be a challenging<br />

<strong>and</strong> often overwhelming task. With recent<br />

m<strong>and</strong>ates from federal policies like, the No<br />

Child Left Behind Act of 2001 (NCLB, 2001)<br />

<strong>and</strong> the Individuals with <strong>Disabilities</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

Improvement Act (IDEA, 2004), schools<br />

are now m<strong>and</strong>ated that all students have equal<br />

access to the general educati<strong>on</strong> academic curriculum<br />

<strong>and</strong> nati<strong>on</strong>al <strong>and</strong> state st<strong>and</strong>ards (Nati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Council of Teachers of Mathematics,<br />

2000; U. S. Department of Educati<strong>on</strong>, 2007).<br />

Thus, an increasing number of students with<br />

mild intellectual disabilities are now receiving<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> being placed into general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

classrooms where they are not <strong>on</strong>ly<br />

expected to succeed in the classroom, but also<br />

<strong>on</strong> high-stakes assessments such as the Georgia<br />

End of Course Test (EOCT) <strong>and</strong> the Geor-<br />

Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence c<strong>on</strong>cerning this article should<br />

be addressed to Richard T. Bo<strong>on</strong>, The University of<br />

Georgia, Department of Communicati<strong>on</strong> Sciences<br />

& Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 557 Aderhold Hall, Athens,<br />

GA 30602-7153. E-mail: rbo<strong>on</strong>@uga.edu<br />

gia High School Graduati<strong>on</strong> Test (GHSGT).<br />

These new classroom rigors include moving<br />

away from functi<strong>on</strong>al academics <strong>and</strong> replacing<br />

them with more traditi<strong>on</strong>al academic skills.<br />

While this is a positive move toward allowing<br />

all students regardless of their disability the<br />

opportunity to graduate with a high school<br />

diploma <strong>and</strong> prepare them to attend further<br />

educati<strong>on</strong>al <strong>and</strong> occupati<strong>on</strong>al prospects, this<br />

does not allow students with mild intellectual<br />

disabilities the chance to learn the essential<br />

life skills (e.g., m<strong>on</strong>ey computati<strong>on</strong>) that are<br />

crucial for their survival in the community as<br />

independent members of society.<br />

Students with mild intellectual disabilities<br />

often exhibit deficits in basic mathematics instructi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

especially in the area of m<strong>on</strong>ey computati<strong>on</strong><br />

(e.g., purchasing skills), which has<br />

been well documented (Browder & Grasso,<br />

1999; Browder, Spo<strong>on</strong>er, Ahlgrim-Delzell,<br />

Harris, & Wakeman, 2008; Butler, Miller, Kithung,<br />

& Pierce, 2001; Jitendra & Xin, 1997;<br />

Kroesbergen & Van Luit, 2003; Mastropieri,<br />

Bakken, & Scruggs, 1991; Miller, Butler, &<br />

Lee, 1998; Swans<strong>on</strong> & Jerman, 2006, Xin &<br />

Jitendra, 1999). In 2000, the Nati<strong>on</strong>al Council<br />

of Teachers of Mathematics (NCTM, 2000) stated<br />

in a comprehensive report five main comp<strong>on</strong>ents<br />

of mathematics instructi<strong>on</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ards<br />

544 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


that all students are required to achieve, <strong>on</strong>e<br />

of which focuses explicitly <strong>on</strong> the ability to<br />

measure attributes of objects such as time <strong>and</strong><br />

m<strong>on</strong>ey applicati<strong>on</strong>s, which is comm<strong>on</strong>ly problematic<br />

for a large number of students with<br />

mild intellectual disabilities. Fortunately, a<br />

growing research-base of new <strong>and</strong> innovative<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>s, like the TouchMath© program,<br />

has been developing in the literature <strong>and</strong> has<br />

shown some promising results to be effective<br />

in increasing students with <strong>and</strong> without disabilities<br />

mathematics performance.<br />

The TouchMath© program (Bullock et al.,<br />

1989), a multi-sensory “dot-notati<strong>on</strong>” system,<br />

previously employed by Kramer <strong>and</strong> Krug<br />

(1973) was used to teach mathematics skills to<br />

students with disabilities. The TouchMath©<br />

program uses “dot-notati<strong>on</strong>s” often referred<br />

to as “touch-points” either with <strong>on</strong>e dot, for<br />

numbers 1 to 5, or a dot-notati<strong>on</strong> with a circle<br />

around them, to indicate two or double touchpoints<br />

to assist students with <strong>and</strong> without disabilities<br />

with basic counting <strong>and</strong> computati<strong>on</strong><br />

skills. The TouchMath© program using the<br />

touch-points strategy, has been shown in previous<br />

research to be effective for students with<br />

mathematical disabilities in basic mathematics<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> (e.g., adding single, double-digit<br />

mathematics problems with <strong>and</strong> without regrouping)<br />

at the elementary level for students<br />

with specific learning disabilities <strong>and</strong> moderate<br />

intellectual disabilities (Scott, 1993; Sim<strong>on</strong><br />

& Hanrahan, 2004), autism spectrum disorders<br />

(Cihak & Foust, 2008), <strong>and</strong> more recently,<br />

at the middle school level including<br />

students with autism spectrum disorders <strong>and</strong><br />

moderate intellectual disabilities (Fletcher,<br />

Bo<strong>on</strong>, & Cihak, 2010). However, no studies to<br />

date have attempted to explore the effectiveness<br />

of the TouchMath© program, using<br />

m<strong>on</strong>ey computati<strong>on</strong> skills, with students with<br />

mild intellectual disabilities in a high school<br />

classroom setting.<br />

Therefore, the purpose of this study was to<br />

examine the effects of the TouchMath© program<br />

<strong>on</strong> the acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of subtracting 3-digit<br />

m<strong>on</strong>ey computati<strong>on</strong>al problems with regrouping<br />

for three students with mild intellectual<br />

disabilities in a high school special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

self-c<strong>on</strong>tained classroom. Prior research <strong>on</strong><br />

the efficacy of the TouchMath© program has<br />

focused <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong> students with specific learning<br />

disabilities, moderate intellectual disabilities,<br />

<strong>and</strong> autism spectrum disorders at the elementary<br />

<strong>and</strong> middle school grade levels, <strong>and</strong> has<br />

not addressed the benefits of such a strategy<br />

for students with mild intellectual disabilities<br />

at the high school level. Although previous<br />

studies have investigated the use of the touchpoints<br />

strategy to teach basic additi<strong>on</strong> using<br />

single <strong>and</strong> double-digit mathematics problems;<br />

no studies have explored the benefits of<br />

the touch-points strategy <strong>on</strong> m<strong>on</strong>ey applicati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Research Questi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

Thus, the two main research questi<strong>on</strong>s posed<br />

were: (a) What are the effects of the Touch-<br />

Math© program <strong>on</strong> the mathematics performance<br />

of solving subtracti<strong>on</strong> 3-digit m<strong>on</strong>ey<br />

computati<strong>on</strong>al problems with regrouping for<br />

students with mild intellectual disabilities at<br />

the high school grade level? And (b) What are<br />

the students, teachers, <strong>and</strong> parents percepti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

of the TouchMath© program to improving<br />

students with mild intellectual disabilities<br />

mathematics performance?<br />

Method<br />

Participants<br />

Three students with mild intellectual disabilities,<br />

two of which had a dual-diagnosis of autism<br />

as well, from the same special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

high school self-c<strong>on</strong>tained classroom participated<br />

in the study. The students’ ages ranged<br />

from 14 to 16 years-old, with a mean of 14.75<br />

<strong>and</strong> intellectual quotients (IQ) scores varied<br />

from 61 to 64, with a mean of 63. All of the<br />

students were classified with a disability based<br />

<strong>on</strong> the county, state, <strong>and</strong> federal criteria,<br />

which indicated having below average intellectual<br />

ability, deficits in adaptive behavior<br />

scores, which both negatively affected their<br />

academic performance. Demographic <strong>and</strong> educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

informati<strong>on</strong> is depicted in Table 1.<br />

All of the students received special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

services since entering high school where they<br />

were in a self-c<strong>on</strong>tained special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

classroom setting for three block periods a day<br />

<strong>and</strong> participated in <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

course elective. The students were taught<br />

all of their academic subjects including mathematics<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> in the same self-c<strong>on</strong>tained<br />

Touchmath© <strong>and</strong> M<strong>on</strong>ey Skills / 545


TABLE 1<br />

Student Demographic Informati<strong>on</strong><br />

classroom from the same teacher for all three<br />

block periods. Finally, all three students<br />

scored well-below grade level in mathematics,<br />

based <strong>on</strong> the Woodcock-McGrew-Werder Mini-Battery<br />

of Achievement (MBA; 1994) test results.<br />

Students were selected based <strong>on</strong> their gradelevel,<br />

special educati<strong>on</strong> classificati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

mathematical ability. All of the students were<br />

unable to properly <strong>and</strong> accurately subtract numerical<br />

or m<strong>on</strong>etary values without a calculator.<br />

The classroom instructor had previously<br />

taught the students to use calculators to determine<br />

purchase price in order to facilitate accuracy<br />

<strong>and</strong> fluency in the classroom <strong>and</strong> community-based<br />

setting.<br />

Trent. Trent was a ninth grader <strong>and</strong> was<br />

15 years, 10 m<strong>on</strong>ths old at the outset of the<br />

study. Trent had received special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

services for ten years for a mild intellectual<br />

disability <strong>and</strong> autism. Placement was supported<br />

with a Full Scale IQ score of 64 from<br />

the WISC-III (Wechsler, 1991) <strong>and</strong> the ABAS-II<br />

(Harris<strong>on</strong> & Oakl<strong>and</strong>, 2003), with a 72 c<strong>on</strong>ceptual<br />

score; 75 social score; 91 practical<br />

score, <strong>and</strong> a general adaptive composite score<br />

of 83. Trent’s IEP (Individualized Educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Plan) goals covered several academic <strong>and</strong> life<br />

skill areas, as he had a mathematics academic<br />

goal of becoming more proficient in basic<br />

mathematics skills. His teacher said that he is<br />

Trent Michael Alex<br />

Chr<strong>on</strong>ological Age 15–0 14–11 16–1<br />

Grade 9 th<br />

9 th<br />

10 th<br />

Sex Male Male Male<br />

IQ* 64 61 64<br />

Adaptive Behavior<br />

Score Composite**<br />

83 54 71<br />

Math Composite***<br />

(Grade Equivalent)<br />

5.1 4.2 2.8<br />

Primary Eligibility Mild Intellectual Mild Intellectual<br />

Mild Intellectual<br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>/<strong>Autism</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>/Asperger’s Syndrome<br />

* WISC-III COG Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (3 rd ed.) by D. Wechsler. Copyright 1991 by Psychological<br />

Corp, San Ant<strong>on</strong>io, TX.<br />

** ABAS-II Adaptive Behavior Assessment System (2 nd ed.) by P. Harris<strong>on</strong> & T. Oakl<strong>and</strong>. Copyright 2003 by<br />

Psychological Corp, San Ant<strong>on</strong>io, TX.<br />

*** MBA Mini-Battery of Achievement by R. Woodcock, K. McGrew, & J. Werder. Copyright 1994 by Riverside<br />

Publishing, Chicago, IL.<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sistently willing to work hard to complete<br />

his assignments <strong>and</strong> complies with directi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

from the classroom teacher <strong>and</strong> paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>als.<br />

Michael. Michael entered his first year of<br />

high school as a ninth grader during the study<br />

at 14 years, 11 m<strong>on</strong>ths old <strong>and</strong> turned 15<br />

within the study’s span. He struggles with all<br />

academic subjects as evident by his instructors’<br />

observati<strong>on</strong>s during the daily educati<strong>on</strong><br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> receives <strong>on</strong>e to four instructi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

daily for his academics. Michael has received<br />

special educati<strong>on</strong> services since his entrance<br />

into the school system in self-c<strong>on</strong>tained classrooms<br />

for students with mild intellectual disabilities.<br />

According to the WISC-III (Wechsler,<br />

1991) Michael had a Full Scale IQ score of 61,<br />

with a 53 c<strong>on</strong>ceptual score; 70 social score; 53<br />

practical score, <strong>and</strong> a general adaptive composite<br />

score of 54. The results from the<br />

ABAS-II (Harris<strong>on</strong> & Oakl<strong>and</strong>, 2003) instrument<br />

determined Michael lacked adaptive behavior<br />

skills <strong>and</strong> met criteri<strong>on</strong> for classificati<strong>on</strong><br />

for a mild intellectual disability.<br />

Increasing basic mathematics skills was <strong>on</strong>e of<br />

his academic (IEP) goals. His teacher stated<br />

that he is cooperative during instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

puts forth much effort towards his classroom<br />

work.<br />

5<str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g> / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


Alex. Alex was a tenth grade student being<br />

served in the same self-c<strong>on</strong>tained special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

classroom as Trent <strong>and</strong> Michael. He<br />

was 16 years, 1 m<strong>on</strong>th at the beginning of the<br />

study <strong>and</strong> has received special educati<strong>on</strong> services<br />

for thirteen years with an eligibility of a<br />

mild intellectual disability <strong>and</strong> Asperger’s Syndrome.<br />

Alex’s placement was determined by a<br />

WISC-III (Wechsler, 1991) Full Scale IQ score<br />

of 64 <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong> the ABAS-II (Harris<strong>on</strong> & Oakl<strong>and</strong>,<br />

2003) he had a 70 c<strong>on</strong>ceptual score, 77<br />

social score, 75 practical score, <strong>and</strong> a general<br />

adaptive composite score of 71. He had two<br />

m<strong>on</strong>ey mathematics skills goals <strong>on</strong> his (IEP),<br />

which included adding <strong>and</strong> subtracting two<br />

<strong>and</strong> three-digit mathematics problems without<br />

a calculator <strong>and</strong> to write checks, make deposits,<br />

<strong>and</strong> balance a checkbook. He had dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

a lack of restraint <strong>and</strong> cooperati<strong>on</strong><br />

with teachers in the past, but has not shown<br />

these behaviors since entering high school, as<br />

his teachers have stated that he has been very<br />

obliging <strong>and</strong> receptive to instructi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Setting <strong>and</strong> Arrangements<br />

The public high school c<strong>on</strong>sisted of approximately<br />

1,500 students, with grades nine<br />

through twelfth, <strong>and</strong> was located in a southeastern<br />

regi<strong>on</strong> of the United States. The<br />

county school system populati<strong>on</strong> was c<strong>on</strong>sidered<br />

low-income, with a low socio-ec<strong>on</strong>omic<br />

status (SES) with manufacturing as the major<br />

employer in the area. Data collecti<strong>on</strong>, training,<br />

<strong>and</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong> procedures were c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

in that same self-c<strong>on</strong>tained special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

mathematics classroom. The<br />

classroom dimensi<strong>on</strong>s were 3 m 6.5 m <strong>and</strong><br />

the room c<strong>on</strong>sisted of 12 student desks <strong>and</strong><br />

two teacher desks. The teacher desks faced the<br />

student desks located to the side of the student<br />

desks <strong>and</strong> immediately in fr<strong>on</strong>t of <strong>on</strong>e<br />

teacher desk, was a podium. The students<br />

were instructed at a distance of 1 meter, facing<br />

the teacher, <strong>and</strong> two student desks were directly<br />

in fr<strong>on</strong>t of the teacher. The TouchMath©<br />

poster displaying the touch-points for the<br />

numbers 1 to 9 was placed <strong>on</strong> the wall between<br />

the student <strong>and</strong> teacher desks as a reminder<br />

<strong>and</strong> visual cue during the training <strong>and</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

phases. No other students were in the<br />

classroom during the block period <strong>and</strong> all<br />

phases of the study.<br />

Materials<br />

The TouchMath© program (Bullock et al.,<br />

1989) was the interventi<strong>on</strong> utilized during the<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> phase to teach students to subtract<br />

3-digit m<strong>on</strong>ey computati<strong>on</strong>al problems<br />

with regrouping. The researcher <strong>and</strong> classroom<br />

teacher collected the data for all phases<br />

of the study. The researcher <strong>and</strong> classroom<br />

teacher were trained to use the TouchMath©<br />

program via the teacher training DVD <strong>and</strong><br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>al materials, that were sent by the<br />

publisher. The TouchMath© system is based <strong>on</strong><br />

the placement of dots (e.g., dot-notati<strong>on</strong>s) <strong>on</strong><br />

numbers (1 to 9). For example, the student<br />

would be asked to state the number aloud<br />

then the student was expected to count aloud<br />

as he made c<strong>on</strong>tact <strong>on</strong> the touch-points; however,<br />

for subtracti<strong>on</strong> problems, the students<br />

must be able to count backwards from 20.<br />

When regrouping, the students were expected<br />

to be able to mark through the number borrowed<br />

from <strong>and</strong> then place a 1 next to the<br />

previous number <strong>and</strong> subtract the numbers.<br />

TouchMath© made a point of ensuring the<br />

number borrowed was the same size as the<br />

other digits. Worksheets were provided by the<br />

publisher that the researcher <strong>and</strong> classroom<br />

teacher utilized to introduce, instruct, practice,<br />

<strong>and</strong> assess all of the students. The worksheets<br />

were designed based <strong>on</strong> the specific<br />

steps previously menti<strong>on</strong>ed above <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sisted<br />

of examples <strong>on</strong> how to count forward<br />

<strong>and</strong> add with <strong>and</strong> without regrouping. A<br />

poster with the touch-points for each of the<br />

numbers 1 to 9 was posted <strong>on</strong> the wall in the<br />

classroom. In additi<strong>on</strong>, mini-posters were provided<br />

to the students <strong>and</strong> laminated <strong>on</strong> their<br />

desktop as a reference, while learning the<br />

touch-points strategy (see Figure 1 for an example<br />

of the mini-posters).<br />

Assessment Materials<br />

Researcher developed worksheets with the<br />

same f<strong>on</strong>t <strong>and</strong> size as the publisher’s were<br />

employed as the probe during the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> maintenance phases (see Figure 2 for<br />

an example of the worksheets). The measures<br />

served as permanent products to collect data.<br />

These worksheets c<strong>on</strong>sisted of 10 subtracti<strong>on</strong><br />

3-digit m<strong>on</strong>ey computati<strong>on</strong>al problems with<br />

regrouping. All probes c<strong>on</strong>sisted of different<br />

Touchmath© <strong>and</strong> M<strong>on</strong>ey Skills / 547


Figure 1. Example of the mini-poster that was provided<br />

to the students <strong>and</strong> laminated <strong>on</strong><br />

their desktop as a reference, while learning<br />

the touch-points strategy. Touch-<br />

Math® TouchPoints. By permissi<strong>on</strong> of J.<br />

Bullock <strong>and</strong> Innovative Learning C<strong>on</strong>cepts<br />

Inc., Colorado Springs, CO. All<br />

rights reserved.<br />

mathematics problems so the students would<br />

not be able to memorize the answers.<br />

Procedure<br />

General procedure. All instructi<strong>on</strong>, training<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s, observati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> probes occurred<br />

during the regular school day during the first<br />

block period from 8:30–10:00 a.m. There<br />

were five sessi<strong>on</strong>s per week for ten weeks <strong>and</strong><br />

each student received instructi<strong>on</strong> in the selfc<strong>on</strong>tained<br />

special educati<strong>on</strong> classroom. The<br />

training <strong>and</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s lasted in<br />

durati<strong>on</strong> from 10 to 15 minutes <strong>on</strong> the Touch-<br />

Math© procedures to subtract numbers with<br />

regrouping <strong>and</strong> probes were designed to take<br />

no l<strong>on</strong>ger than 10 to 15 minutes. Maintenance<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s extended l<strong>on</strong>g enough to complete<br />

the probe (10 to 15 minutes). These sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

were held c<strong>on</strong>currently with the last three interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s of subsequent student’s<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s. The interventi<strong>on</strong> was introduced to<br />

subsequent students based <strong>on</strong> the student<br />

reaching criteri<strong>on</strong>, which was established as<br />

the students’ average score increase to be<br />

above 40% of the average baseline score for<br />

80% of the sessi<strong>on</strong>s. Baseline stability followed<br />

the 80/30 guideline to establish a trend before<br />

the interventi<strong>on</strong> was implemented.<br />

Interventi<strong>on</strong> began with identifying each of<br />

the numbers (1 to 9) <strong>and</strong> where the touchpoints<br />

were located <strong>on</strong> the numbers. Next, the<br />

students were taught how to count the touchpoints<br />

in a certain order, as described in the<br />

publisher’s manual. According to the Touch-<br />

Math© procedures, the students are to count<br />

aloud during instructi<strong>on</strong>, while learning the<br />

touch-points <strong>on</strong> each of the numbers. In additi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

counting backwards was also taught<br />

<strong>and</strong> practiced while utilizing the touch-points<br />

strategy. Once these skills had been mastered,<br />

Figure 2. Example of the subtracti<strong>on</strong> 3-digit m<strong>on</strong>ey computati<strong>on</strong>al problems with regrouping worksheet used<br />

during the interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> maintenance phases.<br />

548 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


the students could move <strong>on</strong> to actual subtracti<strong>on</strong><br />

problems with regrouping. With the subtracti<strong>on</strong><br />

problems, the students were taught<br />

to: (a) state the problem aloud, (b) state the<br />

first number aloud, (c) count backwards using<br />

the touch-points <strong>on</strong> the sec<strong>on</strong>d number (if<br />

the student reaches 0 before finishing counting<br />

the bottom number, then regroup), (d)<br />

mark-out the number borrowed from, write<br />

lowered number above <strong>on</strong> the line, (e) place a<br />

1 next to the number <strong>on</strong> the right making<br />

sure it is the same size, (f) count backwards<br />

using the touch-points <strong>on</strong> the sec<strong>on</strong>d number,<br />

(g) place the difference in the answer blank,<br />

<strong>and</strong> finally (h) repeat the problem aloud with<br />

the answer.<br />

Experimental Procedures<br />

Baseline. Before baseline probes were delivered,<br />

prerequisite skills were taught until<br />

100% of the students’ mastery was achieved.<br />

The student had to be able to learn to count<br />

backwards, place the touch-points <strong>on</strong> the<br />

numbers 1 to 9, <strong>and</strong> count those touch-points<br />

in a proper pattern. The first student was administered<br />

a minimum of three probes to establish<br />

trend stability, which c<strong>on</strong>sisted of 10<br />

subtracti<strong>on</strong> 3-digit m<strong>on</strong>ey computati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

problems with regrouping without the use of<br />

the touch-points strategy. Once stability was<br />

established, the touch-points interventi<strong>on</strong> began.<br />

Subsequent students were probed c<strong>on</strong>currently<br />

with the last three interventi<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

of the previous student. Verbal cues <strong>and</strong><br />

praise were offered for correct <strong>and</strong>/or incorrect<br />

behaviors.<br />

Interventi<strong>on</strong>. First, the TouchMath© strategy<br />

was introduced to each of the students, which<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sisted of the instructor modeling how to<br />

count the “dot-notati<strong>on</strong>s” <strong>on</strong> each of the numbers<br />

1 to 9 to solve a subtracti<strong>on</strong> problem. The<br />

students were then given an opportunity to<br />

practice <strong>on</strong>e problem al<strong>on</strong>g with the special<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> teacher using the touch-points<br />

strategy. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, the teacher dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

the proper steps <strong>and</strong> verbal cues to solve a<br />

subtracti<strong>on</strong> problem. Afterwards, the student<br />

was asked to carry out the task independently<br />

as performed by the teacher. During the problem-solving<br />

procedures, the instructor provided<br />

positive verbal corrective feedback to<br />

redirect any operati<strong>on</strong>al errors performed by<br />

the student. The student then practiced the<br />

steps a minimum of five times. Third, the<br />

instructor modeled the proper steps of the<br />

TouchMath© program <strong>and</strong> verbal cues to solve<br />

a subtracti<strong>on</strong> problem. The student was then<br />

asked to perform the task as modeled. During<br />

the problem-solving procedures, the instructor<br />

provided positive verbal corrective feedback<br />

to redirect any operati<strong>on</strong>al errors performed<br />

by the student for the first two<br />

practice problems <strong>and</strong> then was asked to solve<br />

a minimum of five problems independently.<br />

And finally, in the fourth step, the instructor<br />

modeled the proper steps <strong>and</strong> verbal cues to<br />

solve a subtracti<strong>on</strong> problem. The student was<br />

then expected to solve 10 3-digit m<strong>on</strong>ey subtracti<strong>on</strong><br />

computati<strong>on</strong>al problems with regrouping<br />

using the touch-points interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

independently.<br />

Maintenance. During the maintenance<br />

phase, the students were provided no instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

or visual cues from the TouchMath© materials<br />

to perform the operati<strong>on</strong>al steps to subtract<br />

a 3-digit m<strong>on</strong>ey computati<strong>on</strong>al problem<br />

with regrouping. After the student had<br />

reached criteria for three c<strong>on</strong>secutive days, a<br />

minimum of two sessi<strong>on</strong>s without instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

lapsed before a maintenance probe was given.<br />

These probes c<strong>on</strong>sisted of 10 3-digit subtracti<strong>on</strong><br />

problems with regrouping following the<br />

same format as those menti<strong>on</strong>ed in the baseline<br />

<strong>and</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s. C<strong>on</strong>current<br />

with subsequent students being presented<br />

their last two interventi<strong>on</strong> probes, each previous<br />

student was given a minimum of <strong>on</strong>e<br />

maintenance probe every five days until the<br />

c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> of the study with the last student.<br />

These probes indicated whether the touchpoint<br />

system could be maintained for other<br />

problem sets. Generalizati<strong>on</strong> was m<strong>on</strong>itored<br />

throughout the study with subtracti<strong>on</strong> problems<br />

at the end of each probe. These problems<br />

also accompanied maintenance sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

These three problems, c<strong>on</strong>sisting of the same<br />

skills addressed during the interventi<strong>on</strong> phase<br />

were presented to the students from different<br />

stimuli, workbooks <strong>and</strong> instructor made worksheets.<br />

This measure determined if the students<br />

could generalize TouchMath© techniques<br />

<strong>and</strong> procedures to the same math<br />

behaviors from different stimuli.<br />

Touchmath© <strong>and</strong> M<strong>on</strong>ey Skills / 549


Experimental Design<br />

This study employed a multiple-probe across<br />

participants design (Alberto & Troutman,<br />

2009) to examine the effectiveness of the<br />

touch-points strategy to teach students with<br />

mild intellectual disabilities to subtract 3-digit<br />

m<strong>on</strong>ey computati<strong>on</strong>al problems with regrouping.<br />

Reliability<br />

Inter-observer agreement. Inter-observer reliability<br />

data was collected across all c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

using a point-by-point agreement formula.<br />

The special educati<strong>on</strong> teacher’s paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

was a sec<strong>on</strong>d observer <strong>and</strong> was asked to<br />

independently score the probes <strong>and</strong> evaluate<br />

the procedural fidelity measures. The paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

was familiar with the training materials<br />

in c<strong>on</strong>juncti<strong>on</strong> with the special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

teacher <strong>and</strong> researcher <strong>and</strong> was present<br />

during a minimum of 20% of the sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Inter-observer agreement was calculated<br />

based <strong>on</strong> the point-by-point reliability <strong>and</strong> calculated<br />

by counting the number of agreements<br />

between the special educati<strong>on</strong> teacher<br />

<strong>and</strong> the paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>al <strong>and</strong> dividing this<br />

number by the total number of agreements<br />

<strong>and</strong> disagreements <strong>and</strong> then multiplied by<br />

100% (Cooper, Her<strong>on</strong>, & Heward, 2007).<br />

Procedural reliability. Procedural reliability<br />

was assessed by the special educati<strong>on</strong> classroom<br />

teacher <strong>and</strong> paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>al in the selfc<strong>on</strong>tained<br />

special educati<strong>on</strong> classroom with a<br />

written procedural protocol checklist <strong>and</strong> was<br />

set for a minimum of 90%. Procedural reliability<br />

data was collected during the same sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

as the inter-observer agreement data were<br />

taken by both teachers <strong>on</strong> a minimum of 20%<br />

of the sessi<strong>on</strong>s. A point-by-point agreement<br />

formula (Cooper et al., 2007) was again used<br />

<strong>and</strong> was calculated by counting the number of<br />

times the special educati<strong>on</strong> teacher <strong>and</strong>/or<br />

paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>al agreed that a behavior either<br />

occurred or did not occur during the sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

This number was then divided by the total<br />

number of agreements <strong>and</strong> disagreements<br />

<strong>and</strong> multiplied by 100%. Finally, for each of<br />

the three students their percentage agreement<br />

was recorded for each behavior <strong>on</strong> the<br />

procedural checklist.<br />

Social Validity<br />

A 10-item survey was administered to the students,<br />

teachers, <strong>and</strong> parents to determine the<br />

social validity (Wolf, 1978) of the TouchMath©<br />

program using the touch-points strategy in<br />

mathematics instructi<strong>on</strong>. The items in the social<br />

validity survey were rated <strong>on</strong> a 5-point<br />

Likert scale ranging from 1 (str<strong>on</strong>gly disagree)<br />

to 5 (str<strong>on</strong>gly agree). The social validity<br />

data was collected up<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> of the<br />

study <strong>and</strong> the survey was completed by the<br />

students <strong>and</strong> teachers (e.g., special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

teacher <strong>and</strong> paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>al) in the high<br />

school classroom, while another versi<strong>on</strong> of the<br />

survey for the parents was mailed to their<br />

residence to compete <strong>and</strong> return back to the<br />

special educati<strong>on</strong> teacher. The social validity<br />

survey c<strong>on</strong>sisted of the following items: (1)<br />

TouchMath© is a beneficial strategy to help me<br />

with my subtracti<strong>on</strong> problems; (2) Subtracti<strong>on</strong><br />

is an important skill to have for real-life situati<strong>on</strong>s;<br />

(3) Subtracti<strong>on</strong> is an important skill to<br />

learn before leaving high school; (4) I would<br />

recommend this strategy to some<strong>on</strong>e else; (5)<br />

I understood the TouchMath© strategy <strong>and</strong><br />

what was expected of me; (6) TouchMath© was<br />

easy to use; (7) TouchMath© was an effective<br />

strategy to subtract m<strong>on</strong>ey values; (8) The<br />

target skills are necessary for grade level requirements;<br />

(9) The target skills are necessary<br />

for classroom requirements; <strong>and</strong> (10) The target<br />

skills are necessary for community-life requirements.<br />

Results<br />

Reliability<br />

Inter-observer <strong>and</strong> procedural reliability was<br />

collected during 7 (20%) of the 35 sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Of the 49 probes graded, two were found to<br />

have different scores between the scorers, the<br />

special educati<strong>on</strong> teacher <strong>and</strong> paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>al.<br />

On the two probes, each had <strong>on</strong>e resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

in c<strong>on</strong>flict between the scorers due to<br />

disagreement over identifying a particular<br />

digit in the resp<strong>on</strong>se. The mean percent of<br />

agreements for each student was as follows:<br />

Trent, 100%; Michael, 93.3%; <strong>and</strong> Alex, 95%.<br />

The mean procedural reliability was 100% for<br />

all researcher behaviors across all experimental<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s. Of the observed sessi<strong>on</strong>s, 56%<br />

550 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


Figure 3. Percentage of subtracti<strong>on</strong> 3-digit m<strong>on</strong>ey computati<strong>on</strong>al problems with regrouping using the touchpoints<br />

strategy answered correctly by Trent, Michael, <strong>and</strong> Alex.<br />

were c<strong>on</strong>ducted during the training phase,<br />

while 44% were completed in the probe sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

The inter-observer agreement was<br />

100%.<br />

Effectiveness of the Interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

Figure 3 illustrates the percentage of correct<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses the students received <strong>on</strong> the subtracti<strong>on</strong><br />

problems with regrouping using<br />

m<strong>on</strong>ey computati<strong>on</strong>s during the baseline, interventi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> maintenance phases.<br />

Trent. Trent’s baseline mean score was<br />

6.66% across all 3 sessi<strong>on</strong>s, which dem<strong>on</strong>strates<br />

a stable trend in the data. A substantial<br />

immediate positive score increase was observed<br />

when the interventi<strong>on</strong> probes were issued,<br />

which showed the touch-points strategy<br />

was an effective interventi<strong>on</strong> for acquiring<br />

subtracti<strong>on</strong> problems with regrouping using<br />

m<strong>on</strong>ey values. The average score across all<br />

Touchmath© <strong>and</strong> M<strong>on</strong>ey Skills / 551


interventi<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s was 75.55% dem<strong>on</strong>strating<br />

a c<strong>on</strong>tinual, stable, <strong>and</strong> maintained acquisiti<strong>on</strong><br />

of the target skills set needed to calculate<br />

the correct answers, which was a 68.88%<br />

increase from the baseline phase. Trent<br />

reached criteria within the preset number of<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s, which was established when 80% of<br />

the probe scores were 40% higher than the<br />

baseline scores. There was no data point overlap<br />

observed from the baseline to the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

phases signifying an immediate positive<br />

increase that was maintained throughout<br />

the interventi<strong>on</strong>. The trend from the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

through the maintenance phase was positive<br />

<strong>and</strong> the data points within the trend were<br />

reas<strong>on</strong>ably stable. Trent’s mean score during<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> was 75.55% <strong>and</strong> increased to<br />

83% during the maintenance phase, indicating<br />

a c<strong>on</strong>tinual improvement in skill level with<br />

additi<strong>on</strong>al opportunities to utilize the strategy.<br />

There was an 83% overlap between the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> maintenance phase, though<br />

Trent was able to increase his mean scores<br />

during maintenance. This indicates that Trent<br />

was able to prol<strong>on</strong>g his ability to solve the<br />

subtracti<strong>on</strong> problems with regrouping <strong>and</strong>,<br />

based <strong>on</strong> his scores during the maintenance<br />

phase, improve his scores over time. The interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

strategy was successful initially <strong>and</strong><br />

in sustaining Trent’s ability to solve subtracti<strong>on</strong><br />

problems with regrouping.<br />

Michael. Michael’s mean score during the<br />

baseline phase was 5% indicating that he had<br />

established a flat <strong>and</strong> stable baseline measure.<br />

However, Michael’s mean score during the<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> phase increased to 88%, which<br />

was an 83% increase from the baseline phase.<br />

This abrupt level change <strong>and</strong> 0% overlapping<br />

data points from baseline to interventi<strong>on</strong> substantiated<br />

that the touch-points interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

strategy was effective for Michael in acquiring<br />

the subtracti<strong>on</strong> skills with regrouping. Michael<br />

met criteria after <strong>on</strong>ly four sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

There was a level trend after the abrupt level<br />

change from the baseline to the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

phase indicating a c<strong>on</strong>sistent calculati<strong>on</strong> skill<br />

aptitude. The mean maintenance score for<br />

Michael was 45% with a median score of 35%.<br />

There was 100% data point overlap from interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

to the maintenance phase. A c<strong>on</strong>tinual<br />

decreasing trend occurred during the<br />

maintenance phase ending in a 20% score <strong>on</strong><br />

the final probe sessi<strong>on</strong>. Michael did not revert<br />

to his previous baseline scores, but further<br />

probes would be needed to determine sustained<br />

skill retenti<strong>on</strong>. The interventi<strong>on</strong> strategy<br />

was c<strong>on</strong>firmed to be effective over a relatively<br />

short period of time, as further strategy<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> may prove beneficial for Michael’s<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tinued success.<br />

Alex. Alex’s baseline phase extended for 9<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s, while maintaining a flat <strong>and</strong> stable<br />

trend with a mean score of 2.22%. However,<br />

during the interventi<strong>on</strong> phase, Alex dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

a positive level change with the first<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> probe sessi<strong>on</strong>. The sec<strong>on</strong>d probe<br />

score increased significantly from 20% to<br />

70%, then faltering back to 30% causing an<br />

unstable level change with the first four sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

The fifth sessi<strong>on</strong> established the start of<br />

an observable stable level change. Due to beginning<br />

unstable scores, Alex required the<br />

greatest time to meet criteria. Criteria were<br />

met after ten sessi<strong>on</strong>s with an average score of<br />

76% <strong>and</strong> a 90% median score, which was a<br />

73.77% increase from the baseline measures.<br />

The touch-points interventi<strong>on</strong> strategy was effective<br />

for Alex to subtract 3-digit m<strong>on</strong>ey problems<br />

with regrouping. Between the baseline<br />

<strong>and</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong> phases there was 0% data<br />

point overlap, dem<strong>on</strong>strating a positive level<br />

change though five additi<strong>on</strong>al sessi<strong>on</strong>s were<br />

required until stability <strong>and</strong> criteria were met.<br />

There was 100% overlap from interventi<strong>on</strong> to<br />

maintenance phase. The mean maintenance<br />

score for Michael was 100%. From the beginning<br />

of the baseline to the end of the maintenance<br />

phase, there was a steady increasing<br />

data score trend <strong>and</strong> overall the touch-points<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> strategy proved to be effective for<br />

Alex.<br />

Social Validity Survey<br />

In general, the students, teachers, <strong>and</strong> parents<br />

indicated in the social validity survey that the<br />

TouchMath© program using the touch-points<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> was beneficial. The students<br />

stated that the strategy was easy to use <strong>and</strong><br />

underst<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> improved their ability to solve<br />

subtracti<strong>on</strong> problems with regrouping involving<br />

m<strong>on</strong>ey computati<strong>on</strong>s. The teachers reported<br />

they appreciated the students’ abilities<br />

to quickly acquire <strong>and</strong> successfully follow the<br />

number of steps needed to solve the subtrac-<br />

552 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


ti<strong>on</strong> problems with regrouping. All involved<br />

agreed that others would benefit from exposure<br />

to the TouchMath© program <strong>and</strong> that it<br />

was easy to learn <strong>and</strong> use in the classroom.<br />

The ease of use was also evaluated <strong>and</strong> the<br />

students stated that <strong>on</strong>ce they understood the<br />

steps <strong>and</strong> sequence (e.g., counting the dotnotati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<strong>on</strong> the numbers, etc.), the touchpoints<br />

strategy was a fun, <strong>and</strong> an easy way to<br />

learn how to solve subtracti<strong>on</strong> problems. In<br />

additi<strong>on</strong>, all of the groups indicated that they<br />

agreed or str<strong>on</strong>gly agreed that the skills<br />

gained were necessary for the students to have<br />

before leaving high school to prepare them<br />

for real-life situati<strong>on</strong>s. All involved agreed that<br />

the skill was grade appropriate <strong>and</strong> necessary<br />

for classroom requirements. The students reported<br />

that they were neutral when c<strong>on</strong>sidering<br />

the skills crucial for grade-level requirements;<br />

however, they all agreed that the<br />

touch-points strategy was helpful to learning<br />

mathematics <strong>and</strong> would recommend the strategy<br />

to their peers. Finally, all of the resp<strong>on</strong>dents<br />

agreed that m<strong>on</strong>ey computati<strong>on</strong> skills<br />

are an important <strong>and</strong> critical skill essential for<br />

independent living in the community.<br />

Discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

The purpose of this study was to examine the<br />

effects of the TouchMath© program <strong>on</strong> the<br />

acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of subtracting 3-digit m<strong>on</strong>ey computati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

problems with regrouping for<br />

three students with mild intellectual disabilities<br />

in a high school special educati<strong>on</strong> selfc<strong>on</strong>tained<br />

classroom. The findings indicated<br />

that the use of the touch-point strategy was<br />

effective for all three students in acquiring<br />

3-digit m<strong>on</strong>ey computati<strong>on</strong>al problems with<br />

regrouping. Findings from this study not <strong>on</strong>ly<br />

add to the previous literature base <strong>on</strong> the<br />

TouchMath© program, but also provide new<br />

insights into applicati<strong>on</strong>s to teach m<strong>on</strong>ey computati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

skills to students at the high school<br />

level. As previous studies have suggested, the<br />

touch-points strategy procedures have been<br />

shown to be effective to increase the mathematics<br />

performance for elementary-age students<br />

with specific learning disabilities, moderate<br />

intellectual disabilities, <strong>and</strong> autism spectrum<br />

disorders (Cihak & Foust, 2008; Scott, 1993;<br />

Sim<strong>on</strong> & Hanrahan, 2004), <strong>and</strong> at the middle<br />

school level including students with autism<br />

spectrum disorders <strong>and</strong> moderate intellectual<br />

disabilities (Fletcher et al.). Thus far, no research<br />

has been c<strong>on</strong>ducted <strong>on</strong> the use of the<br />

TouchMath© program with m<strong>on</strong>ey values, including<br />

students with mild intellectual disabilities<br />

at the high school level.<br />

During the baseline phase, all three of the<br />

students dem<strong>on</strong>strated an inability to solve<br />

3-digit subtracti<strong>on</strong> problems with regrouping.<br />

There was an abrupt level change for all three<br />

students with no overlapping data points indicating<br />

an increase in performance <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tinued<br />

level of competence. An ascending trend<br />

was observed for the students during the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

phase exhibiting marked performance<br />

when the interventi<strong>on</strong> was employed.<br />

These observati<strong>on</strong>s provide evidence that the<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> was effective in teaching the students<br />

via the touch-points strategy to subtract<br />

3-digit m<strong>on</strong>ey values with regrouping. During<br />

the interventi<strong>on</strong> probes phase, two of the<br />

three students, Trent <strong>and</strong> Michael, showed<br />

dramatic increases in their mathematics performance<br />

<strong>and</strong> reached criteri<strong>on</strong> in the allotted<br />

amount of time, five sessi<strong>on</strong>s. However,<br />

the third student, Alex, required a total of 10<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s to reach criteri<strong>on</strong>. During the sec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>, Alex scored well <strong>on</strong> the sec<strong>on</strong>d probe<br />

but <strong>on</strong>ly scored 30% <strong>on</strong> the following probe.<br />

This low score caused Alex not to reach criteri<strong>on</strong><br />

in the allotted amount of time. On subsequent<br />

probe scores, Alex averaged 91.4%<br />

over the last seven probes. If the <strong>on</strong>e probe<br />

were erased then Alex would have reached<br />

criteri<strong>on</strong> in five sessi<strong>on</strong>s. Finally, during the<br />

maintenance phase, all three of the students’<br />

mathematics performance showed great variability.<br />

For example, Trent sustained an ascending<br />

trend throughout the study. After six<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s, Trent retained the necessary target<br />

skill set to solve the subtracti<strong>on</strong> problems with<br />

regrouping. Michael dem<strong>on</strong>strated a descending<br />

trend across four of the maintenance sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with a median score of 35%. After the<br />

minimum two-day period, Alex completed<br />

<strong>on</strong>e maintenance sessi<strong>on</strong> with a score of 100%<br />

before the c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> of the study. Clearly,<br />

further research is needed to determine the<br />

ability for students with mild intellectual disabilities<br />

to sustain the skills necessary to subtract<br />

3-digit m<strong>on</strong>ey values with regrouping. To<br />

Touchmath© <strong>and</strong> M<strong>on</strong>ey Skills / 553


help c<strong>on</strong>tinue maximum skill proficiency over<br />

time, refresher sessi<strong>on</strong>s to review the steps <strong>and</strong><br />

procedures of the touch-points method would<br />

be required <strong>and</strong> additi<strong>on</strong>al maintenance<br />

probes may need to be c<strong>on</strong>ducted.<br />

Limitati<strong>on</strong>s of the Study<br />

The following limitati<strong>on</strong>s need to be c<strong>on</strong>sidered<br />

to interpret the findings of this study.<br />

First, the number of students, <strong>on</strong>ly three students,<br />

makes it difficult to support arguments<br />

for generalizati<strong>on</strong> of the touch-points strategy<br />

to all students at the high school level. Also,<br />

the sample <strong>on</strong>ly included students with mild<br />

intellectual disabilities <strong>and</strong>/or autism spectrum<br />

disorders <strong>and</strong> does not represent the<br />

characteristics of typical school-age populati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

So, the findings cannot be generalized<br />

to other disability categories, age, grade, race,<br />

<strong>and</strong>/or other genders. For instance, all three<br />

of the students were male in ninth <strong>and</strong> tenth<br />

grade levels. Sec<strong>on</strong>d, the instructi<strong>on</strong>al procedures<br />

were c<strong>on</strong>ducted <strong>on</strong> a <strong>on</strong>e-to-<strong>on</strong>e basis<br />

<strong>and</strong> would need to be modified for group<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>. Third, during the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> maintenance phases, the students were<br />

provided the “dot-notati<strong>on</strong>s” <strong>on</strong> the numbers<br />

<strong>and</strong> a line to write the lowered number for<br />

regrouping <strong>on</strong> the worksheets; however, this<br />

limits their ability to fully apply <strong>and</strong> generalize<br />

the strategy to novel situati<strong>on</strong>s. Also, the procedures<br />

included <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e specific target skill<br />

set (e.g., 3-digit subtracti<strong>on</strong> problems with regrouping<br />

with m<strong>on</strong>ey values) limiting the ability<br />

to generalize these findings to other mathematical<br />

skills. Fourth, due to student<br />

capability levels, additi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> subtracti<strong>on</strong><br />

without regrouping were not c<strong>on</strong>sidered nor<br />

were higher skill level problems. Maintenance<br />

data was inc<strong>on</strong>sistent <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e probe was<br />

gathered from the last student, Alex, due to a<br />

holiday <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> of the project, as further<br />

investigati<strong>on</strong>s are obviously warranted examining<br />

maintenance capacities of the touchpoints<br />

strategy.<br />

Implicati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

Based <strong>on</strong> the results of this study <strong>and</strong> previous<br />

findings, there are a number of implicati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for classroom teachers, both general <strong>and</strong> spe-<br />

cial educati<strong>on</strong> to c<strong>on</strong>sider. The TouchMath©<br />

program is an easy, simple, <strong>and</strong> teacherfriendly<br />

method to employ as a comp<strong>on</strong>ent of<br />

the instructi<strong>on</strong>al less<strong>on</strong> in a self-c<strong>on</strong>tained,<br />

remedial <strong>and</strong>/or inclusive classroom setting.<br />

The results support a promising <strong>and</strong> growing<br />

research-base for the use of the TouchMath©<br />

strategy to help students not <strong>on</strong>ly with mild<br />

intellectual disabilities <strong>and</strong>/or autism, but<br />

other disability categories, as well as students<br />

without disabilities, that exhibit difficulties in<br />

basic mathematics instructi<strong>on</strong>. Also, the program<br />

allows teachers to adapt their instructi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

at a developmentally appropriate level,<br />

to meet the student’s individual needs <strong>and</strong><br />

learning styles. More recently, TouchMath©<br />

has developed a variety of new products to<br />

teach such c<strong>on</strong>cepts as m<strong>on</strong>ey applicati<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

coins <strong>and</strong> counting, mathematics manipulatives<br />

(e.g., math fans), <strong>and</strong> a software program<br />

known as TouchMath Tutor©, that can easily be<br />

modified for students to teach functi<strong>on</strong>al skills<br />

in a variety of classroom <strong>and</strong> communitybased<br />

settings.<br />

Future Research<br />

In the current research literature base, no<br />

published, empirical studies have examined<br />

the effectiveness of the TouchMath© strategy<br />

to teach students with mild intellectual disabilities<br />

to subtract m<strong>on</strong>ey values, in fact, even<br />

more noteworthy, no studies have explored<br />

this technique with high school populati<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

as much of the limited research-base focuses<br />

almost exclusively <strong>on</strong> elementary-age populati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Future research should address the use<br />

of the touch-points method with not <strong>on</strong>ly additi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> subtracti<strong>on</strong> problems, but with<br />

multiplicati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> divisi<strong>on</strong> problems, with two<br />

<strong>and</strong> three-digits, with <strong>and</strong> without regrouping,<br />

for students with different types of disabilities<br />

in the sec<strong>on</strong>dary grade levels. Also, future<br />

studies should employ experimental group<br />

designs to determine if the strategy can be<br />

implemented <strong>on</strong> a larger scale to reach more<br />

than <strong>on</strong>e student at a time. Further c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong><br />

towards fading the interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> providing<br />

students additi<strong>on</strong>al training time to<br />

memorize <strong>and</strong> independently mark the touchpoints<br />

properly <strong>on</strong> the numbers before solving<br />

the problem is necessary to determine the<br />

efficacy of the strategy. Finally, the probe<br />

554 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


problems employed in this study presented<br />

<strong>on</strong>e instance of regrouping within the problem;<br />

therefore, generalizati<strong>on</strong> to more difficult<br />

skills such as multiple regrouping opportunities<br />

or regrouping with zero in the<br />

problem should be examined.<br />

References<br />

Alberto, P. A., & Troutman, A. C. (2009). Applied<br />

behavior analysis for teachers (8 th ed.). Upper Saddle<br />

River, NJ: Pears<strong>on</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong>, Inc.<br />

Browder, D. M., & Grasso, E. (1999). Teaching<br />

m<strong>on</strong>ey skills to individuals with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>:<br />

A research review with practical applicati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Remedial <strong>and</strong> Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 20, 297–308.<br />

Browder, D. M., Spo<strong>on</strong>er, F., Ahlgrim-Delzell, L.,<br />

Harris, A., & Wakeman, S. Y. (2008). A metaanalysis<br />

<strong>on</strong> teaching mathematics to students with<br />

significant cognitive disabilities. Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children,<br />

74, 407–432.<br />

Bullock, J., Pierce, S., & McClellan, L. (1989). Touch<br />

Math. Colorado Springs, Co: Innovative Learning<br />

C<strong>on</strong>cepts.<br />

Butler, F. M., Miller, S. P., Kit-hung, L., & Pierce, T.<br />

(2001). Teaching mathematics to students with<br />

mild-to-moderate mental retardati<strong>on</strong>: A review of<br />

the literature. Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong>, 39, 20–31.<br />

Cihak, D., & Foust, J. (2008). Comparing number<br />

lines <strong>and</strong> touch points to teach additi<strong>on</strong> facts to<br />

students with autism. Focus <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> Other<br />

<strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 23, 131–137.<br />

Cooper, J. O., Her<strong>on</strong>, T. E., & Heward, W. L.<br />

(2007). Applied behavior analysis (2 nd ed.). Upper<br />

Saddle River, NJ: Merrill.<br />

Fletcher, D., Bo<strong>on</strong>, R., & Cihak, D. (2010). Effects of<br />

the TouchMath program compared to a number<br />

line strategy to teach additi<strong>on</strong> facts to middle<br />

school students with moderate intellectual disabilities.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 45, 449–458.<br />

Harris<strong>on</strong>, P. L., & Oakl<strong>and</strong>, T. (2003), Adaptive Behavior<br />

Assessment System (2nd ed.). San Ant<strong>on</strong>io,<br />

TX: The Psychological Corporati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Individuals with <strong>Disabilities</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong> Improvement<br />

Act of 2004, 20 U. S. C. §§ 1400–1485 (2004<br />

supp. IV), Pub. L. No. 108–4<str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g> (2004), 108 th<br />

C<strong>on</strong>gress, Sec<strong>on</strong>d Sessi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Jitendra, A. K., & Xin, Y. (1997). Mathematical<br />

problem solving instructi<strong>on</strong> for students with<br />

mild disabilities <strong>and</strong> students at risk for math<br />

failure: A research synthesis. The Journal of Special<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>, 30, 412–438.<br />

Kramer, T., & Krug, D. A. (1973). A rati<strong>on</strong>ale <strong>and</strong><br />

procedure for teaching additi<strong>on</strong>. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Training of the Mentally Retarded, 8, 140–145.<br />

Kroesbergen, E. H., & Van Luit, J. (2003). Mathematics<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>s for children with special educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

needs: A meta-analysis. Remedial <strong>and</strong> Special<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>, 24, 97–114.<br />

Mastropieri, M. A., Bakken, J. P., & Scruggs, T. E.<br />

(1991). Mathematics instructi<strong>on</strong> for individuals<br />

with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>: A perspective <strong>and</strong> research<br />

synthesis. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in Mental<br />

Retardati<strong>on</strong>, 26, 115–129.<br />

Miller, S. P., Butler, F. M., & Lee, K. (1998). Validated<br />

practices for teaching mathematics to students<br />

with learning disabilities: A review of literature.<br />

Focus <strong>on</strong> Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, 31, 1–24.<br />

Nati<strong>on</strong>al Council of Teachers of Mathematics.<br />

(2000). Principles <strong>and</strong> NCTM St<strong>and</strong>ards for school<br />

mathematics. Rest<strong>on</strong>, VA: The Nati<strong>on</strong>al Council of<br />

Teachers of Mathematics, Inc.<br />

No Child Left Behind Act of 2001, Public Law 107–<br />

110, 107 th C<strong>on</strong>gress, First Sessi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Scott, K. S. (1993). Multisensory mathematics for<br />

children with mild disabilities. Excepti<strong>on</strong>ality, 4,<br />

97–111.<br />

Sim<strong>on</strong>, R., & Hanrahan, J. (2004). An evaluati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

the touch math method for teaching additi<strong>on</strong> to<br />

students with learning disabilities in mathematics.<br />

European Journal of Special Needs Educati<strong>on</strong>, 19,<br />

191–209.<br />

Swans<strong>on</strong>, H. L., & Jerman, O. (2006). Math disabilities:<br />

A selective meta-analysis of the literature.<br />

Review of Educati<strong>on</strong>al Research, 76, 249–274.<br />

U. S. Department of Educati<strong>on</strong>. (2007). Twentyninth<br />

annual report to C<strong>on</strong>gress <strong>on</strong> the implementati<strong>on</strong><br />

of the Individuals with <strong>Disabilities</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong> Act<br />

(IDEA). Washingt<strong>on</strong>, DC: Author.<br />

Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for<br />

Children-Third Editi<strong>on</strong>. San Ant<strong>on</strong>io, TX: Psychological<br />

Corporati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Wolf, M. M. (1978). Social validity: The case for<br />

subjective measurement or how applied behavioral<br />

analysis is finding its heart. Journal of Applied<br />

Behavior Analysis, 11, 203–214.<br />

Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, K. S., & Werder, J. K.<br />

(1994). Woodcock-McGrew-Werder Mini-Battery of<br />

Achievement. Chicago, IL: Riverside Publishing.<br />

Xin, Y. P., & Jitendra, A. K. (1999). The effects of<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> in solving mathematical word problems<br />

for students with learning problems: A metaanalysis.<br />

The Journal of Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 32, 207–<br />

225.<br />

Received: 7 September 2010<br />

Initial Acceptance: 2 November 2010<br />

Final Acceptance: 23 February 2011<br />

Touchmath© <strong>and</strong> M<strong>on</strong>ey Skills / 555


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 2011, <str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>(4), 556–564<br />

© <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Effects of Computer-Based Practice <strong>on</strong> the Acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Maintenance of Basic Academic Skills for Children with<br />

Moderate to Intensive Educati<strong>on</strong>al Needs<br />

Julie M. Everhart<br />

Westerville City Schools<br />

Sheila R. Alber-Morgan<br />

The Ohio State University<br />

Ju Hee Park<br />

Wheelock College<br />

Abstract: This study investigated the effects of computer-based practice <strong>on</strong> the acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> maintenance of<br />

basic academic skills for two children with moderate to intensive disabilities. The special educati<strong>on</strong> teacher<br />

created individualized computer games that enabled the participants to independently practice academic skills<br />

that corresp<strong>on</strong>ded with their IEP objectives (e.g., letter-sound corresp<strong>on</strong>dence, word identificati<strong>on</strong>, number<br />

identificati<strong>on</strong>). The computer games provided discrete learning trials with immediate feedback for each resp<strong>on</strong>se.<br />

A multiple baseline across skills design dem<strong>on</strong>strated that computer-based practice resulted in the successful<br />

acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of basic academic skills for both participants. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, both participants maintained at least<br />

two mastered skills for two to four weeks.<br />

In this dynamic age of technology, the use of<br />

computers permeates many facets of our daily<br />

living, learning, working, <strong>and</strong> entertainment.<br />

The pervasiveness of rapidly changing technology<br />

<strong>and</strong> the corresp<strong>on</strong>ding need for computer<br />

literacy has required schools to embed<br />

technology into academic instructi<strong>on</strong> in order<br />

to prepare students to live <strong>and</strong> work in a high<br />

tech society. In fact, the Nati<strong>on</strong>al Educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Technology Plan (2004) developed by the<br />

U.S. Department of Educati<strong>on</strong> as part of the<br />

No Child Left Behind Act recommends that<br />

states <strong>and</strong> districts “encourage ubiquitous access<br />

to computers <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>nectivity for each<br />

student” (p. 43).<br />

In additi<strong>on</strong> to preparing students for the<br />

requirements of the real world, student engagement<br />

with computer technology has been<br />

effective for increasing academic skills (e.g.,<br />

Fitzgerald, Koury, & Mitchum, 2008; Hall,<br />

Hughes, & Filbert, 2000). For example, computer<br />

assisted instructi<strong>on</strong> (CAI) has been<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strated to increase reading skills for at<br />

Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence c<strong>on</strong>cerning this article should be<br />

addressed to Sheila Alber-Morgan, Ohio State University,<br />

A356 PAES Building, 305 W. 17 th Ave, Columbus,<br />

OH 43229. E-mail: morgan.651@osu.edu<br />

risk first graders (Chambers et al. 2008), oral<br />

reading fluency for children with attenti<strong>on</strong><br />

deficit/hyperactivity disorder (Clarfield &<br />

St<strong>on</strong>er, 2005), social studies c<strong>on</strong>tent knowledge<br />

for fifth graders with learning disabilities<br />

(Jerome & Barbetta, 2005), comprehensi<strong>on</strong> of<br />

history text by high school students (Twyman<br />

& Tindal, 2006), sight word recogniti<strong>on</strong> for<br />

children with developmental disabilities (Lee<br />

& Vail, 2005), <strong>and</strong> basic academic skills<br />

(shape, color, <strong>and</strong> number identificati<strong>on</strong>) for<br />

preschoolers (Hitchcock & No<strong>on</strong>an, 2000).<br />

CAI may be effective because it utilizes active<br />

student resp<strong>on</strong>ding with immediate feedback<br />

<strong>and</strong>/or reinforcement for each resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

(e.g., Hall et al., 2000; Macaruso & Walker,<br />

2008; Sorrell, Bell, & McCallum, 2007). Research<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strates that students with more<br />

intensive disabilities can benefit a great deal<br />

from frequent opportunities for active student<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ding (e.g., Barbetta, Her<strong>on</strong>, & Heward,<br />

1993). Although CAI has substantial research<br />

support for increasing academic skills, most<br />

CAI research has focused <strong>on</strong> typically developing<br />

students or students with mild to moderate<br />

disabilities. Students with moderate to intensive<br />

disabilities can also benefit from CAI<br />

of academic skills, but most of the technology<br />

556 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


esearch for this populati<strong>on</strong> has focused <strong>on</strong><br />

functi<strong>on</strong>al skills <strong>and</strong> communicati<strong>on</strong> skills<br />

(e.g., Calculator, 2009; Cihak, Kessler, & Alberto,<br />

2008; Lanci<strong>on</strong>i et al., 2009; Mechling,<br />

2008). Therefore, to better substantiate the<br />

effectiveness of CAI, it is necessary to further<br />

investigate the effects of CAI <strong>on</strong> academic<br />

skills for students with moderate to intensive<br />

disabilities. The purpose of this study was to<br />

examine the effects of computer assisted instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>on</strong> the acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> maintenance<br />

of basic skills for children with moderate to<br />

intensive disabilities.<br />

Method<br />

Participants <strong>and</strong> Setting<br />

Participants were two students with moderate<br />

to intensive educati<strong>on</strong>al needs, Joe <strong>and</strong> Nate,<br />

who received individualized instructi<strong>on</strong> in a<br />

self-c<strong>on</strong>tained special educati<strong>on</strong> classroom.<br />

Both participants also received speech therapy,<br />

occupati<strong>on</strong>al therapy, physical therapy,<br />

<strong>and</strong> adapted physical educati<strong>on</strong>. They each<br />

qualified for alternative assessments because<br />

of their cognitive <strong>and</strong> fine motor needs. Joe<br />

was a 9-year-old African American boy who was<br />

diagnosed with traumatic brain injury (TBI).<br />

He participated in a general educati<strong>on</strong> sec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

grade class for approximately 15 minutes per<br />

day. Nate was a 6-year-old Caucasian boy who<br />

was diagnosed with Down syndrome <strong>and</strong> participated<br />

in a general educati<strong>on</strong> kindergarten<br />

class for approximately 30–40 minutes each<br />

day. Joe <strong>and</strong> Nate were selected to participate<br />

in this study because both students had basic<br />

academic skill needs identified in their individual<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> programs <strong>and</strong> both children<br />

had severe learning challenges. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally,<br />

Joe <strong>and</strong> Nate had the following prerequisite<br />

skills needed to participate in this study: (a)<br />

the ability to focus attenti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> a computer<br />

screen, (b) the ability to visually discriminate<br />

between three different academic stimuli <strong>on</strong><br />

the computer screen (e.g., letters), <strong>and</strong> (c)<br />

the ability to use a computer mouse to select<br />

an item <strong>on</strong> the screen.<br />

Experimental sessi<strong>on</strong>s were c<strong>on</strong>ducted in a<br />

self-c<strong>on</strong>tained special educati<strong>on</strong> classroom in<br />

a suburban public elementary school. The<br />

classroom c<strong>on</strong>sisted of seven K-2 students with<br />

multiple disabilities, four paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>als,<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>e special educati<strong>on</strong> teacher (i.e., the<br />

first author). Computer practice took place<br />

during breaks in the morning or afterno<strong>on</strong><br />

when students were typically engaged in individualized<br />

activities. During these sessi<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

two to six other students were present working<br />

with paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>als in other areas of the<br />

classroom away from the computers. The<br />

paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>als also served as observers who<br />

collected IOA <strong>and</strong> procedural reliability data.<br />

Definiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Measurement of Dependent<br />

Variable<br />

The dependent variable was acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of basic<br />

academic skills, which was measured by the<br />

number of correct academic resp<strong>on</strong>ses from<br />

10 resp<strong>on</strong>se prompts per sessi<strong>on</strong> presented <strong>on</strong><br />

flashcards. Flashcard probes were used to assess<br />

academic skill acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> maintenance<br />

because the participants were accustomed<br />

to this form of assessment in their daily<br />

routine. The flashcards also provided a c<strong>on</strong>sistent<br />

visual cue for the participants throughout<br />

the study.<br />

For Joe, a correct resp<strong>on</strong>se was scored each<br />

time he stated the correct answer within 5-s of<br />

the flashcard presentati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> verbal prompt.<br />

For example, the special educati<strong>on</strong> teacher<br />

showed Joe a flashcard with a number printed<br />

<strong>on</strong> it <strong>and</strong> said “What number?” A correct resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

was scored when Joe stated the correct<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se within 5-s. An incorrect resp<strong>on</strong>se was<br />

scored when Joe stated an answer that did not<br />

corresp<strong>on</strong>d to the item presented or made no<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se within 5-s. If Joe resp<strong>on</strong>ded incorrectly<br />

<strong>and</strong> then self-corrected, the resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

was scored as incorrect.<br />

Because of Nate’s difficulties producing<br />

oral language, his resp<strong>on</strong>ses were measured by<br />

observing his gestures. Specifically, Nate’s special<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> teacher presented him with two<br />

flashcards <strong>and</strong> verbally prompted him to<br />

touch or point to the correct resp<strong>on</strong>se. For<br />

example, <strong>on</strong>e flashcard showed the number<br />

“11” <strong>and</strong> the other flashcard showed the number<br />

“15.” The special educati<strong>on</strong> teacher verbally<br />

prompted, “Touch 15.” A correct resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

was counted when Nate touched or<br />

pointed to the correct flashcard within 5-s. An<br />

incorrect resp<strong>on</strong>se was recorded when Nate<br />

touched the wr<strong>on</strong>g card, touched both cards<br />

Computer-Based Practice / 557


at the same time, or did not resp<strong>on</strong>d within<br />

5-s.<br />

The experimenters identified three target<br />

academic skills for each participant based <strong>on</strong><br />

their IEP objectives. For each target skill, 10<br />

items (i.e., letters, numbers, or words) were<br />

selected <strong>and</strong> used as resp<strong>on</strong>se prompts for<br />

computer practice <strong>and</strong> flashcards. The following<br />

basic skills were selected for each participant<br />

in order of when the interventi<strong>on</strong> was<br />

introduced.<br />

Joe: number identificati<strong>on</strong> (11–20), lettersound<br />

identificati<strong>on</strong> (K-T), <strong>and</strong> color word<br />

identificati<strong>on</strong> (e.g., red, blue).<br />

Nate: color word identificati<strong>on</strong> (e.g., red,<br />

blue), letter identificati<strong>on</strong> (A-J), <strong>and</strong> number<br />

identificati<strong>on</strong> (11–20).<br />

Flashcard probes were always c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

<strong>on</strong>e day after computer practice prior to the<br />

next computer practice, <strong>and</strong> flashcards were<br />

presented in r<strong>and</strong>om order during each assessment.<br />

Independent Variable<br />

The independent variable was independent<br />

practice of the target skills using computer<br />

games that were designed by the teacher using<br />

the Microsoft PowerPoint® program. With<br />

this program, the teacher digitally recorded<br />

<strong>and</strong> embedded her voice into each game in<br />

the form of verbal prompts, feedback, <strong>and</strong><br />

praise. The materials used in this experiment<br />

included a computer, a computer mouse, individualized<br />

computer games, a digital timer,<br />

<strong>and</strong> flashcards.<br />

When each participant began the game, he<br />

was presented with a digital verbal prompt to<br />

select a specific item from a choice of three<br />

items (e.g., A, B, C). The games provided<br />

visual <strong>and</strong> auditory praise for correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses.<br />

When the student resp<strong>on</strong>ded correctly,<br />

a written praise statement appeared <strong>on</strong><br />

the computer screen (e.g., “Great job”) accompanied<br />

by the teacher’s voice enthusiastically<br />

saying the praise statement. The praise<br />

screen was immediately followed by animati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<strong>and</strong> sound effects (e.g., a car going<br />

across the screen with the sound of the engine,<br />

a fish swimming across the screen with a<br />

gurgling noise <strong>and</strong> bubbles). After presenting<br />

praise <strong>and</strong> reinforcement for correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses,<br />

the game advanced to the next learning<br />

trial.<br />

When a student emitted an incorrect resp<strong>on</strong>se,<br />

the next screen showed the words<br />

“Try again,” accompanied by the teacher’s<br />

m<strong>on</strong>ot<strong>on</strong>e voice saying “Try again.” No additi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

sound effects or animati<strong>on</strong>s were provided<br />

for incorrect resp<strong>on</strong>ses. The student was<br />

then presented with another opportunity to<br />

attempt the learning trial. The computer program<br />

did not advance to a new learning trial<br />

until the student emitted the correct resp<strong>on</strong>se.<br />

For each sessi<strong>on</strong>, the computer resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

prompts were presented in r<strong>and</strong>om<br />

order.<br />

During computer practice sessi<strong>on</strong>s, the participants<br />

were provided <strong>on</strong>e trial for each of<br />

10 items with a maximum of 3-min to play<br />

each game. For example, for letter identificati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

the participant was given <strong>on</strong>e opportunity<br />

to identify each of the 10 selected letters.<br />

After three minutes of computer practice,<br />

whether or not the participant finished all 10<br />

learning trials, the practice sessi<strong>on</strong> ended for<br />

that game <strong>and</strong> the participant was directed to<br />

another computer game (e.g., number identificati<strong>on</strong>)<br />

or a different classroom activity. If<br />

the participant finished the 10 trials before<br />

three minutes had elapsed, he or she was also<br />

directed to the next task.<br />

Experimental Design <strong>and</strong> Procedure<br />

A multiple-baseline across skills design was<br />

used to examine the effects of computer practice<br />

<strong>on</strong> acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> maintenance of basic<br />

skills. The experimental c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s c<strong>on</strong>sisted<br />

of baseline, training, computer practice, <strong>and</strong><br />

maintenance. For each student, each tier of<br />

the multiple baseline design shows the data<br />

for <strong>on</strong>e of three different target skills. Baseline<br />

data were collected for all three target<br />

skills. Interventi<strong>on</strong> began with the skill that<br />

showed the lowest <strong>and</strong> most stable baseline<br />

data. When the participants attained 60% accuracy<br />

<strong>on</strong> three c<strong>on</strong>secutive sessi<strong>on</strong>s for the<br />

first skill, interventi<strong>on</strong> began with the next<br />

skill <strong>on</strong> the sec<strong>on</strong>d tier. When 60% accuracy<br />

was attained <strong>on</strong> three c<strong>on</strong>secutive sessi<strong>on</strong>s for<br />

the sec<strong>on</strong>d skill, interventi<strong>on</strong> began for the<br />

third skill <strong>on</strong> the final tier. The interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>tinued for each target skill until the par-<br />

558 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


ticipant attained 80% accuracy <strong>on</strong> three c<strong>on</strong>secutive<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s. Then the maintenance c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong><br />

was implemented.<br />

Baseline. During baseline, the special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

teacher sat across from the participant<br />

at a table <strong>and</strong> presented three sets of flash<br />

cards, <strong>on</strong>e set for each skill. The teacher held<br />

up each flashcard <strong>and</strong> provided a verbal<br />

prompt (e.g., “What word?”), <strong>and</strong> recorded<br />

correct <strong>and</strong> incorrect resp<strong>on</strong>ses. Joe was expected<br />

to provide verbal resp<strong>on</strong>ses. Nate was<br />

presented with two flashcards <strong>and</strong> asked to<br />

point to the correct resp<strong>on</strong>se (e.g., “Touch<br />

red.”). When the student resp<strong>on</strong>ded correctly,<br />

the teacher delivered a praise statement (e.g.,<br />

“Good! You got it!”). Feedback was not provided<br />

for incorrect resp<strong>on</strong>ses. If the student<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ded incorrectly, the teacher said nothing<br />

<strong>and</strong> presented the next flashcard. If the<br />

participant became distracted or off-task (e.g.,<br />

looking around, attempting to leave the table),<br />

the teacher used prompts to re-direct<br />

him back to the task (e.g., “Almost d<strong>on</strong>e,”<br />

“Three more,” “We’re working now.”).<br />

Training. Prior to beginning the interventi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

the teacher provided the participants<br />

with training for using the computer mouse to<br />

select items <strong>on</strong> the computer screen. Training<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s were about three minutes in durati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> included verbal directi<strong>on</strong>s, modeling,<br />

least-to-most prompting, guided practice, <strong>and</strong><br />

systematic error correcti<strong>on</strong>. Once the participants<br />

were observed using the mouse independently<br />

to move the cursor to an object <strong>on</strong><br />

the screen <strong>and</strong> click <strong>on</strong> a selecti<strong>on</strong>, they began<br />

the computer practice c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>. Nate received<br />

this training <strong>on</strong> two c<strong>on</strong>secutive days<br />

<strong>and</strong> Joe received training <strong>on</strong> six c<strong>on</strong>secutive<br />

days.<br />

Computer practice. During computer practice,<br />

the teacher set up the game <strong>and</strong><br />

prompted the participant to go to the computer<br />

<strong>and</strong> play the game. The participants<br />

completed the games independently, receiving<br />

feedback <strong>on</strong>ly from the computer game<br />

itself. Each computer game ended with an<br />

audible signal <strong>and</strong> a blank screen after the 10<br />

trials had been completed or after the three<br />

minutes had elapsed, whichever occurred<br />

first. An observer m<strong>on</strong>itored the participants<br />

to make sure they did not leave the computer<br />

before they were finished with the game. If the<br />

participants had attempted to leave the task,<br />

the observer would have redirected the participant<br />

back to the computer, however, no students<br />

attempted to leave the computer during<br />

any of the practice sessi<strong>on</strong>s. The flashcard<br />

probes used to assess acquisiti<strong>on</strong> were administered<br />

the following day prior to the next<br />

computer practice sessi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Maintenance. After the participants attained<br />

80% mastery <strong>on</strong> three c<strong>on</strong>secutive sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for <strong>on</strong>e skill, computer practice was disc<strong>on</strong>tinued<br />

for that skill. For example, if a<br />

student achieved 80% accuracy <strong>on</strong> three c<strong>on</strong>secutive<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s with letter-sound identificati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

but did not meet the 80% criteria for<br />

number <strong>and</strong> sight word recogniti<strong>on</strong>, computer<br />

practice was disc<strong>on</strong>tinued for lettersound<br />

recogniti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly. Maintenance probes<br />

were administered using the same flashcard<br />

procedures used throughout the study <strong>and</strong><br />

were administered approximately two days<br />

each week.<br />

Interobserver Agreement (IOA) <strong>and</strong> Procedural<br />

Reliability<br />

IOA was assessed <strong>on</strong> 20% of the sessi<strong>on</strong>s. A<br />

sec<strong>on</strong>d observer was present to record the<br />

participant’s resp<strong>on</strong>ses to the flashcard<br />

prompts. IOA was calculated by dividing the<br />

number of agreements by the number of<br />

agreements plus disagreements <strong>and</strong> multiplying<br />

by 100.<br />

Procedural reliability was assessed <strong>on</strong> 20%<br />

of the sessi<strong>on</strong>s for computer practice <strong>and</strong><br />

flashcard probes. Procedural reliability checklists<br />

were used to determine the extent to<br />

which the interventi<strong>on</strong> was implemented as<br />

planned. A sec<strong>on</strong>d observer checked off the<br />

steps that were completed correctly <strong>and</strong> in the<br />

correct sequence. The number of steps completed<br />

correctly was divided by the total number<br />

of steps <strong>and</strong> then multiplied by 100 to<br />

calculate the percentage of procedural reliability.<br />

Results<br />

Figure 1 shows the number of correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses<br />

across skills <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s for Joe,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Figure 2 shows the number of correct<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses across skills <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s for<br />

Nate. During baseline, Joe’s data were stable,<br />

ranging between zero <strong>and</strong> four correct re-<br />

Computer-Based Practice / 559


Figure 1. Correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses for Joe for number identificati<strong>on</strong>, letter-sound identificati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> color word<br />

identificati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

560 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


Figure 2. Correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses for Nate for color word identificati<strong>on</strong>, letter identificati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> number identificati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Computer-Based Practice / 561


sp<strong>on</strong>ses for each skill. No trends were apparent<br />

during baseline for Joe. Nate’s baseline<br />

data were more variable, ranging between<br />

zero <strong>and</strong> six correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses across skills.<br />

Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, Nate’s sec<strong>on</strong>d tier (letter identificati<strong>on</strong>)<br />

shows a slight upward trend during<br />

baseline. When the interventi<strong>on</strong> was introduced,<br />

both students showed a clear upward<br />

trend of correct resp<strong>on</strong>ding for each skill.<br />

Joe’s sec<strong>on</strong>d tier (letter-sound identificati<strong>on</strong>)<br />

shows a delayed upward trend beginning <strong>on</strong><br />

the fifth interventi<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong> followed by rapid<br />

progress to mastery (i.e., 4 sessi<strong>on</strong>s). Both<br />

participants also dem<strong>on</strong>strated maintenance.<br />

Nate dem<strong>on</strong>strated maintenance for all three<br />

skills <strong>and</strong> Joe dem<strong>on</strong>strated maintenance for<br />

letters <strong>and</strong> numbers. Although Joe’s color<br />

word proficiency increased, he was unable to<br />

reach the criteria for beginning the maintenance<br />

phase for color words in the time available<br />

for data collecti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Interobserver Agreement <strong>and</strong> Procedural Reliability<br />

Interobserver agreement (IOA) was assessed<br />

in each c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> for each participant <strong>on</strong> 20%<br />

of the sessi<strong>on</strong>s. Across all phases, skills, <strong>and</strong><br />

participants, mean IOA was 99.4% (ranging<br />

from 80% to 100%). Procedural reliability was<br />

assessed <strong>on</strong> 20% of the sessi<strong>on</strong>s for the computer<br />

practice procedures <strong>and</strong> the flashcard<br />

assessment procedures. Procedural reliability<br />

was 100% for computer practice <strong>and</strong> 97% for<br />

the flashcard assessment procedures.<br />

Discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

Results of this study dem<strong>on</strong>strate that computer-based<br />

practice was functi<strong>on</strong>ally related to<br />

acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> maintenance of basic skills by<br />

two children with moderate to intensive disabilities.<br />

Although each participant showed<br />

variati<strong>on</strong>s in rate <strong>and</strong> pattern of resp<strong>on</strong>ding,<br />

they both achieved substantial improvement<br />

of basic academic skills over the course of<br />

12–14 weeks. Both participants also dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

maintenance for at least two academic<br />

skills over two to four weeks.<br />

Joe’s correct resp<strong>on</strong>ding was low <strong>and</strong> stable<br />

in baseline. When computer practice was introduced,<br />

Joe’s data show upward trends for<br />

each skill (see Figure 1). For letter sounds, Joe<br />

did not begin to show improvement until the<br />

fifth interventi<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong> when his pattern of<br />

correct resp<strong>on</strong>ding increased substantially.<br />

The experimenters were unable to determine<br />

the reas<strong>on</strong> for the delayed, then substantial,<br />

increase in correct resp<strong>on</strong>ding for letter<br />

sounds. During maintenance, Joe c<strong>on</strong>sistently<br />

attained 90–100% accuracy for number identificati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> letter sounds. He was unable to<br />

begin the maintenance phase for color words<br />

due to the limited time for data collecti<strong>on</strong>, but<br />

based <strong>on</strong> his data trend <strong>on</strong> the third tier, he<br />

probably would have met mastery for color<br />

words if given more time.<br />

Nate’s data were highly variable throughout<br />

the study until he began the maintenance<br />

phase (see Figure 2). Because he selected the<br />

correct answer from two flashcards, Nate had<br />

a 50% chance of getting the answer correct.<br />

This may have inflated his percent of correct<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses <strong>and</strong> produced increased variability<br />

throughout the experiment. However, during<br />

maintenance, similar to Joe, Nate dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sistently accurate resp<strong>on</strong>ding at<br />

90–100%. The findings of this study support<br />

previous research that computer assisted instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

is an effective tool for increasing<br />

academic performance (e.g., Chambers et al.,<br />

2008; Clarfield & St<strong>on</strong>er, 2005; Jerome & Barbetta,<br />

2005) <strong>and</strong> extends the findings of computer<br />

assisted instructi<strong>on</strong> to children with<br />

moderate to intensive disabilities. Aspects of<br />

computer practice that likely increased the<br />

effectiveness of the interventi<strong>on</strong> include c<strong>on</strong>sistent<br />

visual <strong>and</strong> auditory resp<strong>on</strong>se prompts,<br />

frequent opportunities for active resp<strong>on</strong>ding,<br />

<strong>and</strong> immediate feedback for each resp<strong>on</strong>se.<br />

The results of this study are also c<strong>on</strong>sistent<br />

with previous research dem<strong>on</strong>strating the effectiveness<br />

of active student resp<strong>on</strong>ding with<br />

immediate feedback for students with more<br />

intensive disabilities (e.g., Barbetta et al.,<br />

1993).<br />

Limitati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> Future Research<br />

While this study provides evidence of a functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

relati<strong>on</strong>ship between computer practice<br />

<strong>and</strong> basic skills acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> maintenance,<br />

several limitati<strong>on</strong>s should be c<strong>on</strong>sidered. First,<br />

each student attended a general educati<strong>on</strong><br />

classroom for 15 to 40 minutes each day to<br />

increase their social interacti<strong>on</strong>s with typically<br />

developing peers. The experimenters were<br />

562 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


unable to c<strong>on</strong>trol the amount of exposure to<br />

the target skills outside of the special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

classroom (including home <strong>and</strong> community<br />

settings). Future research should attempt<br />

to c<strong>on</strong>trol for exposure to the target skills in<br />

other settings.<br />

A limitati<strong>on</strong> that may have influenced the<br />

students’ rate of learning is the amount of<br />

practice they received. Due to time restraints<br />

<strong>and</strong> the participants’ limited attenti<strong>on</strong> spans,<br />

each game was c<strong>on</strong>fined to three minutes<br />

which <strong>on</strong>ly allowed for <strong>on</strong>e practice trial for<br />

each item. The students’ rates of learning may<br />

have been faster if they spent more time practicing.<br />

Future research should attempt to determine<br />

the optimum number of learning trials<br />

needed to promote mastery of basic<br />

academic skills for students with moderate to<br />

intensive disabilities.<br />

Throughout the study, flashcard probes<br />

were administered to assess progress of basic<br />

skills. Repeated exposure as well as feedback<br />

for correct resp<strong>on</strong>ses during the flashcard<br />

probes may have c<strong>on</strong>tributed to the participants’<br />

increased proficiency with basic skills.<br />

C<strong>on</strong>sequently, the participants’ performance<br />

may have been the result of the combined<br />

effects of computer practice <strong>and</strong> flashcard<br />

probes as opposed to computer practice<br />

al<strong>on</strong>e. The upward trend during baseline in<br />

the sec<strong>on</strong>d tier of Figure 2 indicates this possibility<br />

for Nate. However, Joe’s baseline data<br />

for each skill displays hardly any change in<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ding until computer practice was implemented.<br />

Future research should attempt to<br />

identify a way to assess the outcomes of computer<br />

practice without introducing possible<br />

c<strong>on</strong>founding variables associated with assessment.<br />

Another limitati<strong>on</strong> of this study is that the<br />

experimenters did not assess generalizati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

the target skills to other settings or situati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Future research would be strengthened by<br />

measures of skill generalizati<strong>on</strong> to other classrooms<br />

or settings, with different teachers or<br />

peers, in different instructi<strong>on</strong>al arrangements,<br />

or with different applicati<strong>on</strong> activities. One<br />

type of generalizati<strong>on</strong>, resp<strong>on</strong>se maintenance,<br />

was measured in this study. However, the end<br />

of the school year limited the amount of maintenance<br />

data collected to <strong>on</strong>ly a few weeks.<br />

Future research should attempt to implement<br />

l<strong>on</strong>ger maintenance phases or collect follow-up<br />

data.<br />

The research <strong>on</strong> academic interventi<strong>on</strong>s for<br />

children with moderate to intensive disabilities<br />

is an important area of need in the field of<br />

special educati<strong>on</strong>. Future research should<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tinue to investigate <strong>and</strong> identify effective<br />

strategies for teaching a range of academic<br />

<strong>and</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>al skills to individuals moderate<br />

to intensive educati<strong>on</strong>al needs.<br />

Implicati<strong>on</strong>s for Practice<br />

Students with moderate to intensive disabilities<br />

need individualized instructi<strong>on</strong>, repeated<br />

practice with immediate feedback, <strong>and</strong> frequent<br />

opportunities to increase their independent<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>ing. Customized computer<br />

games can provide a means for meeting these<br />

student needs. The flexibility of programming<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> with computers allows for a wide<br />

range of possibilities. Computer games can be<br />

customized to each student’s IEP objectives,<br />

modes of resp<strong>on</strong>ding, <strong>and</strong> reinforcer preferences<br />

(e.g., animated characters, colors,<br />

sound effects).<br />

In this study, the special educati<strong>on</strong> teacher<br />

created individualized computer games that<br />

the participants could play independently.<br />

The results dem<strong>on</strong>strated that computer practice<br />

enables students with moderate to intensive<br />

disabilities to work independently for at<br />

least brief periods of time. While the participants<br />

were <strong>on</strong>ly playing each game for a maximum<br />

of three minutes (totaling nine minutes<br />

if they reached criteria to play the games for<br />

all three skills), n<strong>on</strong>e of the participants attempted<br />

to leave the computer area while<br />

playing. Anecdotally, the classroom staff observed<br />

the participants smiling, giggling, clapping<br />

their h<strong>and</strong>s, <strong>and</strong> repeating sounds while<br />

they were engaged in computer practice; <strong>and</strong><br />

both participants requested to play the computer<br />

games when it was not their designated<br />

time to play. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, using the teacher’s<br />

voice for auditory prompts <strong>and</strong> feedback may<br />

have promoted student motivati<strong>on</strong> as well as<br />

generalized resp<strong>on</strong>ding.<br />

Creating customized computer games for<br />

this study required a little creativity, a basic<br />

underst<strong>and</strong>ing of Microsoft PowerPoint®, <strong>and</strong><br />

time to c<strong>on</strong>struct the games. The first game<br />

can take up to an hour to create, but could<br />

Computer-Based Practice / 563


then be used as a template for quicker creati<strong>on</strong><br />

of additi<strong>on</strong>al games. Once the teacher<br />

creates the games, she can use them throughout<br />

the school year <strong>and</strong> modify them as<br />

needed for individual students. In additi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

Microsoft PowerPoint® is a st<strong>and</strong>ard feature<br />

<strong>on</strong> most school computers, so teachers can<br />

create their own games for free. In fact, <strong>on</strong>e of<br />

the reas<strong>on</strong>s that the teacher in this study designed<br />

the customized games using Microsoft<br />

PowerPoint® was that appropriate computer<br />

software was unavailable for her students.<br />

Equipment <strong>and</strong> materials for students with<br />

significant disabilities tends to be expensive,<br />

however, Microsoft PowerPoint® was already<br />

available throughout the school district at no<br />

additi<strong>on</strong>al cost. C<strong>on</strong>sidering the potential for<br />

increased academic achievement <strong>and</strong> opportunities<br />

to work independently, this interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

can have an excellent cost-benefit ratio.<br />

Teachers of students with moderate to intensive<br />

disabilities can learn to make customized<br />

computer games designed to meet each of<br />

their students’ unique academic learning<br />

needs.<br />

References<br />

Barbetta, P., Her<strong>on</strong>, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (1993).<br />

Effects of active student resp<strong>on</strong>se during error<br />

correcti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the acquisiti<strong>on</strong>, maintenance, <strong>and</strong><br />

generalizati<strong>on</strong> of sight words by students with<br />

developmental disabilities. Journal of Applied Behavior<br />

Analysis, 26, 111–119.<br />

Calculator, S. (2009). Augmentative <strong>and</strong> alternative<br />

communicati<strong>on</strong> (AAC) <strong>and</strong> inclusive educati<strong>on</strong><br />

for students with the most severe disabilities. Internati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Journal of Inclusive Educati<strong>on</strong>, 13, 93–<br />

113.<br />

Chambers, B., Slavin, R., Madden, N., Abrami, P.,<br />

Tucker, B., Cheung, A., et al. (2008). Technology<br />

infusi<strong>on</strong> in Success for All: Reading outcomes for<br />

first graders. Elementary School Journal, 109, 1–15.<br />

Cihak, D., Kessler, K., & Alberto, P. (2008). Use of a<br />

h<strong>and</strong>held prompting system to transiti<strong>on</strong> independently<br />

through vocati<strong>on</strong>al tasks for students<br />

with moderate <strong>and</strong> severe intellectual disabilities.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>,<br />

43, 102–110.<br />

Clarfield, J., & St<strong>on</strong>er, G. (2005). The effects of<br />

computerized reading instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the academic<br />

performance of students identified with<br />

ADHD. School Psychology Review, 4, 2<str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>–254.<br />

Fitzgerald, G., Koury, K., & Mitchem, K. (2008).<br />

Research <strong>on</strong> computer-mediated instructi<strong>on</strong> for<br />

students with high incidence disabilities. Journal of<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>al Research, 38, 201–233.<br />

Hall, T. E., Hughes, C. A., & Filbert, M. (2000).<br />

Computer assisted instructi<strong>on</strong> in reading for students<br />

with learning disabilities: A research synthesis.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Treatment of Children, 23, 173–<br />

193.<br />

Hitchcock, C. H., & No<strong>on</strong>an, M. J. (2000). Computer<br />

assisted instructi<strong>on</strong> of early academic skills.<br />

Topics in Early Childhood Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 20, 145–<br />

168.<br />

Jerome, A., & Barbetta, P. (2005). The effect of<br />

active student resp<strong>on</strong>ding during computer-assisted<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> social studies learning by<br />

students with learning disabilities. Journal of Special<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> Technology, 20, 13–23.<br />

Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, G., Singh, N., O’Reilly, M. F., Sigafoos, J.,<br />

Oliva, D., & Cingolani, E. (2009). Students with<br />

multiple disabilities using technology-based programs<br />

to choose <strong>and</strong> access stimulus events al<strong>on</strong>e<br />

or with caregiver participati<strong>on</strong>. Research in <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 30, 689–701.<br />

Lee, Y., & Vail, C. O. (2005). Computer-based reading<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> for young children with disabilities.<br />

Journal of Special Educati<strong>on</strong> Technology, 20,<br />

145–158.<br />

Macaruso, P., & Walker, A. (2008). The efficacy of<br />

computer-assisted instructi<strong>on</strong> for advancing literacy<br />

skills in kindergarten children. Reading Psychology,<br />

29, 266–287.<br />

Mechling, L. (2008). High tech cooking: A literature<br />

review of evolving technologies for teaching<br />

a functi<strong>on</strong>al skill. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 43, 474–485.<br />

Sorrell, C. A., Bell, S. M., & McCallum, R. S. (2007).<br />

Reading rate <strong>and</strong> comprehensi<strong>on</strong> as a functi<strong>on</strong> of<br />

computerized versus traditi<strong>on</strong>al presentati<strong>on</strong><br />

mode: A preliminary study. Journal of Special Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

Technology, 22, 1–12.<br />

Twyman, T., & Tindal, G. (2006). Using a computeradapted<br />

c<strong>on</strong>ceptually based history text to increase<br />

comprehensi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> problem-solving skills<br />

of students with disabilities. Journal of Special Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

Technology, 21, 5–16.<br />

U.S. Department of Educati<strong>on</strong>. (2004). Nati<strong>on</strong>al Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

Technology Plan 2004. Retrieved April 1,<br />

2009, from U.S. Department of Educati<strong>on</strong> Web<br />

Site: http://www.ed.gov/about/offices/list/os/<br />

technology/plan/2004/site/edlite-default.html<br />

U.S. Department of Educati<strong>on</strong>. (2004). Toward a<br />

new golden age in American educati<strong>on</strong>: How the internet,<br />

the law <strong>and</strong> today’s students are revoluti<strong>on</strong>izing<br />

expectati<strong>on</strong>s. Washingt<strong>on</strong>, D.C., Office of Educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Technology.<br />

Received: 27 July 2010<br />

Initial Acceptance: 20 September 2010<br />

Final Acceptance: 20 November 2010<br />

564 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 2011, <str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>(4), 565–575<br />

© <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Providing Choice Making in Employment Programs: The<br />

Beginning or End of Self-Determinati<strong>on</strong>?<br />

Martin Agran <strong>and</strong> Michael Krupp<br />

University of Wyoming<br />

Abstract: Choice making represents the central element of self-determinati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> efforts are being made across<br />

all service programs to promote choice making for c<strong>on</strong>sumers with intellectual <strong>and</strong> developmental disabilities.<br />

Although choice making appears to be a relatively simple resp<strong>on</strong>se for a c<strong>on</strong>sumer to perform (selecting <strong>on</strong>e<br />

stimulus over another), it is a complex phenomen<strong>on</strong> involving several different comp<strong>on</strong>ents. This paper provides<br />

a selected review of the choice-making research literature, relative to employment service delivery. The paper<br />

examines the relati<strong>on</strong>ship of choice making to self-determinati<strong>on</strong>, how choice making can promote engagement<br />

<strong>and</strong> motivati<strong>on</strong> for employees, <strong>and</strong> what barriers may exist that thwart meaningful choice making. Recommendati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

to support pers<strong>on</strong>nel <strong>and</strong> practiti<strong>on</strong>ers <strong>on</strong> practices to promote effective choice making are provided.<br />

Self-determinati<strong>on</strong> refers to strategies that enable<br />

individuals to regulate their own behavior,<br />

independent of external c<strong>on</strong>trol, <strong>and</strong> allow<br />

them to have c<strong>on</strong>trol over choices <strong>and</strong><br />

decisi<strong>on</strong>s that impact their lives (e.g., where<br />

<strong>and</strong> how they want to work, where they live).<br />

These strategies aim to teach individuals to set<br />

appropriate goals for themselves, m<strong>on</strong>itor<br />

their performance, identify soluti<strong>on</strong>s to present<br />

or future problems, verbally direct their<br />

own behavior, reinforce themselves, or evaluate<br />

their own performance. Self-determined<br />

individuals make choices, act <strong>on</strong> those<br />

choices, experience the results, <strong>and</strong> then<br />

make new choices (Martin, Woods, Sylvester,<br />

& Gardner, 2006).<br />

Although data <strong>on</strong> the relati<strong>on</strong>ship between<br />

self-determinati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> employment outcomes<br />

remain limited, several researchers have suggested<br />

that individuals who are more self-determined<br />

attain more positive transiti<strong>on</strong> outcomes.<br />

Wehmeyer <strong>and</strong> Schwartz (1997)<br />

examined adult outcomes of a sample of students<br />

with learning or intellectual disabilities<br />

<strong>on</strong>e year after they left high school. Those<br />

Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence c<strong>on</strong>cerning this article should<br />

be addressed to Martin Agran, Department of Special<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong>, College of Educati<strong>on</strong>, Department<br />

3374, 1000 E. University Avenue, University of Wyoming,<br />

Laramie, Wyoming, 82071. Email: magran@<br />

uwyo.edu<br />

with higher levels of self-determinati<strong>on</strong> were<br />

more than twice as likely to be employed <strong>and</strong><br />

earned, <strong>on</strong> average, $2.00 per hour more than<br />

employees with lower self-determinati<strong>on</strong> levels.<br />

Also, Wehmeyer <strong>and</strong> Palmer (2003) examined<br />

the adult status of 94 individuals with<br />

intellectual disabilities 1 <strong>and</strong> 3 years after<br />

graduati<strong>on</strong>. One year after high school, employees<br />

with higher levels of self-determinati<strong>on</strong><br />

were more likely to have moved from<br />

where they were living during high school,<br />

<strong>and</strong> by year 3 were still more likely to live<br />

somewhere other than their high school<br />

home. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, individuals who scored<br />

higher in self-determinati<strong>on</strong> made statistically<br />

significant advances in obtaining job benefits,<br />

including vacati<strong>on</strong>, sick leave, <strong>and</strong> health insurance,<br />

than their peers with lower self-determinati<strong>on</strong><br />

levels. Further, Wehmeyer <strong>and</strong><br />

Schwartz (1997) reported that am<strong>on</strong>g 50<br />

adults with intellectual disabilities higher selfdeterminati<strong>on</strong><br />

scores predicted a higher quality<br />

of life. Last, Wehmeyer (2003) indicated<br />

that c<strong>on</strong>sumers served through the vocati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> system achieve better outcomes<br />

when they are actively involved in planning<br />

<strong>and</strong> related decisi<strong>on</strong>-making.<br />

Choice Making <strong>and</strong> Self-Determinati<strong>on</strong><br />

Choice making is regarded as the central element<br />

of self-determinati<strong>on</strong> (Wehmeyer et al.,<br />

Choice Making / 565


2007). Choice making initiates the self-determinati<strong>on</strong><br />

process <strong>and</strong> prompts acti<strong>on</strong> (Deci &<br />

Ryan, 1985; Schloss, Alper, & Jayne, 1993).<br />

Opportunities to make choices allows employees<br />

to select work tasks <strong>and</strong> jobs that best<br />

match their wishes, interests, <strong>and</strong> capabilities,<br />

<strong>and</strong>, in doing so, promotes greater engagement<br />

<strong>and</strong> motivati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> their part (Mithaug,<br />

2005). To a large extent, self-determinati<strong>on</strong> is<br />

largely understood in terms of pers<strong>on</strong>al<br />

choice. For example, when asked what strategies<br />

c<strong>on</strong>stitute self-determinati<strong>on</strong>, 91% of the<br />

educators in the sample identified choice<br />

making (Agran, Snow, & Swaner, 1999). Promoting<br />

choice making has become an important<br />

focus of disability services <strong>and</strong> supports<br />

<strong>and</strong> a basic comp<strong>on</strong>ent in service delivery<br />

(Wehmeyer, 2001), <strong>and</strong> serves as the foundati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

credo for many educati<strong>on</strong>al <strong>and</strong> human<br />

services (Bambara, 2004). It provokes self-determinati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> self-regulati<strong>on</strong> by allowing individuals<br />

with intellectual disabilities to express<br />

their preferences, make choices based<br />

<strong>on</strong> those preferences, <strong>and</strong>, subsequently, act<br />

<strong>on</strong> those choices. Since these acti<strong>on</strong>s have<br />

been historically denied to these individuals,<br />

choice making is thought now as a universal<br />

right <strong>and</strong> entitlement (Brown & Brown, 2009;<br />

Mithaug, 1998; Powers, 2005).<br />

That is, there is widespread agreement that<br />

people with disabilities have the right to choose<br />

<strong>and</strong>, <strong>on</strong> the basis of government policy, are<br />

entitled to this right (Brown & Brown). However,<br />

it should be noted that, although both<br />

employment <strong>and</strong> transiti<strong>on</strong> programs are directed<br />

to maximize active student or c<strong>on</strong>sumer<br />

involvement, choice making is regarded<br />

as a best practice rather than a legally<br />

m<strong>and</strong>ated right.<br />

For individuals with intellectual disabilities,<br />

choice making is clearly the self-directed<br />

learning strategy that has received the most<br />

attenti<strong>on</strong> in both the research literature <strong>and</strong><br />

in adult <strong>and</strong> support services. More than any<br />

other skill associated with self-determinati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

choice making has been a focal point in the<br />

self-advocacy <strong>and</strong> self-determinati<strong>on</strong> movement<br />

(Agran & Wehmeyer, 2003). As has often<br />

been the case, individuals with intellectual<br />

disabilities have not been provided opportunities<br />

to make choices <strong>and</strong> decisi<strong>on</strong>s based <strong>on</strong><br />

their own preferences (Stancliffe & Abery,<br />

1997; Wehmeyer & Metzler, 1995). As Bodgan<br />

(1996) noted, individuals with intellectual disabilities<br />

have always been told what to do,<br />

where to go, <strong>and</strong> what to do next so it is not<br />

surprising that many individuals do not know<br />

how to make choices <strong>and</strong> need targeted, systematic<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> this skill. C<strong>on</strong>sequently,<br />

much of the choice-making research has involved<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong>s that individuals with<br />

intellectual disabilities do have preferences<br />

<strong>and</strong> can be taught to make choices based <strong>on</strong><br />

these preferences (see Martin et al., 2003;<br />

Powers, 2005; Wehmeyer et al., 2007). However,<br />

the types of choice-making opportunities<br />

provided to c<strong>on</strong>sumers <strong>and</strong> the extent to<br />

which their choices are supported warrants<br />

further research.<br />

This paper examined the relati<strong>on</strong>ship of<br />

choice making to self-determinati<strong>on</strong>. Specifically,<br />

practices to promote choice making, engagement,<br />

<strong>and</strong> motivati<strong>on</strong> were presented.<br />

Also, the need to support employees’ choices<br />

was discussed, as well as barriers to choice<br />

making.<br />

Capacity <strong>and</strong> Opportunity<br />

It is well acknowledged that to ensure choice<br />

making is an integral comp<strong>on</strong>ent in work for<br />

individuals with intellectual <strong>and</strong> developmental<br />

disabilities, efforts must be made to infuse<br />

it into the daily work routine for individuals<br />

(Bambara, 2004; Brown & Brown, 2009). In<br />

<strong>on</strong>e study that directly asked c<strong>on</strong>sumers<br />

served by a variety of employment preparati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

supported employment, <strong>and</strong> sheltered<br />

employment programs their opini<strong>on</strong>s about<br />

choice making, 77% of individuals in the sample<br />

indicated that their job coaches wanted<br />

them to make choices every day, <strong>and</strong> 55%<br />

indicated that they made choices each day;<br />

twenty-two percent (22%) indicated that they<br />

didn’t, <strong>and</strong> 23% refrained from resp<strong>on</strong>ding<br />

(Agran, Storey, & Krupp, 2010). Although<br />

these figures are specific to the programs included<br />

in the sample, integrating choice making<br />

into work routines is increasingly being<br />

implemented, <strong>and</strong> we can assume that most<br />

employees are being provided frequent<br />

choice-making opportunities of <strong>on</strong>e sort or<br />

another at work (Bambara, 2004). It is clear<br />

that increasing choice-making opportunities<br />

will expose individuals to a pool of potentially<br />

rich choice opti<strong>on</strong>s to experience—for exam-<br />

566 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


ple, different work tasks <strong>and</strong> settings—<strong>and</strong>,<br />

thus, provide an experiential basis (familiarity)<br />

for them to make informed decisi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

However, Bambara noted that “more choices”<br />

does not necessarily mean “better choices,”<br />

<strong>and</strong> this is a critical issue that warrants c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

As indicated previously, choice making is<br />

arguably the self-directed learning strategy<br />

that has received the most attenti<strong>on</strong> in both<br />

the research literature <strong>and</strong> in adult <strong>and</strong> support<br />

services; in effect, choice making <strong>and</strong><br />

self-determinati<strong>on</strong> have become syn<strong>on</strong>ymous.<br />

More than any other skill associated with selfdeterminati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

choice making, <strong>and</strong> the right<br />

of people with disabilities to make choices, has<br />

been a focal point in the self-determinati<strong>on</strong><br />

movement (Agran & Wehmeyer, 2003). However,<br />

associated with this “notoriety” may be<br />

the mistaken belief that the act of choosing is<br />

sufficient in promoting self-determinati<strong>on</strong>; in<br />

other words, choice making in <strong>and</strong> of itself<br />

produces self-determinati<strong>on</strong> (Agran, Wehmeyer,<br />

& Krupp, 2009). Accordingly, service<br />

providers may opt to teach the former but<br />

refrain from teaching other self-directed strategies.<br />

For example, in the Agran et al. study in<br />

which c<strong>on</strong>sumers were asked to share their<br />

opini<strong>on</strong>s about choice making (as well as<br />

other self-determinati<strong>on</strong> strategies), less than<br />

20% of the resp<strong>on</strong>dents indicated that they<br />

were taught to self-reinforce (provide their<br />

own verbal reinforcement) or self-m<strong>on</strong>itor<br />

(count the number of times they performed a<br />

desired behavior). In another study that examined<br />

the extent to which individuals with<br />

intellectual disabilities, who were served in<br />

employment <strong>and</strong> community living programs,<br />

were taught different self-determinati<strong>on</strong> strategies,<br />

approximately half were taught to selfm<strong>on</strong>itor<br />

but less than half were taught to selfinstruct<br />

(verbally remind themselves of what<br />

they needed to do) or self-schedule (develop<br />

<strong>and</strong> follow a schedule of daily activities) (Wehmeyer,<br />

Agran, & Hughes, 2000). Wehmeyer<br />

(1998) commented that choice making is an<br />

important comp<strong>on</strong>ent of self-determinati<strong>on</strong><br />

but it is <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e of several comp<strong>on</strong>ents (e.g.,<br />

problem solving, goal setting, self-evaluati<strong>on</strong>).<br />

As Wehmeyer noted, our intent is not just to<br />

teach individuals to choose but to take c<strong>on</strong>trol<br />

over their lives. Expressing preferences <strong>and</strong><br />

making choices based <strong>on</strong> these preferences is<br />

a critical first step but it is just that. Choice<br />

making has a vital <strong>and</strong> integral role in promoting<br />

self-determinati<strong>on</strong> but it does not necessarily<br />

ensure it. An employee may choose to<br />

perform a certain work task with a preferred<br />

co-worker at a preferred time but this will not<br />

promote self-determinati<strong>on</strong> if other program<br />

comp<strong>on</strong>ents (e.g., goal setting, evaluati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

level of support) c<strong>on</strong>tinue to be c<strong>on</strong>trolled by<br />

the supervisor.<br />

Critical to self-determinati<strong>on</strong> is the dynamic<br />

relati<strong>on</strong>ship between capacity <strong>and</strong> opportunity<br />

(Mithaug, Mithaug, Agran, Martin, & Wehmeyer,<br />

2003). Capacity refers to an individual’s<br />

skills, interests, <strong>and</strong> motivati<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>and</strong><br />

opportunity is the existing situati<strong>on</strong> that will<br />

allow the individual to either realize his or her<br />

wishes (achieve his or her choices) or not<br />

fulfill them. When capacity <strong>and</strong> opportunity<br />

are in sync (i.e., the former is sufficient to<br />

benefit from the latter), an individual’s wishes<br />

can potentially be realized. However, life is of<br />

course replete with tensi<strong>on</strong>s in which capacity<br />

<strong>and</strong> opportunity are not compatible, <strong>and</strong> the<br />

extent to which we derive satisfacti<strong>on</strong> is based<br />

<strong>on</strong> our assessment as to whether we have the<br />

capacity to positively resp<strong>on</strong>d to <strong>and</strong> gain<br />

from a situati<strong>on</strong>, or if the situati<strong>on</strong> is <strong>on</strong>e that<br />

dem<strong>and</strong>s skills or resources that we d<strong>on</strong>’t have<br />

or <strong>on</strong>e that is not worth all the effort that may<br />

be required. Optimally, the value of providing<br />

individuals with opportunities to make<br />

choices is predicated <strong>on</strong> the belief that the<br />

choices individuals make are realistic (they<br />

can be fulfilled) <strong>and</strong> that supports are present<br />

so that such fulfillment can be achieved. The<br />

mere provisi<strong>on</strong> of choice making opportunities<br />

is of limited value if it is not accompanied<br />

by instructi<strong>on</strong> that allows individuals to assess<br />

if their choices are realizable—that is, as Mithaug<br />

et al. suggested, there is a “just-right”<br />

match between capacity <strong>and</strong> opportunity. If<br />

not, c<strong>on</strong>sumer expectati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> motivati<strong>on</strong><br />

will decrease <strong>and</strong> their self-determinati<strong>on</strong><br />

thwarted. As Brown <strong>and</strong> Brown (2009) noted,<br />

we cannot expect an individual to make an<br />

informed choice if he or she is not familiar<br />

with the stimuli available. Thus, broadening<br />

an individual’s array of choice-making opportunities<br />

is potentially of great value in facilitating<br />

such access, but providing these opportunities<br />

without assurance that the individual is<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sidering capacity <strong>and</strong> opportunity when<br />

Choice Making / 567


making choices is clearly not in the c<strong>on</strong>sumers’<br />

best interests. For many individuals with<br />

intellectual disabilities who have had few, if<br />

any, experience making choices, systematic instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

is necessitated, <strong>and</strong> the relati<strong>on</strong>ship<br />

between capacity <strong>and</strong> opportunity should be<br />

incorporated into it.<br />

Promoting Engagement <strong>and</strong> Motivati<strong>on</strong><br />

As Mithaug (1993) noted, engagement is a<br />

behavioral manifestati<strong>on</strong> of motivati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> reflects<br />

the observable level to which some<strong>on</strong>e is<br />

motivated. The degree to which people are<br />

engaged to reach a goal is directly related to<br />

the match between capacity <strong>and</strong> opportunity.<br />

When people are provided with choice situati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<strong>and</strong> they feel the decisi<strong>on</strong> will enable<br />

them to reach their goal, their levels of engagement<br />

<strong>and</strong> motivati<strong>on</strong> will rise commensurately.<br />

When c<strong>on</strong>sumers believe a circumstance<br />

offers a manageable <strong>and</strong> meaningful<br />

opportunity, they regulate their choices <strong>and</strong><br />

acti<strong>on</strong>s to arrive at the results they expect<br />

(Mithaug et al., 2003) <strong>and</strong> experience increased<br />

levels of engagement. These beliefs<br />

underscore the importance of matching c<strong>on</strong>sumers’<br />

capacities with manageable opportunities.<br />

By repeatedly striving to match capacity<br />

<strong>and</strong> opportunity within a real-life c<strong>on</strong>text for<br />

the c<strong>on</strong>sumer, individuals have multiple opportunities<br />

to practice making meaningful<br />

choices <strong>and</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>ding with engagement <strong>and</strong><br />

motivati<strong>on</strong> in the way they adjust to the c<strong>on</strong>sequences<br />

of those choices. Mithaug (1996)<br />

c<strong>on</strong>textualized the need for an accurate<br />

match between c<strong>on</strong>sumers’ capacities <strong>and</strong><br />

manageable opportunities in his asserti<strong>on</strong> that<br />

by “optimizing the match between individual<br />

capacity <strong>and</strong> social opportunity [people experience]<br />

more frequent expressi<strong>on</strong>s of self-determinati<strong>on</strong>”<br />

(p. 2). That is, individuals can<br />

begin to experience a level of pers<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>trol<br />

that they may not have experienced before.<br />

As suggested previously, because many c<strong>on</strong>sumers<br />

have not learned to make choices,<br />

their sense of self-efficacy will remain low <strong>and</strong>,<br />

as a result, they will c<strong>on</strong>tinue to be passive in<br />

their approaches to completing tasks; that is,<br />

others will c<strong>on</strong>tinue to make decisi<strong>on</strong>s for<br />

them. Instead of exercising skills in self-regulati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

including practicing making meaningful<br />

choices, they rely <strong>on</strong> others to make<br />

choices for them <strong>and</strong> to set goals for them.<br />

Rather than being engaged in reaching a goal,<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sumers take a passive role <strong>and</strong> fail to c<strong>on</strong>trol<br />

the situati<strong>on</strong>. This results in disengagement<br />

<strong>and</strong> lack of motivati<strong>on</strong>. Individuals in<br />

these passive states quickly acquire an approach<br />

of learned helplessness, <strong>and</strong> (a) believe<br />

they are helpless to c<strong>on</strong>trol the directi<strong>on</strong><br />

of their learning, <strong>and</strong> (b) perceive a limited<br />

number of choices available to deal with the<br />

situati<strong>on</strong> (Mithaug et al., 2003). Therefore,<br />

they may be likely to avoid being engaged in<br />

the task. For example, a c<strong>on</strong>sumer working in<br />

a public library is asked to reorganize a book<br />

secti<strong>on</strong> for a specific type of book (e.g., science-ficti<strong>on</strong>).<br />

The c<strong>on</strong>sumer, working independently,<br />

would be expected to make<br />

choices <strong>and</strong> adjustments as he or she began to<br />

accomplish this task. However, staff at the library<br />

(like so many other support pers<strong>on</strong>nel)<br />

may assume the choice-making role by determining<br />

for the c<strong>on</strong>sumer which task to do<br />

first, when to have it d<strong>on</strong>e, how to do it, what<br />

to do next, then reinforce the c<strong>on</strong>sumer when<br />

the job is completed, or direct the c<strong>on</strong>sumer<br />

to make needed correcti<strong>on</strong> (e.g., instructs the<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sumer in making necessary adjustments).<br />

After such experiences, the c<strong>on</strong>sumer quickly<br />

learns he or she has little say in what is going<br />

to happen, what is happening, <strong>and</strong> what will<br />

happen. There are no meaningful choices to<br />

make, <strong>and</strong>, therefore, little or no opportunity<br />

to be self-determined. In fact, there is no reas<strong>on</strong><br />

to be engaged or motivated to accomplish<br />

the task.<br />

There are ways of breaking this cycle of<br />

learned helplessness. Earlier, the point was<br />

raised that direct support pers<strong>on</strong>nel need to<br />

provide opportunities for c<strong>on</strong>sumers to practice<br />

making meaningful choices by matching<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sumers’ capacities with meaningful opportunities.<br />

Mithaug et al. (2003) described the<br />

foundati<strong>on</strong> that leads to engagement by stating<br />

that:<br />

[when c<strong>on</strong>sumers] believe a circumstance<br />

offers a valuable <strong>and</strong> manageable opportunity<br />

for gain, they engage it by regulating<br />

their expectati<strong>on</strong>s, choices, <strong>and</strong> acti<strong>on</strong>s to<br />

produce a result that yields a satisfactory<br />

change in circumstances—usually a gain toward<br />

some end. This in turn produces an<br />

experience of c<strong>on</strong>trol over the circum-<br />

568 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


stance that positively affects subsequent beliefs<br />

about the opportunities for gain in that<br />

situati<strong>on</strong> (p. 19).<br />

As c<strong>on</strong>sumers learn to make choices as a<br />

result of being provided those opportunities<br />

to exercise this c<strong>on</strong>trol, they more readily <strong>and</strong><br />

accurately learn to self-regulate <strong>and</strong> problem<br />

solve as a result of making an informed, meaningful<br />

choice. C<strong>on</strong>fidence <strong>and</strong> competence<br />

increase engagement, motivati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> persistence<br />

(Mithaug, 1993), <strong>and</strong> it is imperative<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sumers are provided with opportunities to<br />

practice becoming more self-determined in<br />

their lives.<br />

Mithaug (2005) suggested “before we can<br />

engage in our self-interested pursuits in life we<br />

must have an opportunity to do so, or at least<br />

believe we do” (p. 163), <strong>and</strong> that “opportunities<br />

[to make meaningful choices] will provoke<br />

the regulati<strong>on</strong> of choice making to satisfy<br />

a need or interest” (p. 164). The level of engagement<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strated by c<strong>on</strong>sumers is in<br />

direct relati<strong>on</strong> to the meaningful experiences<br />

they are provided to practice making informed,<br />

meaningful choices (Mithaug, 1993;<br />

Mithaug et al., 2003). As c<strong>on</strong>sumers come to<br />

realize what it means to have some c<strong>on</strong>trol<br />

over their ability to achieve a target goal, c<strong>on</strong>current<br />

with support pers<strong>on</strong>nel providing opportunities<br />

for them to gain more experience<br />

in making meaningful choices, they will dem<strong>on</strong>strate<br />

increased levels of motivati<strong>on</strong> because<br />

they are engaged in the process of selfregulating<br />

through adjustment, <strong>and</strong> are more<br />

likely to improve their abilities to engage in<br />

self-regulated problem solving to reach a goal<br />

state <strong>and</strong>, ultimately, achieve some degree of<br />

self-determined behavior.<br />

Choice Making <strong>and</strong> Job Matching<br />

As Stock, Davies, Secor, <strong>and</strong> Wehmeyer<br />

(2003) indicated, an underlying assumpti<strong>on</strong><br />

in service delivery for people with intellectual<br />

disabilities is that their career goals <strong>and</strong> work<br />

placements will be based <strong>on</strong> their preferences<br />

<strong>and</strong> interests. As they noted, the assumpti<strong>on</strong><br />

when providing rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> services is that<br />

services will be driven with an explicit emphasis<br />

<strong>on</strong> providing individuals with the tools to<br />

express preferences <strong>and</strong> make informed<br />

choices. That said, several researchers have<br />

suggested instead that people with intellectual<br />

disabilities have not been provided the opportunities<br />

to make choices <strong>and</strong> decisi<strong>on</strong>s based<br />

<strong>on</strong> their own preferences (Canella, O’Reilly,<br />

& Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, 2005; Wehmeyer et al., 2007) <strong>and</strong><br />

employment decisi<strong>on</strong>s c<strong>on</strong>tinue to be made<br />

by others (Stock et al.; Wehmeyer et al., 1998).<br />

The reas<strong>on</strong>s that individuals do not make<br />

choices based <strong>on</strong> individual preferences vary<br />

(e.g., lack of choice-making skills, lack of support),<br />

but such a lack of choice-making opportunities<br />

appears to be universal (Wehmeyer &<br />

Metzler, 1995). To c<strong>on</strong>tribute to the problem,<br />

traditi<strong>on</strong>al vocati<strong>on</strong>al assessments (e.g., aptitude<br />

measurement, work sample, job analyses)<br />

have not used individual work setting or<br />

job preferences as the basis for placement<br />

decisi<strong>on</strong>s. As a result, this often resulted in job<br />

dissatisfacti<strong>on</strong> or terminati<strong>on</strong> for employees.<br />

In c<strong>on</strong>trast, placement methods that use individual<br />

preferences appear to produce more<br />

successful outcomes, including: increased engagement,<br />

motivati<strong>on</strong>, job satisfacti<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong><br />

productivity (Martin, Mithaug, Oliphant,<br />

Husch, & Frazier, 2002; Martin et al., 2003).<br />

When assessing job preferences, Martin et<br />

al. (2002) recommended that individuals express<br />

their preferences regarding setting characteristics<br />

(e.g., quiet vs. loud workplace, work<br />

al<strong>on</strong>e vs. work with lots of people), as well as<br />

job task <strong>and</strong> activities. Individuals should be<br />

provided repeated opportunities to identify<br />

what they like, spend time observing specific<br />

jobs, evaluate their initial choices, then<br />

choose again making any needed adjustments<br />

based <strong>on</strong> what they learned from their visit to<br />

the job site. For instance, an employee initially<br />

may have chosen a job site with many employees<br />

as a characteristic preference, but after<br />

spending time at this job site she learned that<br />

she preferred a job with fewer employees. She<br />

then expressed this preference during the<br />

next assessment round. Employees may need<br />

to repeat this process until reliable task <strong>and</strong><br />

characteristics choices emerge.<br />

Prior to job site visits, for example, employees<br />

identify job characteristics they “think”<br />

they like. Afterward, their results are graphed<br />

to show cumulative choices made across time.<br />

After visiting several job sites representing<br />

many of the chosen characteristics, employees<br />

choose again while making any adjustments<br />

based <strong>on</strong> what they learned. The process is<br />

Choice Making / 569


epeated until stable choice patterns emerge.<br />

Martin et al. (2002) used this approach with<br />

more than 750 individuals with disabilities <strong>and</strong><br />

found that those who used the self-directed<br />

employment assessment process had statistically<br />

significant better job results than those<br />

who obtained a job selected by a support pers<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Interestingly, Martin et al. (2006) compared<br />

the vocati<strong>on</strong>al choices made by individuals<br />

with severe cognitive disabilities with those<br />

made by caregivers <strong>on</strong> their behalf. The<br />

choices made by the individuals with disabilities<br />

seldom matched those made by their caregivers.<br />

These findings regrettably were aligned<br />

with those reported by Stancliffe (2000) who<br />

reported that c<strong>on</strong>sumers who had a proxy<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>d for them to choice questi<strong>on</strong>s experienced<br />

fewer choice opportunities than c<strong>on</strong>sumers<br />

who presented their choices themselves.<br />

Similarly, Martin et al. (2002) reported<br />

that, although custodial jobs were the least<br />

frequently selected job type by c<strong>on</strong>sumers, it<br />

was the most frequently selected job type by<br />

practiti<strong>on</strong>ers, <strong>and</strong> clerical work, although the<br />

most frequently selected job by c<strong>on</strong>sumers,<br />

was the least frequently selected by practiti<strong>on</strong>ers.<br />

Martin et al. (2003) emphasized the<br />

need for sensitive <strong>and</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se procedures be<br />

used to ensure that c<strong>on</strong>sumer input is secured<br />

<strong>and</strong> employed. Although practiti<strong>on</strong>ers <strong>and</strong><br />

caregivers may be c<strong>on</strong>fident that they underst<strong>and</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>sumers’ needs <strong>and</strong> preferences, such<br />

beliefs may be presumptive <strong>and</strong> unfounded at<br />

best. As discussed later in this manuscript, this<br />

problem is exacerbated for c<strong>on</strong>sumers who<br />

are n<strong>on</strong>verbal <strong>and</strong> have communicati<strong>on</strong> challenges.<br />

Making Meaningful Choices<br />

Shevin <strong>and</strong> Klein (1984) defined choice as<br />

“the act of an individual’s selecti<strong>on</strong> of a preferred<br />

alternative from am<strong>on</strong>g several familiar<br />

opti<strong>on</strong>s” (p. 232). The fact that an informed<br />

choice requires an element of familiarity is<br />

critical to the act of making a valued choice<br />

<strong>and</strong> ties directly into the discussi<strong>on</strong> earlier<br />

about the need for support pers<strong>on</strong>nel to provide<br />

meaningful opportunities matched to<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sumers’ individual capacities. It is suggested<br />

that c<strong>on</strong>sumers’ preferences be solicited<br />

<strong>and</strong> identified, but these preferences<br />

need to be informed by experience (Storey,<br />

2005). Experience must be actively provided<br />

to c<strong>on</strong>sumers whenever possible. Indeed, Martin<br />

et al. (2003) suggested a structured<br />

method of identifying meaningful choices<br />

that includes (a) repeated opportunities to<br />

make choices, (b) asking the c<strong>on</strong>sumer his or<br />

herself about those choices, (c) c<strong>on</strong>ducting<br />

the inquiries over time, <strong>and</strong> (d) narrowing<br />

preference selecti<strong>on</strong>s down to a list of preferred<br />

<strong>and</strong> n<strong>on</strong>preferred items. By providing<br />

opportunities to identify <strong>and</strong> express preferences,<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sumers have the opportunities to<br />

engage in <strong>and</strong> gain experience in making<br />

meaningful choices. By gaining experience in<br />

making meaningful choices informed by experience<br />

<strong>and</strong> not simply expressing preferences,<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sumers can begin to develop skills<br />

that will allow them to engage in a self-regulated<br />

problem solving process that is the essence<br />

of self-determinati<strong>on</strong> (Mithaug, 2005).<br />

Choice Making Leading to Problem Solving<br />

The immediate benefit of providing choicemaking<br />

opportunities to c<strong>on</strong>sumers is that it<br />

allows them to select a preferred stimulus or<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> to <strong>on</strong>e that is least preferred. Such<br />

a manipulati<strong>on</strong> will potentially provide a reinforcing<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sequence <strong>and</strong> enhance engagement<br />

<strong>and</strong> motivati<strong>on</strong>. Desirable outcomes that<br />

may be achieved include greater productivity,<br />

more <strong>on</strong> task time, greater job satisfacti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> fewer behavioral episodes, am<strong>on</strong>g others.<br />

These are valuable outcomes but there is an<br />

ultimate outcome that hopefully will be<br />

achieved—the capacity to engage in self-regulated<br />

learning. Choice making is beneficial in<br />

that it provides c<strong>on</strong>sumers with a means to<br />

express a preference, but it is not specifically<br />

designed to teach individuals how to evaluate<br />

the relative “weight” or value of the choices<br />

they make; in other words, which choices allow<br />

them to make greater progress in achieving<br />

self-selected goals, or which choices provide<br />

more benefits (reduces the discrepancy<br />

between an actual state {what <strong>on</strong>e has} <strong>and</strong> a<br />

goal state {what <strong>on</strong>e wants}. Mithaug (1993)<br />

described this discrepancy between a goal<br />

state <strong>and</strong> an actual state as a motivating factor<br />

<strong>and</strong> that the desire to reduce this discrepancy<br />

is a key ingredient to making choices <strong>and</strong><br />

subsequent problem solving as a self-deter-<br />

570 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


mined pers<strong>on</strong>. The diss<strong>on</strong>ance created by the<br />

goal-state/actual-state discrepancy will hopefully<br />

cause the pers<strong>on</strong> to engage in reducing<br />

the discrepancy. The effort to reduce this discrepancy<br />

results in a means-ends chain that<br />

allows a pers<strong>on</strong> to seek soluti<strong>on</strong>s using a selfregulated,<br />

problem-solving strategy that allow<br />

him or her to reach a goal. C<strong>on</strong>sequently, the<br />

“soluti<strong>on</strong> searching <strong>and</strong> soluti<strong>on</strong> testing lead<br />

to c<strong>on</strong>sequences that feed back <strong>and</strong> alter subsequent<br />

discrepancies, expectati<strong>on</strong>s, searches,<br />

<strong>and</strong> selecti<strong>on</strong>s” (p. 12). It is a loop of choosing,<br />

acting, evaluating, <strong>and</strong> adjusting. C<strong>on</strong>sequently,<br />

although choice making <strong>and</strong> problem<br />

solving are typically not associated, it is<br />

clear that the latter operati<strong>on</strong> (problem solving)<br />

can be achieved <strong>on</strong>ly after the former<br />

operati<strong>on</strong> (choice making) has been fully experienced<br />

by c<strong>on</strong>sumers.<br />

Agran <strong>and</strong> Hughes (1997) described problem<br />

solving as a process of “determining the<br />

most appropriate <strong>and</strong> efficient resp<strong>on</strong>se to a<br />

given problem” (p. 172). People experience a<br />

discrepancy (the problem), c<strong>on</strong>sider <strong>and</strong> implement<br />

their soluti<strong>on</strong>, evaluate, <strong>and</strong> adjust as<br />

much as necessary to reach the goal state.<br />

Unfortunately, as c<strong>on</strong>sumers have historically<br />

received little exposure to making meaningful<br />

choices, <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sequently little practice at<br />

solving problems, they are not able to regulate<br />

their lives as independently as they could. One<br />

cannot select an efficient resp<strong>on</strong>se to a problem<br />

(or achieve a goal) unless <strong>on</strong>e has familiarity<br />

with the various c<strong>on</strong>sequences that may<br />

be experienced (e.g., greater productivity<br />

leads to more financial compensati<strong>on</strong>, which<br />

provides more goods <strong>and</strong> services). The first<br />

step in problem solving is identifying the<br />

problem or stating a goal (achieving an outcome).<br />

This is similar to choice making—what<br />

choice should be made. The next step that<br />

follows is determining the relative weight or<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sequences of choices—if I opt for Choice<br />

A, this will happen; if I opt for Choice B, this<br />

will happen. With this underst<strong>and</strong>ing individuals<br />

can determine which acti<strong>on</strong>s are in their<br />

best interests—in effect, problem solve. It is<br />

str<strong>on</strong>gly recommended that c<strong>on</strong>sumers are<br />

not <strong>on</strong>ly taught to choose <strong>on</strong>e stimulus over<br />

another but to also learn to evaluate the relative<br />

benefits <strong>and</strong> disadvantages of their<br />

choices. For example, if an employee asks to<br />

work at a fast-food restaurant, he or she may<br />

find him- or herself in a work envir<strong>on</strong>ment,<br />

which has characteristics he or she doesn’t<br />

prefer (e.g., many people, work st<strong>and</strong>ing up).<br />

By underst<strong>and</strong>ing the need for c<strong>on</strong>sumers to<br />

practice choice making <strong>and</strong> problem solving,<br />

support staff can provide opportunities to improve<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sumers’ capacity to successfully perform<br />

self-regulated problem solving <strong>and</strong> begin<br />

to take more ownership <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>trol over their<br />

lives. Self-determined individuals are aware of<br />

their needs <strong>and</strong> make decisi<strong>on</strong>s to meet those<br />

needs by setting goals, taking acti<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> adjusting<br />

through <strong>on</strong>going self-evaluati<strong>on</strong> in order<br />

to meet those goals. Without opportunities<br />

to make meaningful choices <strong>and</strong> practice<br />

the problem-solving process, c<strong>on</strong>sumers will<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tinue to be dependent <strong>on</strong> others, living in<br />

an other-determined state.<br />

Barriers to Choice Making<br />

Although well acknowledged as both a best<br />

practice <strong>and</strong> service requirement, choice making<br />

represents a potentially disquieting event<br />

to service delivery <strong>and</strong> traditi<strong>on</strong>al practice,<br />

<strong>and</strong> may be difficult to provide to c<strong>on</strong>sumers<br />

in a meaningful way. As discussed previously,<br />

the impetus for choice making is to drive <strong>and</strong><br />

plan vocati<strong>on</strong>al <strong>and</strong> habilitati<strong>on</strong> programs<br />

based <strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sumer input, but there are several<br />

barriers that challenge both its purpose<br />

<strong>and</strong> impact <strong>on</strong> an individual’s lifestyle.<br />

No choice making. Agran et al. (2010) investigated<br />

the extent to which c<strong>on</strong>sumers with<br />

intellectual <strong>and</strong> severe intellectual disabilities<br />

were provided choice-making opportunities<br />

<strong>and</strong> the types of choices they were asked to<br />

make. For the participants at <strong>on</strong>e agency that<br />

provided sheltered employment, they were<br />

asked <strong>on</strong>ce a year at their Individual Program<br />

Plan meetings if they wanted to work elsewhere<br />

such as a supported employment placement.<br />

However, this agency did not offer supported<br />

employment, so, although the<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sumers were asked to make a choice, in<br />

effect there was no “real” choice involved.<br />

Also, as Storey (2005) suggested, even though<br />

an adult service agency is committed to providing<br />

a full c<strong>on</strong>tinuum of work placements, it<br />

is unlikely that c<strong>on</strong>sumers will be able to<br />

choose any placement opti<strong>on</strong> since there may<br />

be limitati<strong>on</strong>s in the resources available; the<br />

agency may not have the capacity (i.e., staff,<br />

Choice Making / 571


transportati<strong>on</strong>, commitment) to provide this<br />

full range of services; <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sumers may be<br />

directed to certain job placements to keep the<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tinuum viable (keep all placement opti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

available). As Storey noted, such a situati<strong>on</strong><br />

provides an “illusi<strong>on</strong> of choice” in which a<br />

menu of services is theoretically possible, but<br />

might not be achievable due to budgetary or<br />

other c<strong>on</strong>straints. In both of these cases<br />

choices might have been provided but no real<br />

choice making occurred since c<strong>on</strong>sumers’<br />

preferences <strong>and</strong> choices were not supported--in<br />

short, the acti<strong>on</strong> of choice making occurred<br />

but no meaningful change occurred<br />

due to it. As Agran et al. (2010) noted, this<br />

practice is disingenuous <strong>and</strong> may suggest that<br />

choice making is an activity that must be provided<br />

to c<strong>on</strong>sumers but <strong>on</strong>e that has little or<br />

no influence <strong>on</strong> the nature or quality of their<br />

work experiences. As Brown, Bayer, <strong>and</strong><br />

Brown (1992) indicated, individuals with intellectual<br />

disabilities need to believe that<br />

change is possible before they engage in selfdirected<br />

acti<strong>on</strong>s to promote such change.<br />

Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, Ward (2005) suggested that<br />

service providers must be committed to provide<br />

a variety of choice opti<strong>on</strong>s c<strong>on</strong>sistent with<br />

an individual’s interests <strong>and</strong> preference. C<strong>on</strong>sequently,<br />

when given a choice that involves<br />

either engaging in an activity or not—“Do you<br />

want to go bowling or not?,” this really isn’t<br />

choice making, since no other opti<strong>on</strong>s are<br />

available. Far too enough such dichotomous<br />

choice-making opportunities are provided. As<br />

Bambara (2004) recommended, individuals<br />

should be exposed to opti<strong>on</strong>-rich envir<strong>on</strong>ments<br />

in which they will have ample opportunities<br />

to identify, evaluate, <strong>and</strong> experience the<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sequences of their expressed preferences<br />

<strong>and</strong> choices. Having the choice of taking what<br />

is offered or nothing at all has little instructive<br />

value <strong>and</strong> does little to enhance the quality of<br />

an individual’s lifestyle. This type of choice<br />

making is referred to as a “Hobs<strong>on</strong>’s Choice”<br />

(Brown, 2005) <strong>and</strong>, regrettably, may represent<br />

many of the choice-making situati<strong>on</strong>s provided<br />

to pers<strong>on</strong>s with disabilities. Ward suggested<br />

that choice making at a minimum<br />

should involve deciding which of two or more<br />

opti<strong>on</strong>s the individual wishes to engage in.<br />

Further, the magnitude or scope of the life<br />

choices c<strong>on</strong>sumers make warrant further<br />

study. The available research suggests that<br />

choices involving minor or routine changes<br />

(e.g., time for a coffee break) are offered far<br />

more frequently than choices that may have a<br />

major impact <strong>on</strong> an individual’s life (e.g.,<br />

change of job) (Burt<strong>on</strong> Smith, Morgan, &<br />

Davids<strong>on</strong>, 2005; Stancliffe & Wehmeyer,<br />

1995). For example, Agran et al. (2010) reported<br />

that more c<strong>on</strong>sumers in their sample<br />

were asked what they wanted for lunch or<br />

what they wanted to do after work than if they<br />

wanted another job, who they wanted as a<br />

support pers<strong>on</strong>, or who they wanted to work<br />

with it. It would appear that major life decisi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tinue to be made others <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sumers<br />

may be restricted to a small set of<br />

choice opti<strong>on</strong>s that will have little or no impact<br />

<strong>on</strong> their quality of life. In this respect<br />

pers<strong>on</strong>al choice <strong>and</strong> aut<strong>on</strong>omy are curtailed,<br />

<strong>and</strong> choice making is restricted to a relatively<br />

small pool of opportunities. Major work <strong>and</strong><br />

life decisi<strong>on</strong>s c<strong>on</strong>tinue to be externally c<strong>on</strong>trolled<br />

<strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sumers have a seemingly minor<br />

role in the directi<strong>on</strong> their lives take. C<strong>on</strong>sequently,<br />

the purpose of choice making—<br />

providing a means to express <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>trol<br />

<strong>on</strong>e’s life decisi<strong>on</strong>s—is greatly compromised.<br />

To correct this situati<strong>on</strong>, it is critical that we<br />

not <strong>on</strong>ly provide c<strong>on</strong>sumers with ample<br />

choice-making opportunities but that we endeavor<br />

to provide choices that are pers<strong>on</strong>ally<br />

meaningful <strong>and</strong> important.<br />

Interpreting Choices<br />

Choice making (<strong>and</strong> other forms of self-determinati<strong>on</strong>)<br />

is c<strong>on</strong>tingent <strong>on</strong> the assumpti<strong>on</strong><br />

that a c<strong>on</strong>sumer’s expressi<strong>on</strong> of a choice or<br />

wish is understood by service providers or support<br />

pers<strong>on</strong>nel. Accordingly, communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

competence is syn<strong>on</strong>ymous with self-determinati<strong>on</strong><br />

(Brown, Gothelf, Guess, & Lehr, 1998).<br />

However, as Brown et al. noted, c<strong>on</strong>sumers<br />

who have severe communicati<strong>on</strong> challenges<br />

may employ n<strong>on</strong>symbolic, idiosyncratic, inc<strong>on</strong>sistent,<br />

or self-selected modes of communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

to express their choices. C<strong>on</strong>sequently,<br />

choice making for these individuals<br />

presumes that the communicati<strong>on</strong> receivers<br />

(e.g., caregivers, support people) can interpret<br />

such communicati<strong>on</strong>. In this respect, selfdeterminati<strong>on</strong><br />

may be wholly dependent <strong>on</strong><br />

others <strong>and</strong> is meaningful <strong>on</strong>ly to the extent<br />

that the choice is correctly interpreted. Most<br />

572 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


importantly, we must be certain that the message<br />

received is the message intended. Typically<br />

choice making is observed when an individual<br />

verbally or physically selects <strong>on</strong>e<br />

stimulus (e.g., a specific support pers<strong>on</strong>) over<br />

another stimulus (e.g., another support pers<strong>on</strong>).<br />

For c<strong>on</strong>sumers who have communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

challenges, oftentimes their preferences<br />

<strong>and</strong> choices need to be inferred through such<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses as time engaged (e.g., more time<br />

more engaged, the more preferred the task),<br />

facial expressi<strong>on</strong>s (e.g., frown may suggest a<br />

n<strong>on</strong>-preferred task), or sound producti<strong>on</strong><br />

(e.g., a hum may suggest a preferred task).<br />

The resp<strong>on</strong>siveness of partners then becomes<br />

a factor critical in choice making <strong>and</strong> reminds<br />

us that choice making is not a single, isolated<br />

behavior but <strong>on</strong>e that involves reciprocal exchanges<br />

<strong>and</strong> envir<strong>on</strong>mental support (Bambara,<br />

2004).<br />

One method used to facilitate choice making<br />

for c<strong>on</strong>sumers who have communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

challenges is the use of proxies. The assumpti<strong>on</strong><br />

here is that a support pers<strong>on</strong> who knows<br />

a c<strong>on</strong>sumer well is in a good positi<strong>on</strong> to represent<br />

that pers<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> make choices believed<br />

to be in that individual’s best interests. However,<br />

as discussed above, this assumes that the<br />

proxy truly knows what the c<strong>on</strong>sumer’s preferences<br />

are. Additi<strong>on</strong>al studies have examined<br />

this relati<strong>on</strong>ship <strong>and</strong> found great discrepancies<br />

between the choices made by c<strong>on</strong>sumers<br />

<strong>and</strong> the choices made by support pers<strong>on</strong>nel<br />

<strong>on</strong> their behalf (Neeley-Barnes, Marcenko, &<br />

Weber, 2008; Stancliffe, 1995). For example,<br />

Martin et al. (2006) reported that there was<br />

<strong>on</strong>ly an 18–36% agreement between c<strong>on</strong>sumers<br />

<strong>and</strong> caregivers when asked to indicate<br />

their preferred settings (job sites), activities<br />

(work tasks), <strong>and</strong> characteristics (ecological<br />

features of the job sites). Neely-Barnes et al.<br />

reported that the more proxies, the fewer<br />

choices individuals made. Last, Stancliffe indicated<br />

that when the agreement between c<strong>on</strong>sumers<br />

self-reports (choices) was compared to<br />

third-party reports the correlati<strong>on</strong> when corrected<br />

for chance by kappa was uniformly low.<br />

In all, there appears to be a great discrepancy<br />

between support staff percepti<strong>on</strong>s of what<br />

they believe c<strong>on</strong>sumers desire <strong>and</strong> the opini<strong>on</strong>s<br />

of the c<strong>on</strong>sumers themselves. Hence, staff<br />

percepti<strong>on</strong>s of the self-determinati<strong>on</strong> needs of<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sumers may be markedly different than<br />

those expressed by c<strong>on</strong>sumers. Brown <strong>and</strong><br />

Brown (2009) suggested that proxy assessment<br />

is both invalid <strong>and</strong> challenging. For n<strong>on</strong>verbal<br />

individuals such procedures as using<br />

computer touch screens <strong>and</strong> microswitches<br />

have been suggested (Wehmeyer et al., 2007).<br />

Whatever methods we use it is obvious that we<br />

need to secure the opini<strong>on</strong>s of c<strong>on</strong>sumers to<br />

ensure that programs are indeed driven by<br />

their preferences, needs, <strong>and</strong> interests.<br />

Employment-Related Choices<br />

As noted previously, transiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> employment<br />

services are predicated <strong>on</strong> the assumpti<strong>on</strong> that<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sumers’ interests <strong>and</strong> preferences will drive<br />

skill development programs (improving c<strong>on</strong>sumer<br />

capacity) <strong>and</strong> job placement decisi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

(Martin et al., 2002). Asking an individual if he<br />

or she would like to work in a bowling alley<br />

when <strong>on</strong>e does not exist in the community<br />

achieves the goal of providing choices but not its<br />

purpose—matching jobs to an employee’s preferences<br />

<strong>and</strong> interests so that appropriate planning<br />

can occur. A sec<strong>on</strong>d <strong>and</strong> related assumpti<strong>on</strong><br />

is that the choices of c<strong>on</strong>sumers will be<br />

h<strong>on</strong>ored <strong>and</strong> are compatible with existent work<br />

routines. Choice-making opportunities need to<br />

be perceived by c<strong>on</strong>sumers as natural <strong>and</strong> integral<br />

to work routines if they are to promote<br />

aut<strong>on</strong>omy <strong>and</strong> independence.<br />

Choice-making opportunities are typically<br />

thought of as c<strong>on</strong>sumers choosing job types or<br />

preferred work tasks. With this informati<strong>on</strong><br />

support pers<strong>on</strong>nel can develop <strong>and</strong> deliver<br />

skills development programs accordingly.<br />

However, as West <strong>and</strong> Parent (1992) suggested,<br />

numerous other choice-making opportunities<br />

can be incorporated into employment<br />

programs. These include: selecting<br />

support pers<strong>on</strong>nel or service providers, proximity<br />

to preferred co-workers, training methods,<br />

adaptive devices, changing job resp<strong>on</strong>sibilities,<br />

or resigning from a current positi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, Martin et al. (2002) suggest that<br />

we take the next step <strong>and</strong> try to underst<strong>and</strong><br />

why c<strong>on</strong>sumers prefer certain jobs or work<br />

tasks. This is d<strong>on</strong>e by asking c<strong>on</strong>sumers if they<br />

prefer, for example, working al<strong>on</strong>e or with<br />

others, or if they prefer to work where speed is<br />

important or not. By obtaining input from<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sumers about preferred work characteristics,<br />

a better match between job <strong>and</strong> employee<br />

Choice Making / 573


can be made. Last, because c<strong>on</strong>sumers may<br />

not be able to initially select job type, work<br />

task, or characteristic preferences, asking c<strong>on</strong>sumers<br />

to identify preferences <strong>and</strong> make<br />

choices should be c<strong>on</strong>ducted several times until<br />

there is a c<strong>on</strong>sistent pattern in resp<strong>on</strong>ding<br />

(i.e., discernable preferences).<br />

Summary<br />

Choice making represents a critical step in<br />

facilitating c<strong>on</strong>sumers’ attainment of self-determinati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

It is believed that choice making<br />

initiates the self-determinati<strong>on</strong> process <strong>and</strong><br />

provokes greater self-regulati<strong>on</strong> by allowing<br />

individuals to make choices, act <strong>on</strong> these<br />

choices, <strong>and</strong> evaluate the resultant c<strong>on</strong>sequences.<br />

On the other h<strong>and</strong>, service providers<br />

may think of choice making as essentially another<br />

term for self-determinati<strong>on</strong>; that is, providing<br />

the former will produce the latter. In<br />

this respect choice making is thought of as an<br />

outcome rather than a means to achieve selfdeterminati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Rather, it was recommended<br />

in this paper that choice making initiates a<br />

process that involves self-regulated planning<br />

to achieve a desired outcome, a means to<br />

evaluate this acti<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> the applicati<strong>on</strong> of a<br />

further adjustment if satisfactory progress is<br />

not being made. The outcome of self-determinati<strong>on</strong><br />

is achieved when c<strong>on</strong>sumers perform<br />

self-determined behaviors. As menti<strong>on</strong>ed previously,<br />

for self-determinati<strong>on</strong> to be achieved<br />

we need to enhance an individual’s capacity<br />

<strong>and</strong> the opportunities we provide so that<br />

choices can match interests <strong>and</strong> preferences<br />

(Mithaug, 2005). It is critical that meaningful<br />

choices are naturally integrated into work routines<br />

<strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sumers are taught to perform a<br />

repertoire of self-determinati<strong>on</strong> strategies. By<br />

doing so, we will certainly ensure c<strong>on</strong>sumers’<br />

aut<strong>on</strong>omy <strong>and</strong> independence.<br />

References<br />

Agran, M., & Hughes, C. (1997). Problem solving.<br />

In M. Agran (Ed.), Student directed learning: Teaching<br />

self-determinati<strong>on</strong> skills (pp. 171–198). Pacific<br />

Grove: Brooks/Cole.<br />

Agran, M., Snow, K., & Swaner, J. (1999). Self-determinati<strong>on</strong>:<br />

Benefits, challenges, characteristics.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 34, 293–301.<br />

Agran, M., Storey, K., & Krupp, M. (2010). Choosing<br />

<strong>and</strong> choice making are not the same: Asking<br />

“what do you want for lunch?” is not self-determinati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Journal of Vocati<strong>on</strong>al Rehabilitati<strong>on</strong>, 33, 77–<br />

88.<br />

Agran, M., & Wehmeyer, M. (2003). Self-determinati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

In D. Ryndak & S. Alper (Eds.), Curriculum<br />

<strong>and</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong> for students with significant disabilities<br />

in inclusive settings (pp. 259–276). Needham<br />

Heights, Mass: Allyn <strong>and</strong> Bac<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Agran, M., Wehmeyer, M., & Krupp, M. (2009).<br />

Promoting self-regulated learning. In A. Mourad<br />

& J. de la Fuente Arias (Eds.), Internati<strong>on</strong>al perspectives<br />

<strong>on</strong> applying self-regulated learning in different<br />

settings. New York: Peter Lang Publishing.<br />

Bambara, L. M. (2004). Fostering choice-making<br />

skills: We’ve come a l<strong>on</strong>g way but still have a l<strong>on</strong>g<br />

way to go. Research <strong>and</strong> Practice for Pers<strong>on</strong>s with<br />

Severe <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 29, 169–171.<br />

Bodgan, A. (1996). C<strong>on</strong>sumer driven approach to<br />

organizati<strong>on</strong>al change. Journal of Vocati<strong>on</strong>al Rehabilitati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

6, 63–68.<br />

Brown, F. (2005, November). Hobs<strong>on</strong>’s choice. Paper<br />

presented at the 2005 TASH c<strong>on</strong>ference, Milwaukee.<br />

Brown, R. I., Bayer, M. B., & Brown, R. J. (1992).<br />

Empowerment <strong>and</strong> developmental h<strong>and</strong>icaps: Choice<br />

<strong>and</strong> quality of life. Tor<strong>on</strong>to, Canada: Captus Press.<br />

Brown, I., & Brown, R.M. (2009). Choice as an<br />

aspect of quality of life for people with intellectual<br />

disabilities. Journal of Policy <strong>and</strong> Practice in<br />

Intellectual <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 6, 11–18.<br />

Brown, F., Gothelf, C. R., Guess, D., & Lehr, D.<br />

(1998). Self-determinati<strong>on</strong> for individuals with<br />

the most severe disabilities: Moving bey<strong>on</strong>d chimera.<br />

Journal of the Associati<strong>on</strong> for Pers<strong>on</strong>s with Severe<br />

H<strong>and</strong>icaps, 23, 17–26.<br />

Burt<strong>on</strong> Smith, R., Morgan, M., & Davids<strong>on</strong>, J.<br />

(2005). Does the daily choice making of adults<br />

with intellectual disability meet the normalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

principle? Journal of Intellectaul & <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

Disability, 30, 226–235.<br />

Cannella, H. I., O’Reilly, M. F., & Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, G. E.<br />

(2005). Choice <strong>and</strong> preference assessment research<br />

with people with severe to profound developmental<br />

disabilities: A review of the research.<br />

Research in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 26, 1–15.<br />

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> self-determinati<strong>on</strong> in human behavior. New York:<br />

Plenum Press.<br />

Martin, J. E., Mithaug, D. E., Oliphant, J. H., Husch,<br />

J. V., & Frazier, E. S. (2002). Self-directed employment:<br />

A h<strong>and</strong>book for transiti<strong>on</strong> teachers <strong>and</strong> employment<br />

specialists. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.<br />

Martin, J. E., Mithaug, D. E., Husch, J. V., Frazier.<br />

E. S., & Marshall, L. H. (2003). The effects of<br />

optimal opportunities <strong>and</strong> adjustments <strong>on</strong> job<br />

choice of adults with severe disabilities. In D. E.<br />

Mithaug, D. Mithaug, M. Agran, J. E. Martin, & M.<br />

Wehmeyer (Eds.), Self-determined learning theory:<br />

574 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


Predicti<strong>on</strong>s, prescripti<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>and</strong> practice (pp. 188–<br />

205). Mahwah, N.J.: Erlbaum Publishers.<br />

Martin, J. E., Woods, L.L., Sylvester, L., & Gardner,<br />

J. E. (2006). A challenge to self-determinati<strong>on</strong>:<br />

Disagreement between the vocati<strong>on</strong>al choices<br />

made by individuals with severe disabilities <strong>and</strong><br />

their caregivers. Research <strong>and</strong> Practice for Pers<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with Severe <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 30, 41–43.<br />

Mithaug, D. E. (1993). Self-regulati<strong>on</strong> theory: How optimal<br />

adjustment maximizes gain. Westport, CT:<br />

Pager.<br />

Mithaug, D. E. (1996). Equal opportunity theory. L<strong>on</strong>d<strong>on</strong>:<br />

Sage.<br />

Mithaug, D.E. (1998). Your right, my obligati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Research <strong>and</strong> Practice for Pers<strong>on</strong>s with Severe <strong>Disabilities</strong>,<br />

23, 147–153.<br />

Mithaug, D. E. (2005). On persistent pursuits of<br />

self-interest. Research <strong>and</strong> Practice for Pers<strong>on</strong>s with<br />

Severe <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 30, 163–167.<br />

Mithaug, D. E., Mithaug, D. K., Agran, M., Martin,<br />

J. E., & Wehmeyer, M. (2003). Self-determined learning<br />

theory: C<strong>on</strong>structi<strong>on</strong>, verificati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> evaluati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum.<br />

Neely-Barnes, S., Marcenko, M., & Weber, L.<br />

(2008). Does choice influence quality of life for<br />

people with mild intellectual disabilities? Intellectual<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, <str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>, 12–26.<br />

Powers, L. (2005). Self-determinati<strong>on</strong> by individuals<br />

with severe disabilities: Limitati<strong>on</strong>s or excuses.<br />

Research <strong>and</strong> Practice for Pers<strong>on</strong>s with Severe <strong>Disabilities</strong>,<br />

30, 168–172.<br />

Schloss, P., Alper, S., & Jayne, D. (1993). Self-determinati<strong>on</strong><br />

for pers<strong>on</strong>s with disabilities: Choice,<br />

risk, <strong>and</strong> dignity. Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, 60, 215–225.<br />

Shevin, M., & Klein, N. K. (1984). The importance<br />

of choice making skills as an interventi<strong>on</strong> for<br />

problem behavior: A meta-analysis. Journal of Positive<br />

Behavior Interventi<strong>on</strong>s, 9, 159–166.<br />

Stancliffe, R. J. (1995). Assessing opportunities for<br />

choice making: A comparis<strong>on</strong> of self- <strong>and</strong> staff<br />

reports. American Journal <strong>on</strong> Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong>, 99,<br />

418–429.<br />

Stancliffe, R. J. (2000). Proxy resp<strong>on</strong>dents <strong>and</strong> quality<br />

of life. Evaluati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Program Planning, 23,<br />

89–93.<br />

Stancliffe, R. J., & Abery, B. H. (1997). L<strong>on</strong>gitudinal<br />

study of deinstituti<strong>on</strong>alizati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> the exercise of<br />

choice. Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong>, 35, 159–169.<br />

Stancliffe, R. J., & Wehmeyer, M. L. (1995). Variability<br />

in the variability of choice to adults with<br />

mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. Journal of Vocati<strong>on</strong>al Rehabilitati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

5, 319–328.<br />

Stock, S. E., Davies, D. K., Secor, R. R., & Wehmeyer,<br />

M. L. (2003). Self-directed career preference selecti<strong>on</strong><br />

for individuals with intellectual disabilities:<br />

Using computer technology to enhance selfdeterminati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Journal of Vocati<strong>on</strong>al Rehabilitati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

19, 95–103.<br />

Storey, K. (2005). Informed choice: The catch-22 of<br />

self-determinati<strong>on</strong>. Research <strong>and</strong> Practice for Pers<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with Severe <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 30, 232–234.<br />

Ward, M. (2005). An historical perspective of selfdeterminati<strong>on</strong><br />

in special educati<strong>on</strong>: Accomplishments<br />

<strong>and</strong> challenges. Research <strong>and</strong> Practice for<br />

Pers<strong>on</strong>s with Severe <strong>Disabilities</strong> (RPSD), 30, 108–112.<br />

Wehmeyer, M. (1998). Self-determinati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> individuals<br />

with significant disabilities: Examining<br />

meanings <strong>and</strong> misinterpretati<strong>on</strong>s. The Journal of<br />

the Associati<strong>on</strong> for Pers<strong>on</strong>s with Severe H<strong>and</strong>icaps, 23,<br />

5–16.<br />

Wehmeyer, M. L. (2001). Self-determinati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. In L. M. Glidden (Ed.), Internati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Review of Research in Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong><br />

(Vol. 24, pp. 1–48). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum<br />

Publishers.<br />

Wehmeyer, M. L. (2003). Self-determinati<strong>on</strong>, vocati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

rehabilitati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> workplace supports.<br />

Journal of Vocati<strong>on</strong>al Rehabilitati<strong>on</strong>, 19, 67–69.<br />

Wehmeyer, M., Agran, M., & Hughes, C. (1997).<br />

Teaching self-determinati<strong>on</strong> skills to students with disabilities:<br />

Basic skills for successful transiti<strong>on</strong>. Baltimore:<br />

Paul H. Brookes.<br />

Wehmeyer, M. L., Agran, M., & Hughes, C. (2000).<br />

A nati<strong>on</strong>al survey <strong>on</strong> teachers’ promoti<strong>on</strong> of selfdeterminati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> student-directed learning. The<br />

Journal of Special Educati<strong>on</strong>, 34, 58–68.<br />

Wehmeyer, M., Agran, M., Palmer, S., Mithaug, D.,<br />

Martin, J., & Hughes, C. (2007). Promoting selfdeterminati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> student-directed learning for students<br />

with intellectual <strong>and</strong> developmental disabilities.<br />

New York: Guilford.<br />

Wehmeyer, M. L., & Metzler, C. (1995) How selfdetermined<br />

are people with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>?<br />

The Nati<strong>on</strong>al C<strong>on</strong>sumer Survey. Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

33, 111–119.<br />

Wehmeyer, M. L., & Palmer, S. (2003). Adult outcomes<br />

for students with cognitive disabilities<br />

three years after high school: The impact of selfdeterminati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in Mental<br />

Retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>,<br />

38, 131–144.<br />

Wehmeyer, M., & Schwartz, M. (1997). Self-determinati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> positive adult outcomes: A follow-up<br />

study of youth with mental retardati<strong>on</strong> or<br />

learning disabilities. Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, 63, 245–<br />

255.<br />

West, M. D., & Parent, W. 1992). C<strong>on</strong>sumer choice<br />

<strong>and</strong> employment in supported employment. In P.<br />

Wehman, P. Sale, & W. Parent (Eds.), Supported<br />

employment: Strategies for integrati<strong>on</strong> of workers with<br />

disabilities (pp. 29–48. St<strong>on</strong>eham, MA: Andover<br />

Medical Publishers.<br />

Received: 24 August 2010<br />

Initial Acceptance: 21 October 2010<br />

Final Acceptance: 7 January 2011<br />

Choice Making / 575


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 2011, <str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>(4), 576-595<br />

© <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Review of the Choice <strong>and</strong> Preference Assessment Literature<br />

for Individuals with Severe to Profound <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Christopher A. Tullis, Helen I. Cannella-Mal<strong>on</strong>e, Abby R. Basbigill,<br />

Am<strong>and</strong>a Yeager, Courtney V. Fleming, Daniel Payne, <strong>and</strong> Pei-Fang Wu<br />

The Ohio State University<br />

Abstract: Since 2002, the body of literature examining choice interventi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> preference assessments for<br />

individuals with severe to profound disabilities has grown substantially. This paper is an extensi<strong>on</strong> of the<br />

Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, O’Reilly, & Emers<strong>on</strong> (1996) <strong>and</strong> Cannella, O’Reilly, & Lanci<strong>on</strong>i (2005) papers <strong>and</strong> reviews 50<br />

studies c<strong>on</strong>ducted between 2002 <strong>and</strong> 2010 that were divided into five categories: (a) effectiveness of various<br />

preference assessment formats <strong>and</strong> methodologies, (b) comparis<strong>on</strong>s of specific comp<strong>on</strong>ents of preference assessments,<br />

(c) underlying mechanisms of preference, (d) effects of choice <strong>on</strong> behavior, <strong>and</strong> (e) staff <strong>and</strong> participant<br />

training. Findings from these studies support previous research findings in that choice was an effective<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> for changing behavior, reinforcers were identified for individuals with severe to profound<br />

disabilities using preference assessments, <strong>and</strong> participants were taught to make, <strong>and</strong> staff were trained to<br />

provide, choices. Other major findings highlight some mechanisms that underlie preference <strong>and</strong> provide insight<br />

into some nuances of the various preference assessment methodologies. The findings are discussed in terms of<br />

their implicati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> practice related to individuals with severe to profound disabilities <strong>and</strong> suggesti<strong>on</strong>s for<br />

future research are provided.<br />

Research has dem<strong>on</strong>strated that individuals<br />

with severe to profound disabilities can dem<strong>on</strong>strate<br />

clear preferences through methodologically<br />

rigorous assessments <strong>and</strong> can make<br />

choices that have resulted in increased appropriate<br />

behavior <strong>and</strong> decreased challenging behavior<br />

(e.g., Cannella, O’Reilly, & Lanci<strong>on</strong>i,<br />

2005; Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, O’Reilly, & Emers<strong>on</strong>, 1996).<br />

Based <strong>on</strong> the available research, it appears<br />

that the provisi<strong>on</strong> of choice <strong>and</strong> identificati<strong>on</strong><br />

of preferred stimuli am<strong>on</strong>g individuals with<br />

severe to profound disabilities is increasing,<br />

thus potentially increasing their overall quality<br />

of life.<br />

Although there appears to be a clear evidence-base<br />

supporting the use of preference<br />

assessments to identify reinforcers <strong>and</strong> choice<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>s to change behavior, much of<br />

this research has been c<strong>on</strong>ducted by researchers<br />

rather than the direct-care providers (Cannella<br />

et al., 2005). Questi<strong>on</strong>s remain regard-<br />

Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence c<strong>on</strong>cerning this article should<br />

be addressed to Helen I. Cannella-Mal<strong>on</strong>e, A348<br />

PAES Building, 305 W 17th Avenue, Columbus, OH<br />

43210. Email: mal<strong>on</strong>e.175@osu.edu<br />

ing how these assessments <strong>and</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

should be implemented in home, school, <strong>and</strong><br />

work settings by parents, teachers, <strong>and</strong> other<br />

care providers. It is also unclear if <strong>on</strong>e method<br />

of assessing preference is more appropriate<br />

for a particular type of individual, how reinforcing<br />

items identified through a preference<br />

assessment might might be in various c<strong>on</strong>texts,<br />

or if choice provisi<strong>on</strong> is meaningful in<br />

all c<strong>on</strong>texts. To determine if these <strong>and</strong> other<br />

questi<strong>on</strong>s regarding choice interventi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong><br />

preference assessment methodologies have<br />

been addressed, this paper will review studies<br />

that have examined such interventi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong><br />

assessments with individuals who have severe<br />

to profound disabilities. It represents an extensi<strong>on</strong><br />

of the Lanci<strong>on</strong>i et al. (1996) <strong>and</strong> Cannella<br />

et al. (2005) papers as it includes studies<br />

published between 2002 <strong>and</strong> 2010. During<br />

this time period, 50 studies examined the effects<br />

of choice making <strong>and</strong> preference assessments<br />

for individuals with severe to profound<br />

disabilities.<br />

After providing an overview of the available<br />

research literature, this paper will discuss the<br />

literature in relati<strong>on</strong> to the following ques-<br />

576 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


ti<strong>on</strong>s: (a) Are choice interventi<strong>on</strong>s successful<br />

at reducing challenging behavior <strong>and</strong> increasing<br />

appropriate behaviors, <strong>and</strong> how have<br />

these interventi<strong>on</strong>s changed over time?, (b)<br />

How successful are preference assessments at<br />

identifying reinforcing stimuli, <strong>and</strong> how has<br />

their administrati<strong>on</strong> changed in recent years?,<br />

(c) How accessible are preference assessments<br />

<strong>and</strong> choice interventi<strong>on</strong>s to parents <strong>and</strong> staff<br />

who care <strong>and</strong> work for individuals with severe<br />

to profound disabilities?, <strong>and</strong> (d) What populati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

of individuals may benefit the most<br />

from preference assessment methodologies?<br />

Finally, potential lines for future research will<br />

be explored.<br />

Method<br />

Studies were included in this review based <strong>on</strong><br />

the following criteria: each study (a) was an<br />

assessment or interventi<strong>on</strong> study c<strong>on</strong>cerned<br />

with either choice or preference, (b) had at<br />

least <strong>on</strong>e participant with a severe to profound<br />

disability, <strong>and</strong> (c) was published between 2002<br />

<strong>and</strong> 2010. Studies in which preference assessments<br />

were used but were not the focus of the<br />

study were excluded (e.g., if a preference assessment<br />

was used to identify items used in<br />

m<strong>and</strong> training).<br />

Articles that did not meet the inclusi<strong>on</strong> criteria<br />

were excluded from the review. For example,<br />

studies were excluded if participants<br />

were not sufficiently described such that the<br />

reader could determine if the participants had<br />

a severe to profound disability. For example,<br />

Cicc<strong>on</strong>e, Graff, <strong>and</strong> Ahearn (2007) was excluded<br />

because participants were described as<br />

having a diagnosis of a developmental disability<br />

<strong>and</strong>/or autism, no other informati<strong>on</strong> was<br />

available to determine severity of the disability<br />

(e.g., limited communicati<strong>on</strong>).<br />

Electr<strong>on</strong>ic searches were c<strong>on</strong>ducted using<br />

ERIC, PsycINFO, MedLine, <strong>and</strong> Google<br />

Scholar. H<strong>and</strong> searches were also c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

using the reference secti<strong>on</strong>s of the articles<br />

identified through the electr<strong>on</strong>ic searches to<br />

identify a more complete set of articles. Sixtythree<br />

studies were identified <strong>and</strong> 50 studies<br />

met the inclusi<strong>on</strong> criteria. Two independent<br />

readers summarized <str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>% of the articles. Each<br />

summary included the reference, sample size,<br />

age of participants, stimuli used in the preference<br />

assessments, experimental design (if ap-<br />

plicable), type of preference assessment used,<br />

<strong>and</strong> the reported findings (i.e., positive, negative,<br />

or mixed). No major discrepancies were<br />

observed across readers (i.e., both readers<br />

noted the same informati<strong>on</strong> in the summaries).<br />

Studies included in this review were divided<br />

into five categories <strong>and</strong> are presented in Table<br />

1. Studies in the first category examined the<br />

effectiveness of various preference assessment<br />

formats <strong>and</strong> methodologies (e.g., Hanley,<br />

Iwata, Lindberg, & C<strong>on</strong>ners, 2003; O’Reilly,<br />

Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, & Sigafoos, 2004). Studies included<br />

in the sec<strong>on</strong>d category compared the effects<br />

of various preference assessment formats or<br />

comp<strong>on</strong>ents within a preference assessment<br />

(e.g., Horrocks & Morgan, 2009; Kodak,<br />

Fisher, Kelley, & Kisamore, 2009). Studies in<br />

the third category examined the underlying<br />

mechanisms of preference for individuals with<br />

severe to profound disabilities (e.g., DeLe<strong>on</strong>,<br />

Frank, Gregory, & Allman, 2009: Glover,<br />

Roane, Kadey, & Grow, 2008). Studies in the<br />

fourth category examined the effects of<br />

choice <strong>on</strong> both appropriate <strong>and</strong> challenging<br />

behavior (e.g., Carls<strong>on</strong>, Luiselli, Slyman, &<br />

Markowski, 2008; Hoch, McComas, Johns<strong>on</strong>,<br />

Far<strong>and</strong>a, & Guenther, 2002). Finally, studies<br />

in the fifth category examined methods for<br />

training staff to provide choices as well as<br />

teaching individuals with severe to profound<br />

disabilities to make choices (e.g., Clevenger &<br />

Graff, 2005; Roscoe, Fisher, Glover, & Volkert,<br />

2006).<br />

For each study, Table 1 reports the number<br />

of participants with severe to profound disabilities<br />

(n), their age, the stimuli provided (e.g.,<br />

tangible items), the experimental design used<br />

(where given), the preference assessment format,<br />

<strong>and</strong> the major findings. Eight preference<br />

assessment formats were examined in the<br />

studies reviewed <strong>and</strong> included: single stimulus<br />

(SS), paired choice (PC), multiple stimulus<br />

with replacement (MSW), multiple stimulus<br />

without replacement (MSWO), free operant<br />

(FO), resp<strong>on</strong>se restricti<strong>on</strong> (RR), c<strong>on</strong>current<br />

operant (CO), <strong>and</strong> questi<strong>on</strong>naires (Q). In the<br />

SS assessment, a single stimulus was presented<br />

to the participant. In the PC assessment, participants<br />

could choose <strong>on</strong>e of two opti<strong>on</strong>s. In<br />

the MSW <strong>and</strong> MSWO assessments, an array of<br />

stimuli were presented to the participant, who<br />

was allowed to choose <strong>on</strong>e item at a time from<br />

Review of Choice <strong>and</strong> Preference Assessment / 577


TABLE 1<br />

Studies Listed According to Categories with Number <strong>and</strong> Age of Participants (<strong>on</strong>ly those with a severe to profound disability), Experimental Design, Type of<br />

Stimuli Presented, Choice Format, <strong>and</strong> Results<br />

Stimuli Choice Format Results<br />

Category/Article Name n Age Design 12<br />

Assessing Preference<br />

Ahearn, Clark, DeBar, & Florentino. (2005). 2 11–13 ME Tangible items SS Positive<br />

On the role of preference in resp<strong>on</strong>se competiti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Cobigo, Morin, & Lachapelle. (2009). 19 23–58 N/A Work materials/ pictures PC Mixed<br />

A method to assess work task preferences.<br />

Didden, Korzilius, Kamphuis, Sturmey, Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, & Curfs. (2006). 105 1–45 N/A N/A Q Positive<br />

Preference in individuals with angelman syndrome assessed by a modified<br />

choice assessment scale.<br />

Didden, Korzilius, Sturmey, Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, & Curfs. (2008). 81 Mean Group Design N/A Q Positive<br />

Preference for water-related items in angelman syndrome, down<br />

age:<br />

syndrome, <strong>and</strong> n<strong>on</strong>-specific intellectual disability.<br />

12<br />

Dozier, Vollmer, Borrero, Borrero, Rapp, Bourett, & Guiterrez. (2007). 2 6, 14 Reversal w/ AT Access to baseline or CO Positive<br />

Assessment for behavioral treatment versus baseline c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

treatment c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

Fisher, Adelinis, Volkert, Keeney, Neidert, & Hovanetz. (2005). 2 13, 14 Reversal/AT Access to positive or CO Positive<br />

Assessing preferences for positive <strong>and</strong> negative reinforcement during<br />

negative reinforcement<br />

treatment of destructive behavior with functi<strong>on</strong>al communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

training.<br />

4 14–21 Reversal Tangible/edible items PC Positive<br />

Fleming, Wheeler, Cannella-Mal<strong>on</strong>e, Basbagill, Chung, & Graham-Day.<br />

(2010).<br />

An evaluati<strong>on</strong> of the use of eye gaze to measure preference of individuals<br />

with severe physical <strong>and</strong> developmental disabilities.<br />

578 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011<br />

Hanley, Iwata, Lindberg, & C<strong>on</strong>ners. (2003). 2 34–66 N/A Leisure activities RR Positive<br />

Resp<strong>on</strong>se-restricti<strong>on</strong> analysis: I. assessment of activity preferences. FO Positive<br />

Hanley, Piazza, Fisher, & Maglieri. (2005). 2 5–8 ME Tangible items CO Positive<br />

On the effectiveness of <strong>and</strong> preference for punishment <strong>and</strong> extincti<strong>on</strong><br />

Reversal w/ Access to attenti<strong>on</strong>, FCT, CO Positive<br />

comp<strong>on</strong>ents of functi<strong>on</strong>-based interventi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

ME<br />

punishment,<br />

reinforcement


TABLE 1 —(C<strong>on</strong>tinued)<br />

Studies Listed According to Categories with Number <strong>and</strong> Age of Participants (<strong>on</strong>ly those with a severe to profound disability), Experimental Design, Type of<br />

Stimuli Presented, Choice Format, <strong>and</strong> Results<br />

Stimuli Choice Format Results<br />

Category/Article Name n Age Design 12<br />

Keen & Pennell. (2010). 4 4–5 Reversal Leisure activities FO Mixed<br />

Evaluating an engagement-based preference assessment for children with<br />

autism.<br />

N/A Positive<br />

Kreiner & Flexer. (2009). 29 16–22 N/A Pictures of leisure<br />

Assessment of leisure preferences for students with severe developmental<br />

activities<br />

disabilities <strong>and</strong> communicati<strong>on</strong> difficulties<br />

Lattimore, Pars<strong>on</strong>s, & Reid. (2003). 5 26–38 N/A Work materials MSW Positive<br />

Assessing preferred work am<strong>on</strong>g adults with autism beginning supported<br />

jobs: Identificati<strong>on</strong> of c<strong>on</strong>stant <strong>and</strong> alternating preferences.<br />

Mechling & Moser. (2010). 1 11–13 N/A Video clips N/A Positive<br />

Video preference assessment of students with autism for watching self,<br />

adults, or peers.<br />

O’Reilly, Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, & Sigafoos. (2004). 1 5 Reversal Activities/toys PC Positive<br />

Using paired-choice assessment to identify variables maintaining sleep<br />

problems in a child with severe disabilities.<br />

10 32–52 AT Electr<strong>on</strong>ic devices CO Mixed<br />

Saunders, Saunders, Mulugeta, Henders<strong>on</strong>, Kedziorski, Hekker, & Wils<strong>on</strong>.<br />

(2005).<br />

A novel method for testing learning <strong>and</strong> preferences in people with<br />

minimal motor movement<br />

Smaby, MacD<strong>on</strong>ald, Ahearn, & Dube. (2007). 3 4–7 N/A Social c<strong>on</strong>sequences SS post EXT Positive<br />

Assessment protocol for identifying preferred social c<strong>on</strong>sequences.<br />

Smith, Bihm, Tavkar, & Sturmey. (2005). 9 Adults N/A N/A FO Positive<br />

Approach-avoidance <strong>and</strong> happiness indicators in natural envir<strong>on</strong>ments: a 4 7–9 N/A Staff members <strong>and</strong> tasks FO Positive<br />

preliminary analysis of the stimulus preference coding system<br />

Review of Choice <strong>and</strong> Preference Assessment / 579


TABLE 1—(C<strong>on</strong>tinued)<br />

Studies Listed According to Categories with Number <strong>and</strong> Age of Participants (<strong>on</strong>ly those with a severe to profound disability), Experimental Design, Type of<br />

Stimuli Presented, Choice Format, <strong>and</strong> Results<br />

Stimuli Choice Format Results<br />

Category/Article Name n Age Design 12<br />

Spevack, Yu, Lee, & Martin. (2006). 2 8, 12 Reversal Sensoryactivities SS Positive<br />

Sensitivity of passive approach during preference <strong>and</strong> reinforcer<br />

assessments for children with severe <strong>and</strong> profound intellectual<br />

disabilities <strong>and</strong> minimal movement<br />

Olfactory stimuli PC Mixed<br />

Wilder, Schadler, Higbee, Haymes, Bajagic, & Register. (2008). 2 13–38 Reversal w/<br />

Identificati<strong>on</strong> of olfactory stimuli as reinforcers in individuals with autism:<br />

ME<br />

A preliminary investigati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Preference Assessment Comparis<strong>on</strong>s<br />

Graff & Gibs<strong>on</strong>. (2003). 2 14–20 Reversal Edible items, pictures PC Positive<br />

Using pictures to assess reinforcers in individuals with developmental<br />

disabilities.<br />

Graff, Gibs<strong>on</strong>, & Gallatsatos. (2006). 4 14–15 Reversal w/ AT Tangible items/pictures PC Positive<br />

The impact of high- <strong>and</strong> low-preference stimuli <strong>on</strong> vocati<strong>on</strong>al <strong>and</strong><br />

academic performances of youths with severe disabilities.<br />

Groskreutz & Graff. (2009). 5 15–17 Reversal Edible items PC Positive<br />

Evaluating pictorial preference assessment: The effect of differential 4 15–17 Reversal w/ AT Edible items PC Mixed<br />

outcomes <strong>on</strong> preference assessment results.<br />

Horrocks & Morgan. (2009). 3 18–22 N/A Work materials PC vs. Positive<br />

Comparis<strong>on</strong> of a video-based assessment <strong>and</strong> a multiple stimulus to identify<br />

MSWO<br />

preferred jobs for individuals with significant intellectual disabilities.<br />

580 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011<br />

Kodak, Fisher, Kelley, & Kisamore. (2009). 4 3–10 Reversal Food/toys FO vs. MSW Mixed<br />

Comparing preference assessments: Selecti<strong>on</strong>- versus durati<strong>on</strong>-based<br />

preference assessment procedures.<br />

Reed, Luiselli, Magnus<strong>on</strong>, Fillers, Vieira, & Rue. (2009). 1 19.5 AT Edible Items PC vs. Positive<br />

A comparis<strong>on</strong> between traditi<strong>on</strong>al ec<strong>on</strong>omical <strong>and</strong> dem<strong>and</strong> curve analyses<br />

MSWO vs.<br />

of relative reinforcer efficacy in the validati<strong>on</strong> of preference assessment<br />

FO<br />

predicti<strong>on</strong>s.


TABLE 1—(C<strong>on</strong>tinued)<br />

Studies Listed According to Categories with Number <strong>and</strong> Age of Participants (<strong>on</strong>ly those with a severe to profound disability), Experimental Design, Type of<br />

Stimuli Presented, Choice Format, <strong>and</strong> Results<br />

Stimuli Choice Format Results<br />

Category/Article Name n Age Design 12<br />

Reid, Pars<strong>on</strong>s, Towery, Lattimore, Green, & Brackett. (2007). 12 29–76 N/A Work materials MSWO vs. Mixed<br />

PC vs. Q<br />

Identifying work preferences am<strong>on</strong>g supported worked s with severe<br />

disabilities: Efficiency <strong>and</strong> accuracy of a preference-assessment<br />

protocol.<br />

Thoms<strong>on</strong>, Czarnecki, Martin, Yu, & Martin. (2007). 11 Adults N/A Edible/tangible PC vs. SS Positive<br />

Predicting optimal preference assessment methods for individuals with<br />

developmental disabilities.<br />

Mechanisms Underlying Preference<br />

DeLe<strong>on</strong>, Frank, Gregory, & Allman. (2009). 1 9–20 N/A Leisure items PC Positive<br />

On the corresp<strong>on</strong>dence between preference assessment outcomes <strong>and</strong><br />

progressive-rati<strong>on</strong> schedule of assessments of stimulus value.<br />

Glover, Roane, Kadey, & Grow. (2008). 3 10–16 AT w/ CC Unspecified PC Positive<br />

Preference for reinforcers under progressive- <strong>and</strong> fixed-ratio schedules: A<br />

CC w/ Reversal Positive<br />

comparis<strong>on</strong> of single <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>current arrangements.<br />

Lee, Yu, Martin, & Martin. (2010). 2 35, 37 Reversal Edible items PC Positive<br />

On the relati<strong>on</strong> between reinforcer efficacy <strong>and</strong> preference.<br />

Kodak, Lerman, Volkert, & Trosclair. (2007). 1 4–8 ME/Reversal Edible/tangible items CO Positive<br />

Further examinati<strong>on</strong> of factors that influence preference for positive 1 Reversal Positive<br />

versus negative reinforcement.<br />

1 Reversal Positive<br />

Milo, Mace, & Nevin. (2010). 3 6–10 CC w/ Reversal Edible items CO Positive<br />

The effects of c<strong>on</strong>stant versus varied reinforcers <strong>on</strong> preference <strong>and</strong><br />

resistance to change.<br />

Review of Choice <strong>and</strong> Preference Assessment / 581


TABLE 1—(C<strong>on</strong>tinued)<br />

Studies Listed According to Categories with Number <strong>and</strong> Age of Participants (<strong>on</strong>ly those with a severe to profound disability), Experimental Design, Type of<br />

Stimuli Presented, Choice Format, <strong>and</strong> Results<br />

Stimuli Choice Format Results<br />

Category/Article Name n Age Design 12<br />

Effects of Choice<br />

Cannella-Mal<strong>on</strong>e, DeBar, & Sigafoos. (2009). 2 11–13 MB CC VOCA/picture exchange CO Positive<br />

An examinati<strong>on</strong> of preference for augmentative <strong>and</strong> alternative<br />

communicati<strong>on</strong> devices with two boys with significant intellectual<br />

disabilities.<br />

Carls<strong>on</strong>, Luiselli, Slyman, & Markowski. (2008). 2 3–15 MB Clothing PC Positive<br />

Choice-making as interventi<strong>on</strong> for public disrobing in children with<br />

developmental disabilities.<br />

Dix<strong>on</strong> & Falcomata. (2004). 1 31 N/A Not specified MSWO Positive<br />

Preference for progressive delays <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>current physical therapy<br />

exercise in an adult with acquired brain injury.<br />

Harding, Wacker, Berg, Barretto, & Lee. (2005). 2 2–5 CC w/ Reversal Toys/parent attenti<strong>on</strong> CO Positive<br />

An evaluati<strong>on</strong> of choice resp<strong>on</strong>ding during assessment <strong>and</strong> treatment of<br />

problem behavior.<br />

Harding, Wacker, Berg, Winborn-Kemmerer, & Lee. (2009). 2 4 CC w/ Reversal Toys/parent attenti<strong>on</strong> CO Positive<br />

Evaluati<strong>on</strong> of choice allocati<strong>on</strong> between positive <strong>and</strong> negative<br />

reinforcement during functi<strong>on</strong>al communicati<strong>on</strong> training with young<br />

children.<br />

Positive<br />

Positive<br />

Negative<br />

Leisure activities Paired vs.<br />

al<strong>on</strong>e<br />

Hoch, McComas, Johns<strong>on</strong>, Far<strong>and</strong>a, & Guenther. (2002). 3 9–11 AT or Reversal<br />

The effects of magnitude <strong>on</strong> choice <strong>and</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>ding during play activities.<br />

w/ MB<br />

582 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011<br />

Positive<br />

Humenik, Curran, Luiselli, & Child. (2008). 1 7 Reversal Food Choice vs.<br />

Interventi<strong>on</strong> for self-injury in a child with autism: effects of choice <strong>and</strong><br />

no choice<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tinuous access to preferred stimuli.<br />

Positive<br />

Reed, Pace, & Luiselli. (2009). 1 16 ME Tangible items Choice vs.<br />

An investigati<strong>on</strong> into the provisi<strong>on</strong> of choice in tangible c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s of a<br />

no choice<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>al analysis.


TABLE 1—(C<strong>on</strong>tinued)<br />

Studies Listed According to Categories with Number <strong>and</strong> Age of Participants (<strong>on</strong>ly those with a severe to profound disability), Experimental Design, Type of<br />

Stimuli Presented, Choice Format, <strong>and</strong> Results<br />

Stimuli Choice Format Results<br />

Category/Article Name n Age Design 12<br />

CO Negative<br />

Positive<br />

Positive<br />

CO Positive<br />

Positive<br />

Sigafoos, Green, Payne, S<strong>on</strong>, O’Reilly, & Lanci<strong>on</strong>i. (2009). 1 15 AT<br />

Speech generating<br />

A comparis<strong>on</strong> of picture exchange <strong>and</strong> speech-generating devices:<br />

AT device/picture exchange<br />

Acquisiti<strong>on</strong>, preference <strong>and</strong> effects <strong>on</strong> social interacti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Reversal<br />

(PE)<br />

Sigafoos, O’Reilly, Ganz, Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, & Schlosser. (2005). 2 12–13 MB VOCA 3<br />

Supporting self-determinati<strong>on</strong> in AAC interventi<strong>on</strong>s by assessing<br />

VOCA/PE<br />

preference for communicati<strong>on</strong> devices.<br />

S<strong>on</strong>, Sigafoos, O’Reilly, & Lanci<strong>on</strong>i. (2006). 3 3–4 AT VOCA/PE CO Positive<br />

Comparing two types of augmentative <strong>and</strong> alternative communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

systems for children with autism.<br />

Spevak, Martin, Hiebert, Yu, & Martin. (2005). 4 23–45 AT Work tasks N/A Negative<br />

Effects of choice of work tasks <strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>-task, aberrant, happiness, <strong>and</strong><br />

unhappiness behaviors of pers<strong>on</strong>s with developmental disabilities.<br />

Tasky, Rudrud, Schulze, & Rapp. (2008). 3 21–42 Reversal Work tasks N/A Positive<br />

Using choice to increase <strong>on</strong>-task behavior in individuals with traumatic<br />

brain injury.<br />

Teaching Individuals to Make Choices <strong>and</strong> Training Staff to Provide Choice<br />

Opportunities<br />

Clevenger & Graff. (2005). 6 9–16 Reversal Edible items/pictures PC Mixed<br />

Assessing object-to-picture <strong>and</strong> picture-to-object matching as prerequisite<br />

skills for pictorial preference assessments.<br />

Positive<br />

Positive<br />

Leisure items PC<br />

CO<br />

3 N/A N/A<br />

CC<br />

Machalicek, O’Reilly, Chan, Rispoli, Lang, Davis, Shogren, Sorrells,<br />

Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, Sigafoos, Green, & Langthorne. (2009).<br />

Using videoc<strong>on</strong>ferencing to support teachers to c<strong>on</strong>duct preference<br />

assessments with students with autism <strong>and</strong> developmental disabilities.<br />

(Participants were undergraduate pre-service teachers)<br />

Review of Choice <strong>and</strong> Preference Assessment / 583


TABLE 1—(C<strong>on</strong>tinued)<br />

Studies Listed According to Categories with Number <strong>and</strong> Age of Participants (<strong>on</strong>ly those with a severe to profound disability), Experimental Design, Type of<br />

Stimuli Presented, Choice Format, <strong>and</strong> Results<br />

Stimuli Choice Format Results<br />

Category/Article Name n Age Design 12<br />

Positive<br />

Reid, Green, & Pars<strong>on</strong>s. (2003). 2 N/A MP N/A Q, PC 4<br />

An outcome management program for extending advances in choice<br />

research into choice opportunities for supported workers with severe<br />

multiple disabilities. (Participants were job coaches)<br />

Roscoe & Fisher. (2008). N/A ME N/A MSWO/PC 4<br />

Positive<br />

Evaluati<strong>on</strong> of an efficient method for training staff to implement stimulus 8<br />

preference assessments.(Participants were clinic trainees)<br />

Positive<br />

Roscoe, Fisher, Glover, & Volkert. (2006). 4 N/A MB w/ AT N/A MSWO/PC 4<br />

Evaluating the relative effects of feedback <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tingent m<strong>on</strong>ey for staff<br />

training of stimulus preference assessments. (Participants were clinic<br />

trainees)<br />

1<br />

ME: multi-element; AT: alternating treatments; CC: c<strong>on</strong>current chains; MB: multiple baseline; MP: multiple probe.<br />

2<br />

For studies with multiple experiments, each experiment is presented separately as indicated by a split cell.<br />

3<br />

Voice output communicati<strong>on</strong> aid.<br />

4<br />

The choice formats presented represent the assessment methodologies <strong>on</strong> which the staff participants were trained in these training studies.<br />

584 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


the array; items were replaced in the array<br />

following a choice in the MSW <strong>and</strong> not replaced<br />

in the MSWO. In the FO assessment,<br />

items were arranged in an array <strong>and</strong> participants<br />

were allowed to engage with any or all<br />

items for the durati<strong>on</strong> of the assessment. In<br />

the RR assessment, sessi<strong>on</strong>s were similar to the<br />

FO assessment, but <strong>on</strong>ce an item was determined<br />

to be preferred, it was not l<strong>on</strong>ger included<br />

in the array of items. In the CO assessment,<br />

participants selected a treatment from<br />

an array; following a treatment sessi<strong>on</strong>, they<br />

were given the opportunity to choose what<br />

treatment they would receive in the next sessi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

In the Q assessment, the participants’<br />

parents <strong>and</strong>/or staff were asked what the participant<br />

preferred.<br />

The findings in Table 1 are classified as<br />

positive, negative, <strong>and</strong> mixed. In the first category,<br />

positive indicates that the study successfully<br />

identified a reinforcer hierarchy for all<br />

included participants, whereas mixed indicates<br />

that the study was successful in identifying a<br />

reinforcer hierarchy for some, but not all participants<br />

(Wilder et al., 2008). In the sec<strong>on</strong>d<br />

category, positive indicates that when two different<br />

preference assessment formats (e.g.,<br />

Horrocks & Morgan, 2009) were compared,<br />

the most effective reinforcers were identified<br />

by the same method for all participants. Mixed<br />

indicates that when different preference assessment<br />

formats or methodologies were compared,<br />

the most effective reinforcers were not<br />

identified by the same method for all participants.<br />

In the third category, positive is used to<br />

indicate that the high preference stimulus<br />

identified was the <strong>on</strong>e that maintained resp<strong>on</strong>ding<br />

under higher resp<strong>on</strong>se requirements<br />

(e.g., DeLe<strong>on</strong> et al., 2009). Mixed is<br />

used to describe results indicating that stability<br />

of preference was not maintained for all<br />

participants (e.g., Cobigo, Morin, &<br />

Lachapelle, 2009) or that some but not all<br />

participants had a preference for a particular<br />

reinforcement magnitude. In the forth category,<br />

positive refers to those studies in which<br />

the provisi<strong>on</strong> of choice led to changes in behavior<br />

(e.g., Carls<strong>on</strong> et al., 2008). Negative is<br />

used either when no change in behavior was<br />

found (e.g., Sigafoos et al., 2009) or when no<br />

difference in behavior was found between<br />

choice <strong>and</strong> no choice interventi<strong>on</strong>s (e.g., Spevack,<br />

Martin, Hiebert, Yu, & Martin, 2005). In<br />

the final category, positive indicates that staff<br />

(or participants) were successfully trained to<br />

provide (or make) a choice.<br />

Overview of Studies<br />

Assessing Preference<br />

Nineteen studies assessed the preferences of<br />

individuals with severe to profound disabilities<br />

(Ahearn, Clark, DeBar, & Florentino, 2005;<br />

Cobigo et al., 2009; Didden et al., 2006; Didden,<br />

Korzilius, Sturmey, Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, & Curfs,<br />

2008; Dozier et al., 2007; Fisher et al., 2005;<br />

Fleming et al., 2010; Hanley et al., 2003; Hanley,<br />

Piazza, Fisher, & Maglieri, 2005; Keen &<br />

Pennell, 2010; Kreiner & Flexer, 2009; Lattimore,<br />

Pars<strong>on</strong>s, & Reid, 2003; Mechling, &<br />

Moser, 2010; O’Reilly, Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, & Sigafoos,<br />

2004; Saunders et al., 2004; Smaby, MacD<strong>on</strong>ald,<br />

Ahearn, & Dube, 2007; Smith, Bihm, Tavkar,<br />

& Sturmey, 2005; Spevack, Yu, Lee, &<br />

Martin, 2006; Wilder et al., 2008). For example,<br />

Ahearn et al. used a single stimulus preference<br />

assessment to identify preferred stimuli<br />

that were presumed to match—<strong>and</strong> not<br />

match—the sensory properties of stereotypy<br />

for two adolescent participants diagnosed with<br />

autism <strong>and</strong> severe to profound mental retardati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Items identified as high preference<br />

were then provided, <strong>and</strong> the effects of both<br />

matched <strong>and</strong> unmatched stimuli <strong>on</strong> levels of<br />

stereotypy were assessed using a multi-element<br />

design. For both participants, significantly<br />

lower levels of stereotypy were observed when<br />

matched or unmatched items were presented.<br />

Items presumed to match the sensory properties<br />

of stereotypy did not produce lower levels<br />

relative to unmatched stimuli, suggesting that<br />

the activities that do not match properties of<br />

stereotypy could also displace resp<strong>on</strong>ding.<br />

Preference Assessment Comparis<strong>on</strong>s<br />

Eight studies compared the effects of various<br />

preference assessment formats <strong>and</strong> methodologies<br />

(Graff & Gibs<strong>on</strong>, 2003; Graff, Gibs<strong>on</strong>, &<br />

Galiasatos, 2006; Groskreutz & Graff, 2009;<br />

Horrocks & Morgan, 2009; Kodak, Fisher, Kelley,<br />

& Kisamore, 2009; Reed, Pace, & Luiselli,<br />

2009; Reid et al., 2007; Thoms<strong>on</strong>, Czarnecki,<br />

Martin, Yu, & Martin, 2007). For example,<br />

Kodak et al. compared the results of selecti<strong>on</strong>-<br />

Review of Choice <strong>and</strong> Preference Assessment / 585


(i.e., MSW) <strong>and</strong> durati<strong>on</strong>-based (i.e., FO)<br />

preference assessments to determine which<br />

method identified the item that produced the<br />

highest rates of resp<strong>on</strong>ding for four participants<br />

between the ages 2 <strong>and</strong> 10 with autism<br />

<strong>and</strong> other developmental disabilities. All participants<br />

engaged in limited communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e followed instructi<strong>on</strong>s requiring<br />

multiple steps. Following the preference assessments,<br />

reinforcer assessments were c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

comparing the high-preferred (HP)<br />

stimulus from each assessment. For two participants,<br />

the HP items identified with the<br />

MSW were more effective than the HP item<br />

identified with the FO. The remaining two<br />

participants engaged in undifferentiated resp<strong>on</strong>ding<br />

during the reinforcer assessments,<br />

<strong>and</strong> the HP items maintained resp<strong>on</strong>ding in<br />

reinforcer assessments.<br />

Mechanisms Underlying Preference<br />

Five studies evaluated the mechanisms underlying<br />

preference in individuals with severe to<br />

profound disabilities (DeLe<strong>on</strong> et al., 2009;<br />

Glover et al., 2008; Kodak, Lerman, Volkert, &<br />

Trosclair, 2007; Lee, Yu, Martin, & Martin,<br />

2010; Milo, Mace, & Nevin, 2010). For example,<br />

DeLe<strong>on</strong> et al. examined if items that differed<br />

in placement <strong>on</strong> a preference hierarchy<br />

would have different break points under a<br />

progressive ratio (PR) schedule of reinforcement<br />

with four participants with severe to profound<br />

disabilities. Paired choice preference<br />

assessments were c<strong>on</strong>ducted to identify a hierarchy<br />

of leisure items, followed by three PR<br />

analyses in r<strong>and</strong>om order. Prior to sessi<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

participants completed a known task three<br />

times, then they were given access to a high,<br />

medium, or low preference item. During PR<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s, the same item remained in view <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>on</strong>e verbal prompt to complete the task that<br />

was presented. The stimulus being assessed<br />

was delivered <strong>on</strong> a PR schedule beginning<br />

with <strong>on</strong>e resp<strong>on</strong>se (e.g., single completi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

two completi<strong>on</strong>s, etc.). The data indicated<br />

that participants completed more work when<br />

a more highly preferred stimulus was delivered<br />

<strong>and</strong> supports the c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> that items<br />

<strong>on</strong> different ends of a preference hierarchy<br />

may support different amounts of work.<br />

Effects of Choice<br />

Thirteen studies assessed the effects of choice<br />

<strong>on</strong> task engagement, challenging behavior,<br />

<strong>and</strong> the affect of individuals with severe to<br />

profound disabilities (Cannella-Mal<strong>on</strong>e, Debar,<br />

& Sigafoos, 2009; Carls<strong>on</strong> et al., 2008;<br />

Dix<strong>on</strong> & Falcomata, 2004; Harding, Wacker,<br />

Berg, Barretto, & Lee, 2005; Harding, Wacker,<br />

Berg, Winborn-Kemmerer, & Lee, 2009; Hoch<br />

et al., 2002; Humenik, Curran, Luiselli, &<br />

Child, 2008; Reed, Pace, & Luiselli, 2009; Sigafoos<br />

et al., 2009; Sigafoos, O’Reilly, Ganz,<br />

Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, & Schlosser, 2005; S<strong>on</strong>, Sigafoos,<br />

O’Reilly, & Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, 2006; Spevack et al.,<br />

2005; Tasky, Rudrud, Schulze & Rapp, 2008).<br />

For example, Tasky et al. evaluated the effects<br />

of choice <strong>on</strong> the task engagement of three<br />

women with traumatic brain injuries. For each<br />

participant, a list of tasks—requiring 10 to 15<br />

min to complete—designed to increase daily<br />

living skills was created. During baseline, participants<br />

were given tasks, asked to complete<br />

the tasks in the order presented, <strong>and</strong> return<br />

the completed list. In the choice c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

participants chose activities from a list <strong>and</strong><br />

were instructed to complete the list in any<br />

order. In the yoked no choice c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, the<br />

activities chosen in the previous choice c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong><br />

were assigned. Using a reversal design,<br />

the researchers found that all three participants<br />

engaged in higher levels of task engagement<br />

when they were given the opportunity to<br />

choose the task <strong>and</strong> completi<strong>on</strong> order.<br />

Teaching Individuals to Make Choices <strong>and</strong><br />

Training Staff to Provide Choice Opportunities<br />

Five studies focused <strong>on</strong> training staff to provide<br />

choice opportunities (Reid, Green, &<br />

Pars<strong>on</strong>s, 2003) <strong>and</strong> implement preference assessments<br />

(Machalicek et al., 2009; Roscoe &<br />

Fisher, 2008; Roscoe et al., 2006), or teaching<br />

individuals with severe to profound disabilities<br />

to make choices (Clevenger & Graff, 2005).<br />

For example, Reid et al. taught two job<br />

coaches to provide choice opportunities to<br />

five supported workers with severe to profound<br />

disabilities. Job coaches were trained to<br />

provide different types of choices (e.g., locati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

task), use different formats (e.g., openended,<br />

two-opti<strong>on</strong> choice), <strong>and</strong> deliver the<br />

stimuli or activities <strong>on</strong>ce the choice was made.<br />

586 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


After training, feedback was provided to the<br />

job coaches <strong>on</strong> their performance in relati<strong>on</strong><br />

to baseline. Results indicated that increased<br />

choice provisi<strong>on</strong> occurred <strong>and</strong> was maintained<br />

above baseline for <strong>on</strong>e-year for both<br />

job coaches. The supported workers resp<strong>on</strong>ded<br />

to 94% of all choices.<br />

In the study by Clevenger <strong>and</strong> Graff (2005),<br />

prerequisite skills for pictorial preference assessments<br />

were assessed for six participants<br />

between the ages of 9 <strong>and</strong> 16 with autism <strong>and</strong><br />

other developmental disabilities. In the first<br />

phase, picture to object <strong>and</strong> object to picture<br />

matching tests were c<strong>on</strong>ducted <strong>and</strong> three participants<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strated matching skills with<br />

both tests. The remaining three participants<br />

engaged in some matching behavior, but not<br />

at a level of mastery. Paired choice preference<br />

assessments were then c<strong>on</strong>ducted with both<br />

the tangible item <strong>and</strong> a pictorial representati<strong>on</strong><br />

of the item. After preference hierarchies<br />

were identified, reinforcer assessments were<br />

c<strong>on</strong>ducted. For the three participants with<br />

matching skills, items identified with both the<br />

tangible item <strong>and</strong> pictorial preference assessments<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>ed as reinforcers. Only the tangible<br />

item preference assessment was successful<br />

in identifying reinforcers for the<br />

participants without matching skills, suggesting<br />

that matching (object to picture or picture<br />

to object) may be a prerequisite skill for using<br />

pictures to assess preferences.<br />

Discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

Comments <strong>on</strong> the Overall Effectiveness of Choice<br />

Interventi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> Preference Assessment<br />

Methodologies<br />

As shown in Table 1, 14 of the 18 studies<br />

investigating various preference assessment<br />

methodologies <strong>and</strong> 10 of the 13 studies investigating<br />

the effects of choice <strong>on</strong> behavior reported<br />

positive results for the entire experiment.<br />

Overall, five studies reported mixed<br />

results <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>e reported clearly negative results.<br />

Four studies reported mixed or negative<br />

results for <strong>on</strong>e experiment within the investigati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

As in Cannella et al. (2005) a large<br />

number of studies reported positive results.<br />

The c<strong>on</strong>tinued replicati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> extensi<strong>on</strong> of<br />

previous research <strong>on</strong> choice <strong>and</strong> preference<br />

assessments with positive outcomes has<br />

strengthened the literature base. The studies<br />

reviewed support the use of preference assessment<br />

methodologies as an effective means of<br />

identifying preferred stimuli for individuals<br />

with severe to profound disabilities <strong>and</strong> using<br />

preferred stimuli as a comp<strong>on</strong>ent in effective<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Positive outcomes for the choice interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

reviewed in the current paper—in combinati<strong>on</strong><br />

with studies reviewed by Lanci<strong>on</strong>i et<br />

al. (1996) <strong>and</strong> Cannella et al. (2005)—support<br />

the c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> that choice interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

may be viewed as an evidence-based practice<br />

for individuals with severe to profound disabilities<br />

(Horner et al., 2005). Horner et al. outlined<br />

five st<strong>and</strong>ards that could be used to<br />

determine if an interventi<strong>on</strong> or practice was<br />

evidence based: (a) the practice is described<br />

so that replicati<strong>on</strong> is possible, (b) outcomes<br />

<strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>text associated with practice are defined,<br />

(c) a functi<strong>on</strong>al relati<strong>on</strong> is dem<strong>on</strong>strated,<br />

(d) experimental c<strong>on</strong>trol was dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

across a sufficient number of authors,<br />

settings, <strong>and</strong> participants, <strong>and</strong> (e) procedural<br />

integrity was documented <strong>and</strong> reported.<br />

First, the practice is described so that others<br />

may replicate the procedures. The c<strong>on</strong>tinued<br />

expansi<strong>on</strong> of literature <strong>on</strong> choice interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

in the past 14 years (cf., Cannella et al.,<br />

2005; Lanci<strong>on</strong>i et al., 1996; Table 1) supports<br />

the c<strong>on</strong>clusi<strong>on</strong> that procedures are clearly defined.<br />

For example Cannella-Mal<strong>on</strong>e et al.<br />

(2009) <strong>and</strong> S<strong>on</strong> et al. (2006) replicated the<br />

investigati<strong>on</strong> of preference for augmentative<br />

<strong>and</strong> alternative communicati<strong>on</strong> devices by Sigafoos<br />

et al. (2005).<br />

Sec<strong>on</strong>d, c<strong>on</strong>text <strong>and</strong> outcomes associated<br />

with practice are defined. Although it is impossible<br />

to define all potential c<strong>on</strong>texts <strong>and</strong><br />

outcomes, the present literature base c<strong>on</strong>tains<br />

a number of examples that may guide future<br />

research, such as participant age <strong>and</strong> the c<strong>on</strong>text<br />

where treatment occurs. The participant<br />

ages for the choice interventi<strong>on</strong>s included in<br />

the current review ranged from 2 to 40 years<br />

of age. Training c<strong>on</strong>texts included home<br />

(e.g., Harding et al., 2009), play (e.g., Hoch et<br />

al., 2002), <strong>and</strong> community settings (e.g., Carls<strong>on</strong><br />

et al., 2008). Similarly, diverse outcomes<br />

were present in the studies in the current<br />

review, including reducing challenging behavior<br />

(e.g., Carls<strong>on</strong> et al.; Humenik et al., 2008),<br />

increasing <strong>on</strong>-task behavior (e.g., Tasky et al.,<br />

Review of Choice <strong>and</strong> Preference Assessment / 587


2008), <strong>and</strong> increasing adaptive behavior (e.g.,<br />

Cannella-Mal<strong>on</strong>e et al., 2009; Harding et al.).<br />

Third, a functi<strong>on</strong>al relati<strong>on</strong> is dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

between the treatment <strong>and</strong> resulting change<br />

in the target behavior. Thirteen studies <strong>on</strong> the<br />

effects of choice interventi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> behavior are<br />

present across the current review (Cannella et<br />

al., 2005; Lanci<strong>on</strong>i et al., 1996) with positive<br />

results. Positive in all three reviews indicated<br />

that the provisi<strong>on</strong> of choice led to a change in<br />

behavior indicating a functi<strong>on</strong>al relati<strong>on</strong>. In<br />

the current review, 12 out of 13 studies implemented<br />

a single subject experimental design<br />

that dem<strong>on</strong>strated a functi<strong>on</strong>al relati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Three studies implemented a multiple baseline<br />

design (Cannella-Mal<strong>on</strong>e et al., 2009;<br />

Carls<strong>on</strong> et al., 2008; Sigafoos et al., 2005)<br />

where experimental c<strong>on</strong>trol is dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

when <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly when the interventi<strong>on</strong> is implemented.<br />

Four studies implemented a reversal<br />

design (Harding et al., 2005; Harding et<br />

al., 2009; Humenik et al., 2008; Tasky et al.,<br />

2008) in which experimental c<strong>on</strong>trol is dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

when a behavior change is observed<br />

when <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>ly when the interventi<strong>on</strong> is implemented,<br />

<strong>and</strong> when the interventi<strong>on</strong> is withdrawn,<br />

the target behavior returns to previously<br />

observed levels without treatment. Two<br />

studies implemented an alternating treatments<br />

design (S<strong>on</strong> et al., 2006; Spevack et al.,<br />

2005) <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>e implemented a multielement<br />

design (Reed et al., 2009). In these designs,<br />

experimental c<strong>on</strong>trol is dem<strong>on</strong>strated when<br />

<strong>on</strong>e data path increases or decreases to a<br />

greater degree than the other independent<br />

variables tested. Finally, two used multiple experimental<br />

designs (Hoch et al., 2002; Sigafoos<br />

et al., 2009) to dem<strong>on</strong>strate experimental<br />

c<strong>on</strong>trol.<br />

Fourth, experimental c<strong>on</strong>trol was dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

across a sufficient range of studies,<br />

researchers, <strong>and</strong> participants. In the current<br />

review, nine of the thirteen studies focusing<br />

<strong>on</strong> choice interventi<strong>on</strong>s did not have overlapping<br />

first authors. That is, the first author of a<br />

study did not appear again in the literature<br />

search as first author <strong>on</strong> another study. Of<br />

these nine studies, results were positive for<br />

eight <strong>and</strong> negative for <strong>on</strong>e. The eight studies<br />

with positive results included a total of 16<br />

individuals with various severe to profound<br />

disabilities, including traumatic brain injury<br />

(Tasky et al., 2008), autism (Carls<strong>on</strong> et al.,<br />

2008), <strong>and</strong> developmental disabilities (Sigafoos<br />

et al., 2009).<br />

Lastly, practices were not implemented with<br />

documented fidelity. Studies included in the<br />

current review did not include data <strong>on</strong> treatment<br />

integrity, limiting support for choice interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

to be c<strong>on</strong>sidered an evidencebased<br />

practice. Although a functi<strong>on</strong>al relati<strong>on</strong><br />

was dem<strong>on</strong>strated in the majority of studies—<br />

indicating the treatment was resp<strong>on</strong>sible for<br />

the behavior change—the extent to which the<br />

treatment was implemented as it was outlined<br />

should be viewed with some cauti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Horner et al. (2005) suggested a criteri<strong>on</strong><br />

of five single-subject studies with acceptable<br />

experimental c<strong>on</strong>trol, across at least three different<br />

researchers, <strong>and</strong> at least 20 participants<br />

across the five studies. Although treatment<br />

integrity data is lacking, the four other criteria<br />

have clearly been met, suggesting that choice<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>s can be c<strong>on</strong>sidered an evidencebased<br />

practice for individuals with severe to<br />

profound disabilities as defined by Horner et<br />

al.<br />

In additi<strong>on</strong> to the support for choice interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

to be c<strong>on</strong>sidered an evidence-based<br />

practice, the findings <strong>on</strong> preference assessments<br />

also have practical implicati<strong>on</strong>s for the<br />

assessment of preferred stimuli <strong>and</strong> the guidance<br />

of interventi<strong>on</strong>s to encourage more independence<br />

<strong>and</strong> involve the individual in<br />

treatment implementati<strong>on</strong>. For example,<br />

Hanley et al. (2005) assessed preference for<br />

treatment comp<strong>on</strong>ents in a functi<strong>on</strong>al communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

training (FCT) package using a<br />

c<strong>on</strong>current chains procedure. Participants<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strated a clear preference for the treatment<br />

package that included FCT <strong>and</strong> punishment<br />

over the package that c<strong>on</strong>sisted of FCT<br />

<strong>and</strong> extincti<strong>on</strong>. These data indicated that<br />

treatment selecti<strong>on</strong> may be guided empirically<br />

while still taking the individual’s treatment<br />

preference into account.<br />

One of the most significant areas where<br />

preference assessments <strong>and</strong> choice interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

may be useful is in implementing quality<br />

services for individuals with severe to profound<br />

disabilities. Cannella et al. (2005) suggested<br />

the use of preference assessments <strong>and</strong><br />

choice interventi<strong>on</strong>s as an aid to guide the IEP<br />

process <strong>and</strong> enhance pers<strong>on</strong>-centered planning.<br />

Current research, though still applicable<br />

to a younger student populati<strong>on</strong>, may also<br />

588 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


have implicati<strong>on</strong>s for supported work envir<strong>on</strong>ments.<br />

As students grow older, a greater<br />

emphasis may be placed <strong>on</strong> vocati<strong>on</strong>al opportunities<br />

<strong>and</strong> independence in these settings.<br />

In the current review, seven studies<br />

were c<strong>on</strong>ducted in vocati<strong>on</strong>al settings or focused<br />

<strong>on</strong> vocati<strong>on</strong>al tasks (Cobigo et al.,<br />

2009; Graff et al., 2006; Horrocks & Morgan,<br />

2009; Lattimore et al., 2003; Reid et al.,<br />

2007; Spevack et al., 2008; Tasky et al.,<br />

2008). For example, Lattimore et al. implemented<br />

multiple stimuli with replacement<br />

(MSW) preference assessments outside of<br />

the work c<strong>on</strong>text with five individuals to<br />

determine preference for specific work<br />

tasks. After preferences were identified, participants<br />

were given the choice of tasks to<br />

complete during their daily work routine.<br />

Data indicated that the MSW assessment was<br />

effective in identifying preferred work preferences<br />

for the participants. Increases in<br />

choice making were observed in participants<br />

who did not display stable work preferences,<br />

<strong>and</strong> stable choice making was observed in<br />

participants who displayed stable preferences<br />

during the MSW. These data suggest<br />

that including a choice comp<strong>on</strong>ent may enhance<br />

the vocati<strong>on</strong>al planning process for<br />

adults with severe to profound disabilities.<br />

Although a large number of investigati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

reported positive results, these results should<br />

be viewed with a degree of cauti<strong>on</strong>. Of the<br />

studies cited, 11 reported mixed findings overall<br />

or findings that were negative or mixed for<br />

at least <strong>on</strong>e experiment in the investigati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

For example, Sigafoos et al. (2009) reported<br />

positive findings in the final two phases, but<br />

negative findings in the first phase of the investigati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

These findings are important to<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sider when replicating procedures for future<br />

research as well as in clinical practice.<br />

First, the findings may indicate further areas<br />

of research into the implementati<strong>on</strong> of choice<br />

<strong>and</strong> preference assessment methodology. The<br />

failure of an assessment methodology to yield<br />

positive results may be an indicator of the<br />

need for procedural refinement. Sec<strong>on</strong>d,<br />

mixed results may be an indicati<strong>on</strong> that specific<br />

procedures are not appropriate for use<br />

with certain populati<strong>on</strong>s of individuals or under<br />

specific envir<strong>on</strong>mental c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Accessibility of Preference Assessments <strong>and</strong> Choice<br />

Interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

All four studies focusing <strong>on</strong> staff training reported<br />

clearly positive results, indicating the<br />

potential to successfully teach direct-care staff<br />

members to implement choice interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

(Reid et al., 2003) <strong>and</strong> preference assessments<br />

(Roscoe & Fisher, 2008; Roscoe et al., 2006;<br />

Machalicek et al., 2009). Although these studies<br />

reported positive results, a potential limitati<strong>on</strong><br />

in the training literature is the degree<br />

that these procedures may be effective in<br />

training parents or other care providers in<br />

more naturalistic envir<strong>on</strong>ments. Participants<br />

in all four studies were employed at the facility<br />

where the research was c<strong>on</strong>ducted or were<br />

supervised by the first author of the study, <strong>and</strong><br />

training for two studies (Roscoe & Fisher,<br />

2008; Roscoe et al.) was c<strong>on</strong>ducted outside of<br />

the natural c<strong>on</strong>text. Both the populati<strong>on</strong> (i.e.,<br />

therapists, job coaches) <strong>and</strong> the settings have<br />

the potential to limit the accessibility of choice<br />

<strong>and</strong> preference procedures to specific envir<strong>on</strong>ments<br />

<strong>and</strong> individuals for three studies<br />

(Reid et al.; Roscoe & Fisher, 2008; Roscoe et<br />

al.). In c<strong>on</strong>trast Machalicek et al. dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

a method of providing supervisi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

training in implementing preference assessment<br />

methodology via telec<strong>on</strong>ference. The<br />

use of technology allowed the researchers to<br />

assess skill acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of the participants (preservice<br />

teachers) in the natural c<strong>on</strong>text with<br />

their students.<br />

Although it is important for professi<strong>on</strong>als to<br />

know how to implement choice <strong>and</strong> preference<br />

methodologies, it may be even more useful<br />

to parents or other caregivers. Three articles<br />

that implemented choice interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

(Carls<strong>on</strong> et al., 2008; Harding et al., 2009;<br />

Humenik et al., 2008) <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>e preference<br />

assessment study (O’Reilly et al., 2004) included<br />

a parent, teacher, or other care provider<br />

as the interventi<strong>on</strong>ist. Carls<strong>on</strong> et al. implemented<br />

a choice interventi<strong>on</strong> to decrease<br />

urinary inc<strong>on</strong>tinence <strong>and</strong> public disrobing for<br />

two individuals. Teachers were trained to implement<br />

the interventi<strong>on</strong>, but data <strong>on</strong> their<br />

skill acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> treatment fidelity were<br />

not presented. Data indicated that the choice<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> decreased the occurrence of inc<strong>on</strong>tinence<br />

<strong>and</strong> disrobing for both participants.<br />

Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, the teachers did not re-<br />

Review of Choice <strong>and</strong> Preference Assessment / 589


port difficulty implementing the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

nor did the experimenters observe difficulties.<br />

These data—al<strong>on</strong>g with those discussed previously—are<br />

promising for implementing preference<br />

assessments <strong>and</strong> choice interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

across a wide range of caregivers <strong>and</strong> practiti<strong>on</strong>ers<br />

in varied settings.<br />

Comments <strong>on</strong> Complexity of Preference<br />

In recent years, there has been a shift from<br />

assessing preference <strong>and</strong> comparing methodologies<br />

to investigating the underlying mechanisms<br />

of preference. Specifically, the extent<br />

to which availability, magnitude, <strong>and</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

effort may impact the effectiveness of an item<br />

identified as being highly preferred. For example,<br />

Glover et al. (2008) evaluated PR<br />

schedules when two stimuli were evaluated<br />

under single <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>current arrangements<br />

<strong>and</strong> found that participants resp<strong>on</strong>ded more<br />

for high preference stimuli under PR schedules<br />

than low preference stimuli, regardless of<br />

the presentati<strong>on</strong> format. During single stimulus<br />

arrangements, low preference stimuli functi<strong>on</strong>ed<br />

as reinforcers for two participants.<br />

These results suggest that resp<strong>on</strong>ding for high<br />

preference stimuli was not disrupted by the<br />

c<strong>on</strong>current availability of a low preference<br />

stimulus <strong>and</strong> that a LP stimulus may be effective<br />

as a reinforcer under less effortful resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

requirements.<br />

These studies may have implicati<strong>on</strong>s for the<br />

applicati<strong>on</strong> of preference assessment methodologies<br />

in both clinical <strong>and</strong> classroom settings.<br />

Magnitude, availability, <strong>and</strong> persistence of a<br />

stimulus to functi<strong>on</strong> as a reinforcer may be<br />

manipulated to increase resp<strong>on</strong>ding for adaptive<br />

behavior or to decrease inappropriate behavior.<br />

For example, during an academic program,<br />

reinforcement may be altered <strong>on</strong> a<br />

given dimensi<strong>on</strong> depending <strong>on</strong> the task difficulty<br />

to increase the probability of task compliance.<br />

Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, stimulus availability may<br />

be structured in a manner to decrease competiti<strong>on</strong><br />

between two highly preferred items,<br />

which may increase the relative value of stimuli<br />

during instructi<strong>on</strong>al opportunities.<br />

C<strong>on</strong>cluding Comments <strong>and</strong> Future Research<br />

In this paper, studies focusing <strong>on</strong> preference<br />

assessment methodologies <strong>and</strong> choice inter-<br />

venti<strong>on</strong>s for individuals with severe to profound<br />

disabilities were summarized <strong>and</strong> discussed.<br />

Based <strong>on</strong> the results of these studies,<br />

research <strong>on</strong> choice is strengthening the evidence<br />

of its efficacy, <strong>and</strong> research <strong>on</strong> preference<br />

seems to be progressing toward a more<br />

complete descripti<strong>on</strong> of the mechanisms <strong>and</strong><br />

processes that underlie preference.<br />

The answers to the research questi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

posed at the beginning of this review are relatively<br />

clear, but should be further clarified.<br />

First, choice interventi<strong>on</strong>s c<strong>on</strong>tinue to be effective<br />

for reducing challenging behavior <strong>and</strong><br />

increasing appropriate behavior. Overall procedures<br />

have changed little. These procedures<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tinue to have implicati<strong>on</strong>s for enhancing<br />

programming in special educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> n<strong>on</strong>educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

settings (e.g., vocati<strong>on</strong>al settings,<br />

community, home, etc.).<br />

Sec<strong>on</strong>d, preference assessments c<strong>on</strong>tinue to<br />

be effective in identifying items that functi<strong>on</strong><br />

as reinforcers. The most comm<strong>on</strong> methods<br />

for assessing preference in the studies reviewed<br />

were the PC <strong>and</strong> MSWO. Twenty studies<br />

used a PC preference assessment <strong>and</strong> six<br />

included an MSWO. The type of stimuli used<br />

in these assessments has exp<strong>and</strong>ed bey<strong>on</strong>d<br />

edible <strong>and</strong> leisure items. For example, Wilder<br />

et al. (2008) assessed preference for olfactory<br />

stimuli with three adults diagnosed with autism<br />

<strong>and</strong> severe to profound mental retardati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

A PC preference assessment was implemented<br />

to determine a hierarchy of preferred<br />

smells. In the subsequent reinforcer assessments,<br />

all participants engaged in increased<br />

levels of resp<strong>on</strong>ding in relati<strong>on</strong> to baseline,<br />

indicating that the highly preferred stimuli<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>ed as reinforcers.<br />

Third, preference <strong>and</strong> choice technologies—though<br />

very effective—may be inaccessible<br />

to individuals who are not systematically<br />

trained. The research-base supports the implementati<strong>on</strong><br />

of choice- <strong>and</strong> preference-based<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>s to increase independence, decrease<br />

challenging behavior, <strong>and</strong> teach appropriate<br />

behavior, <strong>and</strong> that the available training<br />

protocols have been dem<strong>on</strong>strated to be effective<br />

(e.g., Roscoe & Fisher, 2008). One major<br />

limitati<strong>on</strong> of this literature is the absence of<br />

training procedures that have been shown to<br />

be effective in teaching caregivers specific procedures.<br />

Without easily implemented procedures<br />

or training programs, research findings<br />

590 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


may be of little use to caregivers or other<br />

individuals without specialized training.<br />

Lastly, preference <strong>and</strong> choice methodologies<br />

seem to be beneficial for a wide range of<br />

individuals with severe to profound disabilities.<br />

Implementati<strong>on</strong> of choice <strong>and</strong> preference<br />

technologies is effective for identifying<br />

various potentially reinforcing stimuli (e.g.,<br />

edible items, leisure items, smells). Less apparent<br />

are strategies that are more effective<br />

with specific populati<strong>on</strong>s or individual characteristics.<br />

The effectiveness of the most widely<br />

used preference assessments (i.e., PC &<br />

MSWO) may be influenced by each individual’s<br />

behavioral repertoire or physical ability.<br />

For example, if an individual has a physical<br />

impairment (e.g., cerebral palsy) an MSWO<br />

may be ineffective as a means of determining<br />

preference due to limited mobility, whereas a<br />

PC assessment using eye gaze may be more<br />

appropriate (Fleming et al., 2010). Clevenger<br />

<strong>and</strong> Graff (2005) dem<strong>on</strong>strated that pictorial<br />

preference assessments were more appropriate<br />

for individuals who engaged in object-topicture<br />

matching. The effectiveness of any<br />

preference assessment methodology to identify<br />

reinforcers may be impacted by individual<br />

differences such as behavioral repertoire <strong>and</strong><br />

physical ability.<br />

One method that may be promising is assessing<br />

prerequisite skills that may be required<br />

for an assessment to be applicable to a given<br />

populati<strong>on</strong> of individuals. Thoms<strong>on</strong> et al.<br />

(2007) tested the efficacy of SS <strong>and</strong> PC preference<br />

assessments with 11 individuals diagnosed<br />

with severe disabilities. In the preassessment<br />

phase, participants were assessed with<br />

the Assessment of Basic Learning Abilities<br />

(ABLA; Kerr, Meyers<strong>on</strong>, & Flora, 1977) to<br />

determine individual skill levels. PC <strong>and</strong> SS<br />

assessments were then c<strong>on</strong>ducted with the<br />

participants. The data indicated that either<br />

assessment was effective in identifying high<strong>and</strong><br />

low-preference items for individuals who<br />

had mastered two-choice discriminati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong><br />

the ABLA. Although these results are positive,<br />

Thoms<strong>on</strong> et al. did not c<strong>on</strong>duct reinforcer<br />

assessments to determine the predictive validity<br />

of the assessments. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally the authors<br />

did not assess the effectiveness of the preference<br />

assessments with individuals that did not<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strate mastery of two item discriminati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<strong>on</strong> the ABLA.<br />

With regard to future research, it may be<br />

especially relevant to determine if there are<br />

prerequisite skills that influence the results of<br />

specific preference assessment methodologies<br />

<strong>and</strong> resulting choice interventi<strong>on</strong>s or specific<br />

individuals who may benefit from <strong>on</strong>e type of<br />

assessment over another. No clear criteria exist<br />

to guide the use of <strong>on</strong>e preference assessment<br />

over another, to determine who would<br />

benefit from a specific type of preference assessment,<br />

how much of the stimulus should be<br />

available (e.g., durati<strong>on</strong>, magnitude), or what<br />

overall method is the most efficient <strong>and</strong> effective.<br />

Graff <strong>and</strong> Gibs<strong>on</strong> (2003) reported mixed<br />

results when assessing preferences using pictures<br />

of stimuli <strong>and</strong> similar results were reported<br />

by Cobigo et al. (2009). These results<br />

indicate the potential for a requisite skill set<br />

for this type of assessment to be accurate.<br />

Another area of future research is <strong>on</strong> the<br />

complexity of preference. For example, what<br />

magnitude of a preferred stimulus is most<br />

effective, or how durable are the results of a<br />

preference assessment? Further underst<strong>and</strong>ing<br />

of preference may be required for assessments<br />

<strong>and</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong>s based <strong>on</strong> choice to<br />

be effective in less c<strong>on</strong>trolled envir<strong>on</strong>ments<br />

(e.g., home or classroom). Current research<br />

supports the efficacy of using highly preferred<br />

items as reinforcers under lean schedules of<br />

reinforcement <strong>and</strong> that this effect is persistent<br />

in the presence of low preference stimuli<br />

(e.g., Glover et al., 2008). It is unknown if<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se allocati<strong>on</strong> in the presence of other<br />

highly preferred items or moderately preferred<br />

items would yield the same or similar<br />

results.<br />

Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, methods for increasing the effectiveness<br />

of low preference stimuli to functi<strong>on</strong><br />

as reinforcers may also be useful. Glover<br />

et al. (2008) dem<strong>on</strong>strated the reinforcing<br />

effectiveness of low preference items at a low<br />

schedule requirement. Further investigating<br />

the mechanisms that underlie preference may<br />

also shed light <strong>on</strong> methods of increasing preference<br />

measures for low preference items.<br />

This informati<strong>on</strong> may also be useful for increasing<br />

the effectiveness of lower preferred<br />

items to functi<strong>on</strong> as reinforcers (Hanley et al.,<br />

2003).<br />

New research may also seek to determine<br />

the extent to which magnitude influences<br />

preference for items. Altering the magnitude<br />

Review of Choice <strong>and</strong> Preference Assessment / 591


of a preferred item while c<strong>on</strong>tinuing to observe<br />

an increased resp<strong>on</strong>se rate may have a<br />

great impact <strong>on</strong> the use of choice interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

in n<strong>on</strong>-clinical settings. If a teacher, parent,<br />

or paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>al can deliver a small<br />

amount of a reinforcer <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tinue to observe<br />

persistent levels of a target behavior, the<br />

probability of that interventi<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tinuing<br />

may increase. The effects of differing magnitudes<br />

may also impact the l<strong>on</strong>g-term effectiveness<br />

of choice interventi<strong>on</strong>s to decrease challenging<br />

behavior across settings <strong>and</strong><br />

individuals. For example, magnitude of reinforcement<br />

may be increased or decreased depending<br />

<strong>on</strong> the probability of challenging behavior<br />

occurring in a specific envir<strong>on</strong>ment.<br />

Another area of future research that has<br />

vast practical implicati<strong>on</strong>s is training. Of the<br />

research currently reviewed, training participants<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sisted of professi<strong>on</strong>als who were required<br />

to implement specific procedures as<br />

part of their job. Future research should investigate<br />

training packages to determine what<br />

type of training is the most efficient in teaching<br />

parents or other n<strong>on</strong>-professi<strong>on</strong>als to implement<br />

choice interventi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> preference<br />

assessments. Further research in training professi<strong>on</strong>als<br />

is also warranted. Only three studies<br />

presented data <strong>on</strong> skill acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

implementers (Machalicek et al.,<br />

2009; Reid et al., 2007; Roscoe & Fisher, 2008;<br />

Roscoe et al., 2006). Future research should<br />

focus <strong>on</strong> what aspects of the training procedures<br />

were necessary <strong>and</strong> sufficient for skill<br />

acquisiti<strong>on</strong> to occur. These procedures should<br />

also be implemented with parents, paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>als,<br />

teachers, <strong>and</strong> other direct-care providers<br />

to a greater degree to further dem<strong>on</strong>strate<br />

efficacy.<br />

The current literature base c<strong>on</strong>tains support<br />

for choice interventi<strong>on</strong>s to be classified as<br />

an evidence-based practice for individuals<br />

with severe to profound disabilities. One limitati<strong>on</strong><br />

is the lack of data <strong>on</strong> treatment fidelity.<br />

Future researchers should present this data in<br />

order to provide more support for a functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

relati<strong>on</strong>. Documenting treatment fidelity<br />

in a c<strong>on</strong>trolled setting may also aid researchers<br />

in c<strong>on</strong>structing effective training<br />

programs for parents <strong>and</strong> other caregivers,<br />

which would allow for a higher degree of c<strong>on</strong>trol<br />

over training before implementing the<br />

procedures in a more naturalistic c<strong>on</strong>text.<br />

Informati<strong>on</strong> from the current review supports<br />

the efficacy <strong>and</strong> utility of preference<br />

assessment methodologies <strong>and</strong> choice interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for individuals with severe to profound<br />

disabilities <strong>and</strong> extends the findings of<br />

Cannella et al. (2005) <strong>and</strong> Lanci<strong>on</strong>i et al.<br />

(1996). Overall, the area of choice <strong>and</strong> preference<br />

research seems to be shifting toward a<br />

more complete explanati<strong>on</strong> of the mechanisms<br />

of preference, rather than primarily focusing<br />

<strong>on</strong> assessment. As choice <strong>and</strong> preference<br />

methodologies are refined, it will be<br />

important to collect data <strong>on</strong> the integrity of<br />

treatment implementati<strong>on</strong>, the generality of<br />

methods to natural c<strong>on</strong>texts, <strong>and</strong> the training<br />

of caregivers. Advancements in these areas are<br />

needed <strong>and</strong> would lend further support for<br />

choice <strong>and</strong> preference methodologies as evidence-based<br />

practice for individuals with severe<br />

to profound disabilities.<br />

References<br />

Ahearn, W. H., Clark, K. M., DeBar, R., & Florentino,<br />

C. (2005). On the role of preference in<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se competiti<strong>on</strong>. Journal of Applied Behavior<br />

Analysis, 38, 247–250.<br />

Cannella, H. I., O’Reilly, M. F., & Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, G. E.<br />

(2005). Choice <strong>and</strong> preference assessment research<br />

with people with severe to profound developmental<br />

disabilities: A review of the literature.<br />

Research in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 26, 1–15.<br />

Cannella-Mal<strong>on</strong>e, H. I., DeBar, R. M., & Sigafoos, J.<br />

(2009). An examinati<strong>on</strong> of preference for augmentative<br />

<strong>and</strong> alternative communicati<strong>on</strong> devices<br />

with two boys with significant intellectual disabilities.<br />

Augmentative <strong>and</strong> Alternative Communicati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

25, 262–273.<br />

Carls<strong>on</strong>, J. I., Luiselli, J. K., Slyman, A., &<br />

Markowski, A. (2008). Choice-making as an interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

for public disrobing in children with developmental<br />

disabilities. Journal of Positive Behavior<br />

Interventi<strong>on</strong>s, 10, 86–90.<br />

Cicc<strong>on</strong>e, F. J., Graff, R. B., & Ahearn, W. H. (2007).<br />

L<strong>on</strong>g-term stability of edible preferences in individuals<br />

with developmental disabilities. Behavioral<br />

Interventi<strong>on</strong>s, 22, 223–228.<br />

Clevenger, T. M., & Graff, R. B. (2005). Assessing<br />

object-to-picture <strong>and</strong> picture-to-object matching<br />

as prerequisite skills for pictoral preference assessments.<br />

Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38,<br />

543–547.<br />

Cobigo, V., Morin, D., & Lachapelle, Y. (2009). A<br />

method to assess work preferences. Educati<strong>on</strong> &<br />

Training in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 44, 561–572.<br />

DeLe<strong>on</strong>, I. G., Frank, M. A., Gregory, M. K., &<br />

592 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


Allman, M. J. (2009). On the corresp<strong>on</strong>dence<br />

between preference assessment outcomes <strong>and</strong><br />

progressive-ratio schedule assessment of stimulus<br />

value. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, 729–<br />

733.<br />

Didden, R., Korzilius, H., Kamphuis, A., Sturmey, P.,<br />

Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, G., & Curfs, L. M. G. (2006). Preferences<br />

in individuals with angelman syndrome assessed<br />

by a modified choice assessment scale. Journal<br />

of Intellectual Disability Research, 50, 54–60.<br />

Didden, R., Korzilius, H., Sturmey, P., Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, G.,<br />

& Curfs, L. M. G. (2008). Preference for waterrelated<br />

items in angelman syndrome, down syndrome,<br />

<strong>and</strong> n<strong>on</strong>-specific intellectual disability.<br />

Journal of Intellectual & <strong>Developmental</strong> Disability, 33,<br />

59–64.<br />

Dix<strong>on</strong>, M. R., & Falcomata, T. S. (2004). Preference<br />

for progressive delays <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>current physical<br />

therapy exercise in an adult with acquired brain<br />

injury. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 37, 101–<br />

105.<br />

Dozier, C. L., Vollmer, T. R., Borrero, J. C., Borrero,<br />

C. S., Rapp, J. T., Bourret, J., & Guiterrez, A.<br />

(2007). Assessment of preference for behavioral<br />

versus baseline c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s. Behavioral Interventi<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

22, 245–261.<br />

Fisher, W. W., Adelinis, J. D., Volkert, V. M., Keeney,<br />

K. M., Neidert, P. L., & Hovanetz, A. (2005).<br />

Assessing preferences for positive <strong>and</strong> negative<br />

reinforcement during treatment of destructive<br />

behavior with functi<strong>on</strong>al communicati<strong>on</strong> training.<br />

Research in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 26, 153–<br />

168.<br />

Fleming, C. V., Wheeler, G. M., Cannella-Mal<strong>on</strong>e,<br />

H. I., Basbaill, A. R., Chung, Y. C., & Graham-Day,<br />

K. (2010). An evaluati<strong>on</strong> of the use of eye gaze to<br />

measure preference of individuals with severe<br />

physical <strong>and</strong> developmental disabilities. <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

Neurorehabilitati<strong>on</strong>, 13, 266–275.<br />

Glover, A. C., Roane, H. S., Kadey, H. J., & Grow,<br />

L. L. (2008). Preference for reinforcers under<br />

progressive- <strong>and</strong> fixed-ratio schedules: A comparis<strong>on</strong><br />

of single <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>current arrangements. Journal<br />

of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41, 163–176.<br />

Graff, R. B., & Gibs<strong>on</strong>, L. (2003). Using pictures to<br />

assess reinforcers in individuals with developmental<br />

disabilities. Behavior Modificati<strong>on</strong>, 27, 470–483.<br />

Graff, R. B., Gibs<strong>on</strong>, L., & Galiasatos, G. T. (2006).<br />

The impact of high- <strong>and</strong> low-preference stimuli<br />

<strong>on</strong> vocati<strong>on</strong>al <strong>and</strong> academic performances of<br />

youths with severe disabilities. Journal of Applied<br />

Behavior Analysis, 39, 131–135.<br />

Groskreutz, M. P., & Graff, R. B. (2009). Evaluating<br />

pictorial preference assessment: The effect of differential<br />

outcomes <strong>on</strong> preference assessment results.<br />

Research in <strong>Autism</strong> Spectrum Disorders, 3, 113–<br />

128.<br />

Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., Lindberg, J. S., & C<strong>on</strong>-<br />

ners, J. (2003). Resp<strong>on</strong>se-restricti<strong>on</strong> analysis: I.<br />

assessment of activity preferences. Journal of Applied<br />

Behavior Analysis, 36, 47–58.<br />

Hanley, G. P., Piazza, C. C., Fisher, W. W., &<br />

Maglieri, K. A. (2005). On the effectiveness of<br />

<strong>and</strong> preference for punishment <strong>and</strong> extincti<strong>on</strong><br />

comp<strong>on</strong>ents of functi<strong>on</strong>-based interventi<strong>on</strong>s. Journal<br />

of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38, 51–65.<br />

Harding, J. W., Wacker, D. P., Berg, W. K., Barretto,<br />

A., & Lee, J. F. (2005). An evaluati<strong>on</strong> of choice<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ding during assessment <strong>and</strong> treatment of<br />

problem behavior. European Journal of Behavior<br />

Analysis, 6, 145–164.<br />

Harding, J. W., Wacker, D. P., Berg, W. K., Winborn-<br />

Kemmerer, L., & Lee, J. F. (2009). Evaluati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

choice allocati<strong>on</strong> between positive <strong>and</strong> negative<br />

reinforcement during functi<strong>on</strong>al communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

training with young children. Journal of <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Physical <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 21, 443–456.<br />

Hoch, H., McComas, J. J., Johns<strong>on</strong>, L., Far<strong>and</strong>a, N.,<br />

Guenther, S. L. (2002). The effects of magnitude<br />

<strong>and</strong> quality of reinforcement <strong>on</strong> choice resp<strong>on</strong>ding<br />

during play activities. Journal of Applied Behavior<br />

Analysis, 35, 171–181.<br />

Horner, R. H., Carr, E. G., Halle, J., McGee, G.,<br />

Odom, S., & Wolery, M. (2005). The use of singlesubject<br />

research to identify evidence-based practice<br />

in special educati<strong>on</strong>. Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, 71,<br />

165–179.<br />

Horrocks, E. L., & Morgan, R. L. (2009). Comparis<strong>on</strong><br />

of a video-based assessment <strong>and</strong> a multiple<br />

stimulus assessment to identify preferred jobs for<br />

individuals with significant intellectual disabilities.<br />

Research in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 30, 902–<br />

909.<br />

Humenik, A., Curran, J., Luiselli, J. K., & Child, S. N.<br />

(2008). Interventi<strong>on</strong> for self-injury in a child with<br />

autism: Effects of choice <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tinuous access to<br />

preferred stimuli. Behavioral Development Bulletin,<br />

14, 17–22.<br />

Keen, D., & Pennell, D. (2010). Evaluating <strong>and</strong><br />

engagement-based preference assessment for<br />

children with autism. Research in <strong>Autism</strong> Spectrum<br />

Disorders, 4, 645–652.<br />

Kerr, N., Meyers<strong>on</strong>, L., & Flora, J., (1977). The<br />

measurement of motor, visual <strong>and</strong> auditory discriminati<strong>on</strong><br />

skills. Rehabilitati<strong>on</strong> Psychology, 24, 95–<br />

112.<br />

Kodak, T., Fisher, W. W., Kelley, M. E., & Kisamore,<br />

A. (2009). Comparing preference assessments:<br />

Selecti<strong>on</strong>- versus durati<strong>on</strong>- based preference assessment<br />

procedures. Research in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>,<br />

30, 1068–1077.<br />

Kodak, T., Lerman, D. C., Volkert, V. M., & Trosclair,<br />

N. (2007). Further examinati<strong>on</strong> of factors<br />

that influence preference for positive versus negative<br />

reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior<br />

Analysis, 40, 25–44.<br />

Review of Choice <strong>and</strong> Preference Assessment / 593


Kreiner, J., & Flexer, R. (2009). Assessment of leisure<br />

preferences for students with severe developmental<br />

disabilities <strong>and</strong> communicati<strong>on</strong> difficulties.<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 44, 280–288.<br />

Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, G. E., O’Reilly, M. F., & Emers<strong>on</strong>, E.<br />

(1996). A review of choice research with people<br />

with severe <strong>and</strong> profound developmental disabilities.<br />

Research in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 17, 391–<br />

411.<br />

Lattimore, L. P., Pars<strong>on</strong>s, M. B., & Reid, D. H.<br />

(2003). Assessing preferred work am<strong>on</strong>g adults<br />

with autism beginning supported jobs: Identificati<strong>on</strong><br />

of c<strong>on</strong>stant <strong>and</strong> alternating task preferences.<br />

Behavioral Interventi<strong>on</strong>s, 18, 161–177.<br />

Lee, M. S., Yu, C. T., Martin, T. L., & Martin, G. L.<br />

(2010). On the relati<strong>on</strong> between reinforcer efficacy<br />

<strong>and</strong> preference. Journal of Applied Behavior<br />

Analysis, 43, 95–100.<br />

Machalicek, W., O’Reilly, M., Chan, J. M., Rispoli,<br />

M., Lang, R., Davis, T., & Langthorne, P. (2009).<br />

Using videoc<strong>on</strong>ferencing to support teachers to<br />

c<strong>on</strong>duct preference assessments with students<br />

with autism <strong>and</strong> developmental disabilities. Research<br />

in <strong>Autism</strong> Spectrum Disorders, 3, 32–41.<br />

Mechling, L. C., & Moser, S. V. (2010). Video preference<br />

assessment of students with autism for<br />

watching self, adults, or peers. Focus <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Other <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 25, 76–84.<br />

Milo, J. S., Mace, F. C., & Nevin, J. A. (2010). The<br />

effects of c<strong>on</strong>stant versus varied reinforcers <strong>on</strong><br />

preference <strong>and</strong> resistance to change. Journal of the<br />

Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 93, 385–394.<br />

O’Reilly, M. F., Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, G. E., & Sigafoos, J.<br />

(2004). Using paired-choice assessment to identify<br />

variables maintaining sleep problems in a<br />

child with severe disabilities. Journal of Applied<br />

Behavior Analysis, 37, 209–212.<br />

Reed, D. K., Luiselli, J. K., Magnus<strong>on</strong>, S. F., Vieira,<br />

S., & Rue, H. C. (2009). A comparis<strong>on</strong> between<br />

traditi<strong>on</strong>al ec<strong>on</strong>omical <strong>and</strong> dem<strong>and</strong> curve analyses<br />

of relative reinforcer efficacy in the validati<strong>on</strong><br />

of preference assessment predicti<strong>on</strong>s. <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

Neurorehabilitati<strong>on</strong>, 12, 164–169.<br />

Reed, D. K., Pace, G. M., & Luiselli, J. K. (2009). An<br />

investigati<strong>on</strong> into the provisi<strong>on</strong> of choice in tangible<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s of a functi<strong>on</strong>al analysis. Journal of<br />

<strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>and</strong> Physical <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 21, 485–491.<br />

Reid, D. H., Green, C. W., & Pars<strong>on</strong>s, M. B. (2003).<br />

An outcome management program for extending<br />

advances in choice research into choice opportunities<br />

for supported workers with severe multiple<br />

disabilities. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36,<br />

575–578.<br />

Reid, D. H., Pars<strong>on</strong>s, M. B., Towery, D., Lattimore,<br />

L. P., Green, C. W., & Brackett, L. (2007). Identifying<br />

work preferences am<strong>on</strong>g supported workers<br />

with severe disabilities: Efficiency <strong>and</strong> accu-<br />

racy of a preference-assessment protocol.<br />

Behavioral Interventi<strong>on</strong>s, 22, 279–296.<br />

Roscoe, E. M., & Fisher, W. W. (2008). Evaluati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

an efficient method for training staff to implement<br />

stimulus preference assessments. Journal of<br />

Applied Behavior Analysis, 41, 249–256.<br />

Roscoe, E. M., Fisher, W. W., Glover, A. C., & Volkert,<br />

V. M. (2006). Evaluating the relative effects of<br />

feedback <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tingent m<strong>on</strong>ey for staff training<br />

of stimulus preference assessments. Journal of Applied<br />

Behavior Analysis, 39, 63–77.<br />

Saunders, M. D., Saunders, R. R., Mulugeta, A.,<br />

Henders<strong>on</strong>, K., Kedziorski, T., Hekker, B., & Wils<strong>on</strong>,<br />

S. (2004). A novel method for testing learning<br />

<strong>and</strong> preferences in people with minimal motor<br />

movement. Research in <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 26, 255–266.<br />

Sigafoos, J., Green, V. A., Payne, D., S<strong>on</strong>, S. H.,<br />

O’Reilly, M., & Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, G. E. (2009). A comparis<strong>on</strong><br />

of picture exchange <strong>and</strong> speech-generating<br />

devices: Acquisiti<strong>on</strong>, preference <strong>and</strong> effects <strong>on</strong><br />

social interacti<strong>on</strong>. Augmentative <strong>and</strong> Alternative<br />

Communicati<strong>on</strong>, 25, 99–109.<br />

Sigafoos, J., O’Reilly, M., Ganz, J. B., Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, G. E.,<br />

& Schlosser, R. W. (2005). Supporting self-determinati<strong>on</strong><br />

in AAC interventi<strong>on</strong>s by assessing preference<br />

for communicati<strong>on</strong> devices. Technology <strong>and</strong><br />

Disability, 17, 143–153.<br />

Smaby, K., MacD<strong>on</strong>ald, R. P. F., Ahearn, W. H., &<br />

Dube, W. V. (2007). Assessment protocol for<br />

identifying preferred social c<strong>on</strong>sequences. Behavioral<br />

Interventi<strong>on</strong>s, 22, 311–318.<br />

Smith, A. J., Bihm, E. M., Tavkar, P, & Sturmey, P.<br />

(2004). Approach-avoidance <strong>and</strong> happiness indicators<br />

in natural envir<strong>on</strong>ments: A preliminary<br />

analysis of the stimulus preference coding system.<br />

Research in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 26, 297–313.<br />

S<strong>on</strong>, S. H., Sigafoos, J., O’Reilly, M., & Lanci<strong>on</strong>i,<br />

G. E. (2006). Comparing two types of augmentative<br />

<strong>and</strong> alternative communicati<strong>on</strong> systems for<br />

children with autism. Pediatric Rehabilitati<strong>on</strong>, 9,<br />

389–395.<br />

Spevack, S., Martin, T. L., Hiebert, R., Yu, C. T., &<br />

Martin, G. L. (2005). Effects of choice of work<br />

tasks <strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong>-task, aberrant, happiness <strong>and</strong> unhappiness<br />

behaviours of pers<strong>on</strong>s with developmental<br />

disabilities. Journal <strong>on</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 11,<br />

79–98.<br />

Spevack, S., Yu, C. T., Lee, M. S., & Martin, G. L.<br />

(2006). Sensitivity of passive approach during<br />

preference <strong>and</strong> reinforcer assessments for children<br />

with severe <strong>and</strong> profound intellectual disabilities<br />

<strong>and</strong> minimal movement. Behavioral Interventi<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

21, 165–175.<br />

Tasky, K. M., Rudrud, E. H., Schulze, K. A., & Rapp,<br />

J. T. (2008). Using choice to increase <strong>on</strong>-task<br />

behavior in individuals with traumatic brain in-<br />

594 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


jury. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41, 261–<br />

265.<br />

Thoms<strong>on</strong>, K. M., Czarnecki, D., Martin, T. L., Yu,<br />

C. T., & Martin, G. L. (2007). Predicting optimal<br />

preference assessment methods for individuals<br />

with developmental disabilities. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Training in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 42, 107–114.<br />

Wilder, D. A., Schadler, J., Higbee, T. S., Haymes,<br />

L. K., Bajagic, V., & Register, M. (2008). Identificati<strong>on</strong><br />

of olfactory stimuli as reinforcers in individuals<br />

with autism: A preliminary investigati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Behavioral Interventi<strong>on</strong>s, 23, 97–103.<br />

Received: 7 September 2010<br />

Initial Acceptance: 2 November 2010<br />

Final Acceptance: 18 January 2011<br />

Review of Choice <strong>and</strong> Preference Assessment / 595


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 2011, <str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>(4), 596-606<br />

© <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Effects of a Treatment Package <strong>on</strong> Imitated <strong>and</strong> Sp<strong>on</strong>taneous<br />

Verbal Requests in Children with <strong>Autism</strong><br />

Jennifer B. Ganz<br />

Texas A&M University<br />

Margaret M. Flores<br />

Auburn University<br />

Erin E. Lashley<br />

Northside Independent School District, San Ant<strong>on</strong>io, TX<br />

Abstract: Students with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) have difficulties with verbal language. Many<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>s to remediate such deficits require numerous materials <strong>and</strong> significant teacher time. This study<br />

sought to determine if a simple multi-comp<strong>on</strong>ent interventi<strong>on</strong> that incorporated n<strong>on</strong>c<strong>on</strong>tingent reinforcement<br />

(NCR) <strong>and</strong> verbal modeling would result in increased sp<strong>on</strong>taneous verbal requesting in two preschool boys with<br />

autism. Results indicated that the participants did increase use of sp<strong>on</strong>taneous <strong>and</strong> imitated verbal requests.<br />

Implicati<strong>on</strong>s are discussed.<br />

Deficits in communicati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> speech are a<br />

hallmark of autism spectrum disorders (ASD;<br />

American Psychiatric Associati<strong>on</strong> [APA],<br />

2000). Many individuals with ASD do not<br />

speak or have delayed communicati<strong>on</strong> skills<br />

(APA), including limited communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

functi<strong>on</strong>s (e.g., requests <strong>and</strong> refusals; Ogletree,<br />

2008). Some have difficulty maintaining<br />

c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> use repetitive or echolalic<br />

speech (APA; Janzen, 2003). Other aspects of<br />

speech may be delayed, including articulati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tent, grammar, <strong>and</strong> using abstract c<strong>on</strong>cepts<br />

in language (Janzen).<br />

The Nati<strong>on</strong>al Research Council (2001) recommends<br />

that programming for children<br />

with ASD includes at least 25 hours per week<br />

of systematic instructi<strong>on</strong>, focusing <strong>on</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

<strong>and</strong> sp<strong>on</strong>taneous communicati<strong>on</strong>. Such<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> may cost up to $60,000 per year<br />

(Nati<strong>on</strong>al Research Council). Thus, it is important<br />

to determine if some children with<br />

ASD resp<strong>on</strong>d to less costly, effective strategies,<br />

which allow precious resources, including pers<strong>on</strong>nel<br />

training time <strong>and</strong> cost, instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

time, <strong>and</strong> materials, to be reserved for those<br />

Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence c<strong>on</strong>cerning this article should<br />

be addressed to Jennifer Ganz, Texas A&M University,<br />

Department of Educati<strong>on</strong>al Psychology, 4225<br />

TAMU, College Stati<strong>on</strong>, TX 77843-4225. Email:<br />

jeniganz@tamu.edu<br />

students who require costly <strong>and</strong> intensive interventi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Koegel, Koegel, Shoshan, <strong>and</strong> Mc-<br />

Nerney (1999) report that children with autism<br />

who dem<strong>on</strong>strate more frequent<br />

sp<strong>on</strong>taneous communicati<strong>on</strong>, including n<strong>on</strong>verbal<br />

communicati<strong>on</strong>, prior to communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

treatment resp<strong>on</strong>d better to such treatment.<br />

Several methods have been proposed to replace<br />

challenging behaviors with socially acceptable<br />

communicati<strong>on</strong> skills in children<br />

with ASD. N<strong>on</strong>c<strong>on</strong>tingent reinforcement<br />

(NCR) focuses <strong>on</strong> increasing socially relevant<br />

skills <strong>and</strong> has been proven effective with individuals<br />

with a variety of disabilities. Further,<br />

elements of incidental teaching <strong>and</strong> time-delay<br />

have had promising results.<br />

Modeling of behaviors has been used to<br />

improve a variety of skills in children with ASD<br />

(Mats<strong>on</strong>, Mats<strong>on</strong>, & Rivet, 2007; Stahmer, Ingersoll,<br />

& Carter, 2003). This procedure,<br />

when used for modeling names of items for<br />

requesting, had also been called m<strong>and</strong>-modeling<br />

(Mobayed, Collins, Strangis, Schuster, &<br />

Hemmeter, 2000). Research has supported<br />

the use of in vivo modeling of peers <strong>and</strong> adults<br />

to promote labeling (Charlop, Schreibman, &<br />

Try<strong>on</strong>, 1983) <strong>and</strong> following directi<strong>on</strong>s (Egel,<br />

Richman, & Koegel, 1981). In vivo modeling<br />

has also resulted in improved play <strong>and</strong> social<br />

skills in children with autism, including dis-<br />

596 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


playing affect (Gena, Couloura, & Kymissis,<br />

2005), cooperative play skills (Try<strong>on</strong> & Keane,<br />

1986), <strong>and</strong> independent play (Jahr, Elevik, &<br />

Eikeseth, 2000). Thus, repeated, verbal modeling<br />

of names of preferred items may have<br />

potential in improving the communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

skills in such children.<br />

N<strong>on</strong>c<strong>on</strong>tingent reinforcement (NCR) involves<br />

giving children access to reinforcement<br />

regardless of whether or not they perform<br />

desired behaviors (Heflin & Alaimo, 2007;<br />

Tucker, Sigafoos, & Bushnell, 1998). NCR has<br />

been dem<strong>on</strong>strated to be effective in a few<br />

studies with individuals with ASD <strong>and</strong> developmental<br />

delays. Carr, Dozier, Patel, Adams,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Martin (2002) implemented NCR with<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se blocking to reduce object mouthing.<br />

Lalli, Casey, <strong>and</strong> Kates (1995) implemented<br />

NCR with <strong>and</strong> without extincti<strong>on</strong> procedures,<br />

resulting in reduced aggressive <strong>and</strong> self-injurious<br />

behaviors. Roscoe, Iwata, <strong>and</strong> Goh (1998)<br />

compared the use of NCR with extincti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

found both to be effective in reducing selfinjurious<br />

behaviors. Butler <strong>and</strong> Luiselli (2007)<br />

used n<strong>on</strong>c<strong>on</strong>tingent escape, combined with<br />

other interventi<strong>on</strong>s, to decrease aggressi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

self-injury, <strong>and</strong> tantrums in a child with autism.<br />

One study (Marcus & Vollmer, 1996)<br />

successfully dem<strong>on</strong>strated the combinati<strong>on</strong><br />

NCR with differential reinforcement of alternative<br />

behavior to improve communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

skills in children with mental retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

autism. Similarly, Mild<strong>on</strong> et al. (2004) implemented<br />

n<strong>on</strong>c<strong>on</strong>tingent escape with functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

communicati<strong>on</strong> training to improve verbal<br />

communicati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> compliance in a young<br />

child with autism. Finally, Ganz, Heath,<br />

Rispoli, <strong>and</strong> Earles-Vollrath (2010) noted a<br />

small increase in related speech during a n<strong>on</strong>c<strong>on</strong>tingent<br />

reinforcement plus verbal modeling<br />

procedure for a three-year-old with autism,<br />

which was not found during<br />

implementati<strong>on</strong> of the Picture Exchange<br />

Communicati<strong>on</strong> System.<br />

Time-delay, when used to promote the use<br />

of speech in individuals with ASD, usually involves<br />

offering a child a reinforcing item,<br />

modeling the name of the item, <strong>and</strong> withholding<br />

the item until the child repeats the correct<br />

name of the item (Ross & Greer, 2003). Such<br />

a procedure is often limited to children who<br />

already speak, but need to learn new words.<br />

This procedure has also been successfully im-<br />

plemented within an incidental teaching<br />

model to teach speech within natural c<strong>on</strong>texts<br />

(Charlop-Christy & Carpenter, 2000; McGee,<br />

Almeida, Sulzer-Azaroff, & Feldman, 1992;<br />

McGee, Krantz, Mas<strong>on</strong>, & McClannahan,<br />

1983).<br />

The purpose of this study was to investigate<br />

the impact of implementing a multi-comp<strong>on</strong>ent<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>al package, including NCR in<br />

combinati<strong>on</strong> with repeated verbal modeling,<br />

<strong>on</strong> sp<strong>on</strong>taneous <strong>and</strong> imitated verbalizati<strong>on</strong>s in<br />

children with autism. The following research<br />

questi<strong>on</strong> was investigated: can children with<br />

autism who do not speak sp<strong>on</strong>taneously (but<br />

can imitate 1–2 word phrases) learn to make<br />

sp<strong>on</strong>taneous or imitated verbal requests with<br />

NCR <strong>and</strong> verbal modeling? While increases in<br />

sp<strong>on</strong>taneous speech were preferable, data<br />

were also collected <strong>on</strong> imitated verbalizati<strong>on</strong>s;<br />

increased use of imitated speech may emerge<br />

before sp<strong>on</strong>taneous speech in children with<br />

ASD <strong>and</strong> such progress in echolalic speech<br />

indicates an increased likelihood for further<br />

language development (Carpenter & Tomasello,<br />

2000).<br />

Method<br />

Participants <strong>and</strong> Materials<br />

Participants were recruited from a private<br />

school for children with ASD <strong>and</strong> other developmental<br />

disabilities. They were diagnosed<br />

with autism independently of this research by<br />

certified medical professi<strong>on</strong>als according to<br />

the Diagnostic <strong>and</strong> Statistical Manual of Mental<br />

Disorders (DSM-IV-TR; American Psychiatric Associati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

2000) criteria. The participants’ diagnoses<br />

were then c<strong>on</strong>firmed via the Childhood<br />

<strong>Autism</strong> Rating Scale (CARS; Schopler, Reichler,<br />

& Renner, 1988) <strong>and</strong> the <strong>Autism</strong> Diagnostic<br />

Observati<strong>on</strong> Schedule (ADOS; Lord, Rutter, Di-<br />

Lavore, & Risi, 1999). Two children with autism<br />

were identified as potential participants,<br />

met the inclusi<strong>on</strong> criteria, <strong>and</strong> completed this<br />

study. Participant descripti<strong>on</strong>s given below reflect<br />

informati<strong>on</strong> obtained from the researchers’<br />

administrati<strong>on</strong> of the CARS based <strong>on</strong><br />

observati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> the researchers’ administrati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

of the ADOS.<br />

Josiah was 3-years, 6-m<strong>on</strong>ths old <strong>and</strong> was<br />

diagnosed with autism. He fell within the autism<br />

range <strong>on</strong> the ADOS <strong>and</strong> mild/moderate<br />

Multi-Comp<strong>on</strong>ent Communicati<strong>on</strong> Interventi<strong>on</strong> / 597


autism <strong>on</strong> the CARS. Regarding social interacti<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

he would resp<strong>on</strong>d when an adult persisted<br />

in attempting to gain his attenti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

though he was not as resp<strong>on</strong>sive as typicallydeveloping<br />

peers. Josiah’s verbal communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

was characterized by <strong>on</strong>e-word utterances<br />

that were clearly articulated <strong>and</strong> infrequent<br />

sp<strong>on</strong>taneous verbal requests. Josiah did not<br />

engage in severe problem behaviors, however,<br />

he often snatched or grabbed items he wanted<br />

due to a lack of more socially acceptable<br />

means of requesting. That is, he dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

sp<strong>on</strong>taneous, but n<strong>on</strong>c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al means of<br />

communicati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Ari was 4-years, 11-m<strong>on</strong>ths old <strong>and</strong> was diagnosed<br />

with autism <strong>and</strong> speech impairment.<br />

He fell within the autism range <strong>on</strong> the ADOS<br />

<strong>and</strong> the mild/moderate autism range <strong>on</strong> the<br />

CARS. Specifically, Ari did not resp<strong>on</strong>d to a<br />

social smile, used few facial expressi<strong>on</strong>s directed<br />

at others, <strong>and</strong> did not initiate joint<br />

attenti<strong>on</strong>. Further, Ari’s verbal communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

was characterized by infrequent use of<br />

<strong>on</strong>e-word phrases, poorly articulated word approximati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

(e.g., he frequently left off final<br />

c<strong>on</strong>s<strong>on</strong>ant sounds), <strong>and</strong> infrequent sp<strong>on</strong>taneous<br />

use of verbal requests. Ari frequently engaged<br />

in tantrums (crying <strong>and</strong> screaming)<br />

when he did not obtain items he wanted. Ari<br />

did communicate sp<strong>on</strong>taneously, using n<strong>on</strong>c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al<br />

means.<br />

Setting<br />

Both of the participants were enrolled in a<br />

private school for children with autism <strong>and</strong><br />

developmental disabilities in three classes that<br />

served children from age two through elementary<br />

ages. The study took place in the school<br />

therapy room <strong>and</strong> in an office which was also<br />

used as an assessment clinic. The researcher<br />

sat within arm’s reach of the participants at a<br />

table or <strong>on</strong> the floor. Materials included a<br />

variety of toys <strong>and</strong> food items that the children<br />

preferred.<br />

Resp<strong>on</strong>se Definiti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> Measurement<br />

Each sessi<strong>on</strong> included five opportunities, or<br />

trials, to request items. Each trial began when<br />

the researcher enticed the child by showing<br />

the item <strong>and</strong> ended 10 s after the participant<br />

was given the item (with or without correctly<br />

naming the item). Data were <strong>on</strong>ly recorded<br />

for trials during which the child showed interest<br />

in the item presented by gesturing or<br />

reaching for it or requesting it verbally. If the<br />

child did not show interest in the item, another<br />

item was presented.<br />

The researchers collected data <strong>on</strong> three dependent<br />

variables. For each opportunity to<br />

request an item, the observers recorded<br />

whether () or not (-) the participant engaged<br />

in that behavior. The dependent variables<br />

included (a) requested with verbal model<br />

(did the participant use the correct, intelligible<br />

name for the item following a verbal<br />

model from the researcher?), (b) requested independently<br />

(did the participant correctly name<br />

the item using a single word or phrase that<br />

correctly applied to the item shown <strong>and</strong> without<br />

a verbal model?), <strong>and</strong> (c) word approximati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

(did the participant use a correct word<br />

approximati<strong>on</strong> with or without a verbal model<br />

[at least two ph<strong>on</strong>emes from the actual<br />

word]?). Data for these variables were reported<br />

in percentage correct per sessi<strong>on</strong>. The<br />

researchers also recorded word-for-word what<br />

the participants said during each trial. Data<br />

were collected during all interventi<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Experimental Design<br />

A multiple baseline design across object sets<br />

(3 reinforcing items per set) was used. The<br />

study was implemented in three phases across<br />

the three sets for each participant. The criteri<strong>on</strong><br />

for a phase change, that is implementati<strong>on</strong><br />

of interventi<strong>on</strong> with the next set, was<br />

three c<strong>on</strong>secutive sessi<strong>on</strong>s of 80–100% requested<br />

independently or word approximati<strong>on</strong>s, if<br />

the participant had poor articulati<strong>on</strong> but was<br />

comprehensible.<br />

Procedure<br />

Reinforcer assessment. First, the participants’<br />

parents completed reinforcer checklists. Then<br />

the experimenter offered each participant two<br />

to four items at a time, changing placement of<br />

the items for each presentati<strong>on</strong>. Food items<br />

were offered separately from toys. The experimenter<br />

recorded tally marks to assess which<br />

items each participant chose most frequently.<br />

Each participant’s preferred items were di-<br />

598 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


vided into three sets that were approximately<br />

equivalent in terms of reinforcing value or<br />

topographic use. For example, Josiah’s set 1<br />

included a wind-up zebra, a laser disk, <strong>and</strong><br />

light-up clackers; set 2 included a wind-up<br />

m<strong>on</strong>key, a sound stick, <strong>and</strong> stretchy string;<br />

<strong>and</strong> set 3 included a wind-up car, glitter w<strong>and</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> fun fruit.<br />

Baseline. First, baseline data were collected.<br />

The experimenter held <strong>on</strong>e of the<br />

child’s preferred items <strong>and</strong> enticed him by<br />

shaking the item, playing with it herself, holding<br />

it out as if to h<strong>and</strong> it to the child, or saying<br />

something to get the child’s attenti<strong>on</strong>, but did<br />

not name the item. If the participant showed<br />

interest in the item (e.g., reached for it, tried<br />

to grab it, gesturing toward it) without requesting<br />

it verbally, the experimenter would<br />

hold <strong>on</strong>to the item for a5stime delay. If the<br />

child did independently request the item or<br />

used a word approximati<strong>on</strong>, the experimenter<br />

would immediately give the item to the child.<br />

If the participant did not correctly name the<br />

item, he would not be given the item until the<br />

end of the 5 s delay. Then, he would have brief<br />

(approximately 10 s) access to the item. At<br />

least two items from the current set were used<br />

during a single sessi<strong>on</strong>. That is, the researcher<br />

would entice the participant with at least two<br />

items throughout the sessi<strong>on</strong>, although the<br />

participant did not necessarily initiate trials<br />

with both.<br />

Multi-comp<strong>on</strong>ent instructi<strong>on</strong>al program. The<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> phase was identical to the baseline<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s with the additi<strong>on</strong> of verbal<br />

models <strong>and</strong> expansi<strong>on</strong> of delayed access to<br />

reinforcers. Interventi<strong>on</strong> incorporated Tucker<br />

et al.’s (1998) suggesti<strong>on</strong>s regarding NCR: following<br />

initial c<strong>on</strong>tinuous reinforcement<br />

(baseline), reinforcement should be faded to<br />

a lower frequency, <strong>and</strong> NCR should be combined<br />

with educati<strong>on</strong>al interventi<strong>on</strong>s (e.g.,<br />

modeling). This implementati<strong>on</strong> of NCR was<br />

a slight variati<strong>on</strong> from how NCR is typically<br />

implemented in that the items were not freely<br />

available, but were frequently available without<br />

a prerequisite behavior.<br />

The experimenter would entice the child as<br />

in baseline. When the participant initiated a<br />

trial, the experimenter would give up to a5s<br />

time delay. If the child did not initiate a verbal<br />

request, following the 5 s delay, the examiner<br />

would c<strong>on</strong>tinue to hold the item <strong>and</strong> correctly<br />

name it up to three times, pausing approximately<br />

2 s between each verbal model. If the<br />

child correctly requested the item at any time,<br />

he would be given access to the item for approximately<br />

10 s. Once he requested independently<br />

or requested following a verbal model,<br />

the verbal models would cease for the remainder<br />

of the trial, however, if he used a word<br />

approximati<strong>on</strong>, he would immediately be<br />

given access to the item <strong>and</strong> the experimenter<br />

would c<strong>on</strong>tinue to correctly model the word.<br />

If the child did not request independently or<br />

approximate the name, he would receive the<br />

item 2 s following the third verbal model <strong>and</strong><br />

have access to it for 10 s. That is, he would<br />

n<strong>on</strong>-c<strong>on</strong>tingently obtain the item, but after a<br />

delay. Making a request independently or a<br />

request following a verbal model resulted in<br />

quicker access to the item.<br />

Generalizati<strong>on</strong>. Generalizati<strong>on</strong> involved implementati<strong>on</strong><br />

of the interventi<strong>on</strong> with a novel<br />

experimenter for <strong>on</strong>e sessi<strong>on</strong> for each set with<br />

the participants who completed all three interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

phases. These sessi<strong>on</strong>s took place<br />

within the two days following the completi<strong>on</strong><br />

of interventi<strong>on</strong>. Procedures were identical to<br />

those described above in the previous secti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Follow-up. Follow-up data were collected 3<br />

weeks following the cessati<strong>on</strong> of interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

for each of the participants who completed all<br />

three phases of interventi<strong>on</strong>. Follow-up followed<br />

the procedures described above in the<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> secti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Analysis<br />

Results were analyzed via visual analysis of the<br />

graphically displayed data for each participant<br />

to determine if there was a functi<strong>on</strong>al relati<strong>on</strong><br />

between the interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> the communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

behaviors. Visual analysis inspected for<br />

ascending or descending trends, swiftness of<br />

change in trend <strong>and</strong> change in mean. Data<br />

were also analyzed via an effect size, the Improvement<br />

Rate Difference (IRD) (Parker,<br />

Vannest, & Brown, 2009). IRD calculates the<br />

amount of overlap between data points in different<br />

phases (e.g., baseline versus interventi<strong>on</strong>)<br />

or between different treatments. IRD:<br />

(a) has a lengthy record in medicine, as “risk<br />

difference;” (b) has a known sampling distributi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

thus p-values <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>fidence intervals<br />

may be calculated, <strong>and</strong> (c) correlates with<br />

Multi-Comp<strong>on</strong>ent Communicati<strong>on</strong> Interventi<strong>on</strong> / 599


other effect size measures <strong>and</strong> with visual analysis<br />

(Parker et al.). IRD measures the difference<br />

in “improvement rates” between two<br />

phases or interventi<strong>on</strong>s. “Improved” data<br />

points in phase A are larger than expected.<br />

Parker et al. recommend the following guidelines<br />

when interpreting IRD scores: IRDs of<br />

below 0.50 signify small or questi<strong>on</strong>able effects,<br />

IRDs between 0.50 <strong>and</strong> 0.70 suggest<br />

moderate effects, <strong>and</strong> IRD scores at approximately<br />

0.70 or 0.75 or higher are large or very<br />

large effects.<br />

Inter-observer Agreement <strong>and</strong> Treatment Fidelity<br />

The observers included two college professors<br />

<strong>and</strong> a graduate research assistant, who are the<br />

authors of this article. Inter-observer agreement<br />

was assessed by using a point-by-point<br />

agreement ratio. The number of agreements<br />

was divided by the total number of agreements<br />

plus disagreements <strong>and</strong> multiplied by<br />

100 (Kazdin, 1982). An agreement occurred<br />

when two observers independently recorded<br />

the same score for a dependent variable during<br />

a trial. During baseline, inter-observer<br />

agreement was assessed for 58% of Josiah’s<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> 64% of Ari’s sessi<strong>on</strong>s. Mean interobserver<br />

agreement for baseline was calculated<br />

at 100% <strong>and</strong> 98% (range 80%–100%),<br />

respectively. During interventi<strong>on</strong>, inter-observer<br />

agreement was assessed for 81% of Josiah’s<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> 85% of Ari’s sessi<strong>on</strong>s. Mean<br />

inter-observer agreement for interventi<strong>on</strong> was<br />

calculated at 96% (range 73%–100%) <strong>and</strong><br />

97% (range 80%–100%). Inter-observer<br />

agreement was assessed during generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s for <strong>on</strong>e (33%) of Josiah’s sessi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong><br />

was calculated at 100%, <strong>and</strong> for two (67%) of<br />

Ari’s sessi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> was calculated at 77%<br />

(range 73%–80%). Inter-observer agreement<br />

was assessed for <strong>on</strong>e (33%) of Josiah’s<br />

follow-up sessi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> was calculated at 100%<br />

<strong>and</strong> for all (100%) of Ari’s follow-up sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<strong>and</strong> was calculated at 100%. Coefficients of<br />

agreement broken down by dependent variables<br />

are available by request from the authors.<br />

Treatment fidelity was assessed <strong>on</strong> six days<br />

of interventi<strong>on</strong>. The following steps or comp<strong>on</strong>ents<br />

of interventi<strong>on</strong> were assessed by an<br />

observer <strong>and</strong> she recorded whether or not the<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>ist correctly implemented each<br />

step or comp<strong>on</strong>ent of interventi<strong>on</strong>: (a) held<br />

item within view; (b) waited for the child to<br />

initiate for 5 s; (c) when the child did not<br />

initiate, chose another item; (d) when child<br />

initiated, but did not say the word or an approximati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

gave verbal model of word; (e)<br />

gave up to three verbal models before giving<br />

the item; (f) waited approximately 2 s between<br />

verbal models of word; (g) if the child said the<br />

word or approximati<strong>on</strong>, or after all verbal<br />

models, gave the item immediately; <strong>and</strong> (h)<br />

allowed the child to play with the item for at<br />

least 10 s. Treatment fidelity was assessed as<br />

100% for all observed sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Results<br />

Josiah<br />

Figure 1 presents Josiah’s requests following a<br />

verbal model, those that were requested independently,<br />

<strong>and</strong> the use of word approximati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

to make requests.<br />

Requested following a verbal model. There<br />

were no opportunities for Josiah to request<br />

following a verbal model during baseline sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

During interventi<strong>on</strong> for set 1, Josiah<br />

quickly began requesting following a verbal<br />

model at a high rate (mean 36%, range <br />

0–100% of the trials), however, these rapidly<br />

dropped in level as independent requests increased.<br />

During generalizati<strong>on</strong> for set 1, Josiah<br />

requested following a verbal model during<br />

0% of the trials. At follow-up, Josiah<br />

requested following a verbal model during<br />

40% of the trials. During interventi<strong>on</strong> for set<br />

2, Josiah requested following a verbal model at<br />

a lower rates (mean 13%, range 0–40%<br />

of the trials). During generalizati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> follow-up<br />

for set 2, Josiah never requested following<br />

a verbal model. During interventi<strong>on</strong> for<br />

set 3, Josiah requested following a verbal<br />

model at a low, but variable rates (mean <br />

33% range 0–100% of the trials). During<br />

generalizati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> follow-up for set 3, Josiah<br />

did not request following a verbal model.<br />

Requested independently. For set 1, Josiah infrequently<br />

requested independently (mean <br />

13%, range 0–20% of the trials) during<br />

baseline sessi<strong>on</strong>s. During interventi<strong>on</strong>, Josiah<br />

gradually began independently requesting the<br />

items at a high rate (mean 60%, range <br />

0–100% of the trials). During generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

600 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


Figure 1. Josiah: Percentage of trials during which he requested following a verbal model, requested independently,<br />

<strong>and</strong> used word approximati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

for set 1, Josiah independently requested the<br />

items during 60% of the trials. At follow-up,<br />

Josiah independently requested items during<br />

40% of the trials. For set 2, Josiah infrequently<br />

requested independently (mean 10%,<br />

range 0–20% of the trials) during baseline<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s. During interventi<strong>on</strong>, Josiah began independently<br />

requesting the items at a higher<br />

rate (mean 73%, range 40–100% of the<br />

trials). During generalizati<strong>on</strong> for set 2, Josiah<br />

independently requested the items during<br />

60% of the trials. At follow-up, Josiah independently<br />

requested items during 60% of the trials.<br />

For set 3, Josiah never independently<br />

named the items during baseline sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

During interventi<strong>on</strong>, Josiah gradually began<br />

independently requesting the items at a<br />

higher rate (mean 57%, range 0–100%<br />

of the trials). During generalizati<strong>on</strong> for set 3,<br />

Josiah independently requested the items during<br />

100% of the trials. At follow-up, Josiah<br />

independently requested items during 80% of<br />

the trials. Overall IRD for Josiah’s independent<br />

(i.e., not following a verbal model) requesting<br />

was 0.73, 90% CI [0.47, 0.86], p <br />

.000. This suggests moderate to large effects<br />

for this participant <strong>and</strong> this dependent variable.<br />

Word approximati<strong>on</strong>s. For set 1, Josiah<br />

never used word approximati<strong>on</strong>s to make re-<br />

Multi-Comp<strong>on</strong>ent Communicati<strong>on</strong> Interventi<strong>on</strong> / 601


quests during baseline sessi<strong>on</strong>s. During interventi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

Josiah used word approximati<strong>on</strong>s at a<br />

low rate (mean 6%, range 0–40% of the<br />

trials). During generalizati<strong>on</strong> for set 1, Josiah<br />

used word approximati<strong>on</strong>s during 40% of the<br />

trials. At follow-up, Josiah used word approximati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

during 20% of the trials. For set 2,<br />

Josiah infrequently used word approximati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

(mean 5%, range 0–20% of the trials)<br />

during baseline sessi<strong>on</strong>s. During interventi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

Josiah infrequently used word approximati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

(mean 13%, range 0–40% of the trials).<br />

During generalizati<strong>on</strong> for set 2, Josiah used<br />

word approximati<strong>on</strong>s during 40% of the trials.<br />

At follow-up, Josiah used word approximati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

during 20% of the trials. For set 3, Josiah<br />

never used word approximati<strong>on</strong>s during baseline<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s. During interventi<strong>on</strong>, Josiah used<br />

word approximati<strong>on</strong>s during <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e sessi<strong>on</strong><br />

(mean 3%, range 0–20% of the trials).<br />

During generalizati<strong>on</strong> for set 3, Josiah did not<br />

use word approximati<strong>on</strong>s. At follow-up, Josiah<br />

used word approximati<strong>on</strong>s during 20% of the<br />

trials. Visual analysis clearly dem<strong>on</strong>strates that<br />

the interventi<strong>on</strong> was far more effective for<br />

independent requests for Josiah, thus, IRD<br />

was not calculated for word approximati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Ari<br />

Figure 2 presents Ari’s correct requests with a<br />

verbal model, those that were correctly requested<br />

independently, <strong>and</strong> his use of word<br />

approximati<strong>on</strong>s to make requests.<br />

Requested following a verbal model. There<br />

were no opportunities for Ari to request following<br />

a verbal model during baseline. For set<br />

1, during interventi<strong>on</strong>, Ari began requesting<br />

following a verbal model at a low rate<br />

(mean 11%, range 0–20% of the trials).<br />

During generalizati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> follow-up for set 1,<br />

Ari did not request following a verbal model<br />

during any of the trials. For set 2, during<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>, Ari correctly requested the items<br />

during <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e sessi<strong>on</strong> (mean 8%, range <br />

0–40% of the trials). During generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> follow-up for set 2, Ari did not request<br />

following a verbal model during any of the<br />

trials. During interventi<strong>on</strong> for set 3, Ari infrequently<br />

requested following a verbal model<br />

(mean 4%, range 0–20% of the trials).<br />

During generalizati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> follow-up for set 3,<br />

Ari did not request following a verbal model.<br />

Requested independently. For set 1, Ari never<br />

correctly <strong>and</strong> independently named the items<br />

he was requesting during baseline sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

During interventi<strong>on</strong>, Ari correctly <strong>and</strong> independently<br />

requested the items at low rates<br />

(mean 14%, range 0–60% of the trials).<br />

During generalizati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> follow-up for set 1,<br />

Ari never correctly <strong>and</strong> independently requested<br />

the items. For set 2, Ari did not correctly<br />

<strong>and</strong> independently name the items he<br />

was requesting during any baseline sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

During interventi<strong>on</strong>, Ari correctly <strong>and</strong> independently<br />

requested the items during <strong>on</strong>ly<br />

<strong>on</strong>e sessi<strong>on</strong> (mean 8%, range 0–80% of<br />

the trials). During generalizati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> follow-up<br />

for set 2, Ari did not correctly <strong>and</strong><br />

independently request the items during any of<br />

the trials. For set 3, Ari infrequently correctly<br />

<strong>and</strong> independently named the items (mean <br />

6%, range 0–20% of the trials) during baseline<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s. During interventi<strong>on</strong>, Ari correctly<br />

<strong>and</strong> independently requested the items<br />

at a low rate (mean 28%, range 0–80% of<br />

the trials). During generalizati<strong>on</strong> for set 3, Ari<br />

correctly <strong>and</strong> independently requested the<br />

items during 20% of the trials. At follow-up,<br />

Ari did not correctly <strong>and</strong> independently request<br />

items during any of the trials. Visual<br />

analysis clearly illustrates that the interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

had a greater impact <strong>on</strong> word approximati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for Ari, thus, IRD was not calculated for independent<br />

requests.<br />

Word approximati<strong>on</strong>s. For set 1, Ari never<br />

used word approximati<strong>on</strong>s to make requests<br />

during baseline sessi<strong>on</strong>s. During interventi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

Ari used word approximati<strong>on</strong>s at a high rate<br />

(mean 69%, range 20–100% of the trials).<br />

During generalizati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> follow-up for<br />

set 1, Ari used word approximati<strong>on</strong>s during<br />

100% of the trials. For set 2, Ari never used<br />

word approximati<strong>on</strong>s during baseline sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

During interventi<strong>on</strong>, Ari frequently<br />

used word approximati<strong>on</strong>s (mean 84%,<br />

range 20–100% of the trials). During generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> follow-up for set 2, Ari used<br />

word approximati<strong>on</strong>s during 100% of the trials.<br />

For set 3, Ari used some word approximati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

(mean 20%, range 0–60% of the<br />

trials) during baseline sessi<strong>on</strong>s, using more<br />

near the end of baseline data collecti<strong>on</strong>. During<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>, Ari used word approximati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

frequently (mean 72%, range 20–<br />

100% of the trials). During generalizati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

602 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


Figure 2. Ari: Percentage of trials during which he requested following a verbal model, requested independently,<br />

<strong>and</strong> used word approximati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

follow-up for set 3, Ari used word approximati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

during 80% of the trials. Overall IRD for<br />

Ari’s use of word approximati<strong>on</strong>s was 1.00,<br />

90% CI [0.75, 1.00], p .000. This interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

had large effects for word approximati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for Ari.<br />

Discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

Both participants made progress in targeted<br />

behaviors, <strong>on</strong>e in independent requests <strong>and</strong><br />

the other in use of word approximati<strong>on</strong>s. Jo-<br />

siah rapidly began imitating the correct names<br />

<strong>and</strong> eventually began to sp<strong>on</strong>taneously use the<br />

correct names. Following implementati<strong>on</strong> in<br />

sets 2 <strong>and</strong> 3, Josiah rapidly began to independently<br />

request the items he was requesting.<br />

During generalizati<strong>on</strong> probes with a new communicative<br />

partner, he used the requests,<br />

though at a lower rate for set 2. At follow-up,<br />

Josiah maintained the use of correct <strong>and</strong> independent<br />

requests, though at lower rates<br />

than during interventi<strong>on</strong> phases. While Josiah<br />

did use some word approximati<strong>on</strong>s, his artic-<br />

Multi-Comp<strong>on</strong>ent Communicati<strong>on</strong> Interventi<strong>on</strong> / 603


ulati<strong>on</strong> was generally clear <strong>and</strong> he was able to<br />

correctly pr<strong>on</strong>ounce the names for the items<br />

most of the time.<br />

Ari also made sufficient progress during interventi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

however, he had some difficulties<br />

with articulati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> used word approximati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

more frequently than requests, either<br />

following a verbal model or independently.<br />

Ari did quickly meet criteri<strong>on</strong> for word approximati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for all sets <strong>and</strong> was able to get<br />

his message across. Further, he generalized<br />

use of the word approximati<strong>on</strong>s to a new communicative<br />

partner at high rates <strong>and</strong> maintained<br />

the use of the word approximati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

during follow-up. While, preferably, Ari would<br />

have increased in sp<strong>on</strong>taneous communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

during interventi<strong>on</strong>, an increase in imitated<br />

verbalizati<strong>on</strong>s often precedes sp<strong>on</strong>taneous<br />

communicati<strong>on</strong> in individuals with ASD<br />

<strong>and</strong> is a positive indicator of language development<br />

(Carpenter & Tomasello, 2000).<br />

Though we did not collect data <strong>on</strong> the independent<br />

use of word approximati<strong>on</strong>s, we did<br />

anecdotally note that, during the last sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

of interventi<strong>on</strong> for each set, Ari began using<br />

word approximati<strong>on</strong>s without requiring an<br />

immediate verbal model.<br />

The implicati<strong>on</strong>s of this study indicate that<br />

it may be possible to improve imitated or<br />

sp<strong>on</strong>taneous speech skills in some students<br />

with autism with less costly <strong>and</strong> less intensive<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> packages, such as a combinati<strong>on</strong><br />

of NCR <strong>and</strong> verbal modeling. One questi<strong>on</strong><br />

that remains is what types of students would<br />

be most likely to resp<strong>on</strong>d to such interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<strong>and</strong> which would require more intensive<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>s. C<strong>on</strong>sidering the results of this<br />

study, it may be that children with higher rates<br />

of sp<strong>on</strong>taneous communicati<strong>on</strong> skills, including<br />

n<strong>on</strong>c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al <strong>and</strong> less sophisticated<br />

skills (e.g., grabbing, reaching), <strong>and</strong> those<br />

who initially resp<strong>on</strong>d to others’ initiati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

more frequently (e.g., resp<strong>on</strong>ding to their<br />

name) may more easily resp<strong>on</strong>d to simpler<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>s. Further, younger students <strong>and</strong><br />

those with more complicated health issues<br />

<strong>and</strong> those with more severe scores <strong>on</strong> diagnostic<br />

instruments may require more intensive<br />

<strong>and</strong> costly interventi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

NCR has been shown to decrease mouthing<br />

behaviors (Carr et al., 2002), reduce aggressive<br />

<strong>and</strong> self-injurious behaviors (Lalli et al.,<br />

1995). There is little research in which NCR<br />

has been used to increase communicati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Therefore, the current study extended the<br />

line of research <strong>on</strong> the use of a treatment<br />

package, which included NCR <strong>and</strong> communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>, to increase communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

behaviors (i.e., Marcus & Vollmer, 1996; Mild<strong>on</strong><br />

et al., 2004). This interventi<strong>on</strong> package<br />

incorporated Tucker et al.’s (1998) recommendati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

to fade initially high rates of reinforcement<br />

to lower rates <strong>and</strong> to combine NCR<br />

with educati<strong>on</strong>al interventi<strong>on</strong>s. Further, this<br />

study evaluated a modificati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> how NCR is<br />

typically implemented.<br />

This study has some limitati<strong>on</strong>s. First, due to<br />

the implementati<strong>on</strong> of this study during the<br />

summer, follow-up data was collected <strong>on</strong>ly<br />

<strong>on</strong>ce, three weeks following the cessati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>. It is not possible to assess the<br />

l<strong>on</strong>g term outcomes of this interventi<strong>on</strong> without<br />

the collecti<strong>on</strong> of maintenance data several<br />

m<strong>on</strong>ths following the cessati<strong>on</strong> of interventi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Further, the researchers did not systematically<br />

collect data <strong>on</strong> the independent use of<br />

word approximati<strong>on</strong>s. Though the researchers<br />

noted anecdotally that Ari began using<br />

independent word approximati<strong>on</strong>s near the<br />

end of interventi<strong>on</strong>, it would have been useful<br />

to determine the extent to which he did so.<br />

Clearly, many children with ASD will require<br />

far more intensive communicati<strong>on</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Another limitati<strong>on</strong> is the use of interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s when measuring generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> follow up. Without a true return to<br />

baseline c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s, it is unclear whether or<br />

not Josiah <strong>and</strong> Ari would c<strong>on</strong>tinue to make<br />

independent requests without c<strong>on</strong>tinued<br />

training, though they did dem<strong>on</strong>strate use of<br />

previously learned skills with minimal instructi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

A final limitati<strong>on</strong> is the use of a multielement<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>; because we always combined<br />

NCR with verbal modeling, it may be<br />

the case that modeling al<strong>on</strong>e would have been<br />

sufficient <strong>and</strong> that NCR was irrelevant. Without<br />

investigating each comp<strong>on</strong>ent separately,<br />

we are unable to make the case that the combined<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> is necessary.<br />

The current study included a small sample<br />

of children <strong>and</strong> was an initial dem<strong>on</strong>strati<strong>on</strong><br />

of the combinati<strong>on</strong> of naturalistic teaching<br />

<strong>and</strong> NCR. Further replicati<strong>on</strong> is needed to<br />

generalize the results <strong>and</strong> refine the procedures.<br />

Future research is needed with children<br />

with varied characteristics, within differ-<br />

604 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


ent settings, such as inclusive learning<br />

envir<strong>on</strong>ments, <strong>and</strong> with larger groups of children.<br />

Future research is needed to investigate<br />

whether NCR would affect more complex<br />

communicati<strong>on</strong> behaviors.<br />

Another area of future research is the use of<br />

NCR combined with verbal modeling by parents,<br />

caregivers, <strong>and</strong> teachers in natural settings<br />

such as home <strong>and</strong> childcare settings.<br />

Implementati<strong>on</strong> in these settings might allow<br />

for varied <strong>and</strong> more complex requests than<br />

were allowed with the design of the current<br />

study. Future research might investigate the<br />

ease with which these strategies might be implemented<br />

by parents, caregivers, <strong>and</strong> teachers<br />

as well as their effectiveness in increasing<br />

others types of communicati<strong>on</strong> behaviors<br />

across settings.<br />

References<br />

American Psychiatric Associati<strong>on</strong> [APA]. (2000). Diagnostic<br />

<strong>and</strong> statistical manual of mental disorders (4 th<br />

ed., text rev.). Washingt<strong>on</strong>, DC: Author.<br />

Butler, L. R., & Luiselli, J. K. (2007). Escape-maintained<br />

problem behavior in a child with autism:<br />

Antecedent functi<strong>on</strong>al analysis <strong>and</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

evaluati<strong>on</strong> of n<strong>on</strong>c<strong>on</strong>tingent escape <strong>and</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

fading. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventi<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

9, 195–202.<br />

Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2000). Joint attenti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

cultural learning, <strong>and</strong> language acquisiti<strong>on</strong>:<br />

Implicati<strong>on</strong>s for children with autism (pp. 31–<br />

54). In (A. M. Wetherby & B. M. Prizant, Eds.),<br />

<strong>Autism</strong> spectrum disorders: A transacti<strong>on</strong>al developmental<br />

perspective. Baltimore, MD: Paul H Brookes<br />

Publishing Co.<br />

Carr, J. E., Dozier, C. L., Patel, M. R., Adams, A. N.,<br />

& Martin, M. (2002). Treatment of automatically<br />

reinforced object mouthing with n<strong>on</strong>c<strong>on</strong>tingent<br />

reinforcement <strong>and</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se blocking: Experimental<br />

analysis <strong>and</strong> social validati<strong>on</strong>. Research in<br />

<strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 23, 37–44.<br />

Charlop, M. H., Schreibman, L., & Try<strong>on</strong>, A. S.<br />

(1983). Learning through observati<strong>on</strong>: The effects<br />

of peer modeling <strong>on</strong> acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

in autistic children. Journal of Abnormal<br />

Child Psychology, 11, 355–366.<br />

Charlop-Christy, M. H., & Carpenter, M. H. (2000).<br />

Modified incidental teaching sessi<strong>on</strong>s: A procedure<br />

for parents to increase sp<strong>on</strong>taneous speech<br />

in their children with autism. Journal of Positive<br />

Behavior Interventi<strong>on</strong>s, 2, 98–112.<br />

Egel, A. L., Richman, G. S., & Koegel, R. L. (1981).<br />

Normal peer models <strong>and</strong> autistic children’s learning.<br />

Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 14, 3–12.<br />

Gena, A., Couloura, S., & Kymissis, E. (2005). Modifying<br />

the affective behavior or preschoolers with<br />

autism using in vivo or video modeling <strong>and</strong> reinforcement<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tingencies. Journal of <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>Developmental</strong> Disorders, 35, 545–556.<br />

Ganz, J. B., Heath, A. K., Rispoli, M. J., & Earles-<br />

Vollrath, T. (2010). Impact of AAC versus verbal<br />

modeling <strong>on</strong> verbal imitati<strong>on</strong>, picture discriminati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> related speech: A pilot investigati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Journal of <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>and</strong> Physical <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 22,<br />

179–196. doi: 10.1007/s10882-009-9176-2.<br />

Heflin, L. J., & Alaimo, D. F. (2007). Students with<br />

autism spectrum disorders: Effective instructi<strong>on</strong>al practices.<br />

Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pears<strong>on</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

Inc.<br />

Jahr, E., Elevik, S., & Eikeseth, S. (2000). Teaching<br />

children with autism to initiate <strong>and</strong> sustain cooperative<br />

play. Research in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>,<br />

21, 151–169.<br />

Janzen, J. E. (2003). Underst<strong>and</strong>ing the nature of autism:<br />

A guide to the autism spectrum disorders (2 nd ed.).<br />

San Ant<strong>on</strong>io, TX: Therapy Skill Builders.<br />

Kazdin, A.E. (1982). Single-case research designs: Methods<br />

for clinical <strong>and</strong> applied settings. New York: Oxford<br />

University Press.<br />

Koegel, L., Koegel, R., Shoshan, Y., & McNerney, E.<br />

(1999). Pivotal resp<strong>on</strong>se interventi<strong>on</strong> II: Preliminary<br />

l<strong>on</strong>g-term outcome data. Journal of the Associati<strong>on</strong><br />

for Pers<strong>on</strong>s with Severe H<strong>and</strong>icaps, 24, 186–<br />

198.<br />

Lalli, J. S., Casey, S., & Kates, K. (1995). Reducing<br />

escape behavior <strong>and</strong> increasing task completi<strong>on</strong><br />

with functi<strong>on</strong>al communicati<strong>on</strong> training, extincti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>se chaining. Journal of Applied<br />

Behavior Analysis, 28, 261–268.<br />

Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., & Risi, S.<br />

(1999). <strong>Autism</strong> Diagnostic Observati<strong>on</strong> Schedule. Los<br />

Angeles: Western Psychological Services.<br />

Marcus, B. A., & Vollmer, T. R., (1996). Combining<br />

n<strong>on</strong>c<strong>on</strong>tingent reinforcement <strong>and</strong> differential reinforcement<br />

schedules as treatment for aberrant<br />

behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29,<br />

43–51.<br />

Mats<strong>on</strong>, J. L., Mats<strong>on</strong>, M. L., & Rivet, T. T. (2007).<br />

Social-skills treatments for students with autism<br />

spectrum disorders: An overview. Behavior Modificati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

31, 682–707.<br />

McGee, G. G., Almeida, M. C., Sulzer-Azaroff, B., &<br />

Feldman, R. S. (1992). Promoting reciprocal interacti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

via peer incidental teaching. Journal of<br />

Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 117–126.<br />

McGee, G. G., Krantz, P. J., Mas<strong>on</strong>, D., & McClannahan,<br />

L. E. (1983). A modified incidental-teaching<br />

procedure for autistic youth: Acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

generalizati<strong>on</strong> of receptive object labels. Journal of<br />

Applied Behavior Analysis, 16, 329–388.<br />

Mild<strong>on</strong>, R. L., Moore, D. W., & Dix<strong>on</strong>, R. S. (2004).<br />

Combining n<strong>on</strong>c<strong>on</strong>tingent escape <strong>and</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Multi-Comp<strong>on</strong>ent Communicati<strong>on</strong> Interventi<strong>on</strong> / 605


communicati<strong>on</strong> training as a treatment for negatively<br />

reinforced disruptive behavior. Journal of<br />

Positive Behavior Interventi<strong>on</strong>s, 6, 92–102.<br />

Mobayed, K. L., Collins, B. C., Strangis, D. E., Schuster,<br />

J. W., & Hemmeter, M. L. (2000). Teaching<br />

parents to employ m<strong>and</strong>-model procedures to<br />

teach their children requesting. Journal of Early<br />

Interventi<strong>on</strong>, 23, 165–179.<br />

Nati<strong>on</strong>al Research Council. (2001). Educating children<br />

with autism. Committee <strong>on</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong>al Interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for Children with <strong>Autism</strong>. C. Lord & J. P.<br />

McGee (Eds.). <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> of Behavioral <strong>and</strong> Social<br />

Sciences <strong>and</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong>. Washingt<strong>on</strong>, DC: Nati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Academy Press.<br />

Ogletree, B. T. (2008). The communicative c<strong>on</strong>text<br />

of autism. In R. L. Simps<strong>on</strong> & B. S. Myles (Eds.),<br />

Educating children <strong>and</strong> youth with autism: Strategies<br />

for effective practice, 2 nd ed. (pp. 223–265). Austin,<br />

TX: PRO-ED, Inc.<br />

Parker, R. I., Vannest, K. J., & Brown, L. (2009). The<br />

Improvement Rate Difference for single case research.<br />

Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, 75, 135–150.<br />

Roscoe, E. M., Iwata, B. A., & Goh, H. (1998). A<br />

comparis<strong>on</strong> of n<strong>on</strong>c<strong>on</strong>tingent reinforcement <strong>and</strong><br />

sensory extincti<strong>on</strong> as treatments for self-injurious<br />

behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31,<br />

635–6<str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>.<br />

Ross, D. E. & Greer, R. D. (2003). Generalized<br />

imitati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> the m<strong>and</strong>: Inducing first instances<br />

of speech in young children with autism. Research<br />

in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 24, 58–74.<br />

Schopler, E., Reichler, R. J., & Renner, B. R. (1988).<br />

The Childhood <strong>Autism</strong> Rating Scale (CARS). Los Angeles,<br />

CA: Western Psychological Services.<br />

Stahmer, A. C., Ingersoll, B., & Carter, C. (2003).<br />

Behavioral approaches to promoting play. <strong>Autism</strong>,<br />

7, 401–413.<br />

Try<strong>on</strong>, A. S., & Keane, S. P. (1986). Promoting<br />

imitative play through generalized observati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

learning in autistic-like children. Journal of Abnormal<br />

Child Psychology, 14, 537–549.<br />

Tucker, M., Sigafoos, J., & Bushnell, H. (1998). Use<br />

of n<strong>on</strong>c<strong>on</strong>tingent reinforcement in the treatment<br />

of challenging behavior: A review <strong>and</strong> clinical<br />

guide. Behavior Modificati<strong>on</strong>, 22, 529–547.<br />

Received: 31 August 2010<br />

Initial Acceptance: 21 October 2010<br />

Final Acceptance: 12 December 2011<br />

606 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 2011, <str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>(4), 607-618<br />

© <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Effects of Presessi<strong>on</strong> Satiati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> Challenging Behavior <strong>and</strong><br />

Academic Engagement for Children with <strong>Autism</strong> during<br />

Classroom Instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

M<strong>and</strong>y J. Rispoli<br />

Texas A&MUniversity<br />

Jeff Sigafoos<br />

Victoria University of Wellingt<strong>on</strong><br />

Soye<strong>on</strong> Kang<br />

University of Texas at Austin<br />

Richard Parker<br />

Texas A&MUniversity<br />

Mark F. O’Reilly<br />

University of Texas at Austin<br />

Russell Lang<br />

Texas State University<br />

Giulio Lanci<strong>on</strong>i<br />

University of Bari<br />

Abstract: We evaluated the effects of presessi<strong>on</strong> satiati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> challenging behavior <strong>and</strong> academic engagement<br />

during subsequent classroom activities for three 5–6 year-old children with autism. The percentage of 10-s<br />

intervals with challenging behavior <strong>and</strong> academic engagement during 20-min classroom activity sessi<strong>on</strong>s was<br />

observed under two c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s. One c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> involved presessi<strong>on</strong> satiati<strong>on</strong>, in which participants were given<br />

unrestricted access to tangible items that maintained their challenging behavior prior to the classroom sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

This presessi<strong>on</strong> satiati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>tinued until the children rejected the tangible item three times. The sec<strong>on</strong>d c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong><br />

did not entail presessi<strong>on</strong> satiati<strong>on</strong> prior to the beginning of classroom sessi<strong>on</strong>s. Effects of the two c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong><br />

challenging behavior <strong>and</strong> academic engagement were evaluated using individual participant alternating<br />

treatments designs. Results dem<strong>on</strong>strated that the presessi<strong>on</strong> satiati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> was associated with c<strong>on</strong>sistently<br />

lower percentages of intervals with challenging behavior <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sistently higher percentages of intervals with<br />

academic engagement.<br />

Children with autism spectrum disorder<br />

(ASD) often engage in challenging behaviors,<br />

which can lead to social isolati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> decreased<br />

time spent in instructi<strong>on</strong> (Horner,<br />

Albin, Sprague, & Todd, 2000). Antecedent<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>s have been successfully used to<br />

decrease challenging behavior (Kern,<br />

Choutka, & Sokol, 2002). Antecedent interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

involve altering the envir<strong>on</strong>ment to<br />

eliminate variables that set the occasi<strong>on</strong> for<br />

challenging behavior (Luiselli, 2006). Recently,<br />

a subcategory of antecedent interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for challenging behavior has gained at-<br />

Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence c<strong>on</strong>cerning this article should<br />

be addressed to M<strong>and</strong>y J. Rispoli, Texas A & M<br />

University, 4225 TAMU, College Stati<strong>on</strong>, TX 77843.<br />

Email: mrispoli@tamu.edu<br />

tenti<strong>on</strong>. This category focuses <strong>on</strong> addressing<br />

an individual’s motivati<strong>on</strong> to engage in a specific<br />

behavior <strong>and</strong> is referred to as the manipulati<strong>on</strong><br />

of motivating operati<strong>on</strong>s (MO) (Laraway,<br />

Snycerski, Michael, & Poling, 2003). An<br />

MO is an event that alters the value of reinforcement<br />

<strong>and</strong> the frequency of behavior previously<br />

correlated with such reinforcement<br />

(Laraway, et al. 2003).<br />

In some cases, biological events, such as<br />

hunger, lack of sleep, or illness can functi<strong>on</strong><br />

as MOs by altering an individual’s motivati<strong>on</strong><br />

to engage in a specific behavior (e.g. Carr &<br />

Smith, 1995; O’Reilly, 1995; O’Reilly, 1997).<br />

For example, Kennedy <strong>and</strong> Meyer (1996) c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

a functi<strong>on</strong>al analysis of three individual’s<br />

challenging behavior across several weeks.<br />

They found that when the participants had<br />

less sleep (two participants) or were experi-<br />

Effects of Presessi<strong>on</strong> Satiati<strong>on</strong> / 607


encing allergy symptoms (<strong>on</strong>e participant),<br />

they engaged in higher levels of challenging<br />

behavior in relati<strong>on</strong> to instructi<strong>on</strong>al dem<strong>and</strong>s.<br />

Thus, these biological-setting events appeared<br />

to make dem<strong>and</strong>s more aversive, thereby increasing<br />

the participant’s motivati<strong>on</strong> to escape<br />

or avoid those tasks. This change in motivati<strong>on</strong><br />

resulted in an increase in challenging<br />

behavior, which had in the past enabled the<br />

individuals to escape from or avoid the tasks.<br />

Previous access to reinforcement can also<br />

impact an individual’s motivati<strong>on</strong> to engage in<br />

challenging behavior. For example, McComas,<br />

Thomps<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> Johns<strong>on</strong> (2003) examined<br />

the effects of prior attenti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> levels of<br />

challenging behavior. Participants were exposed<br />

to a functi<strong>on</strong>al analysis procedure similar<br />

to that described by Iwata <strong>and</strong> colleagues<br />

(1982/1994), following 10-min of presessi<strong>on</strong><br />

attenti<strong>on</strong> or following 10-min of no attenti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Results showed that the functi<strong>on</strong>al analysis<br />

that followed presessi<strong>on</strong> access to attenti<strong>on</strong><br />

was associated with lower levels of challenging<br />

behavior than the functi<strong>on</strong>al analysis following<br />

presessi<strong>on</strong> periods without attenti<strong>on</strong>. Presessi<strong>on</strong><br />

attenti<strong>on</strong> may have functi<strong>on</strong>ed as an<br />

abolishing operati<strong>on</strong> (i.e., satiati<strong>on</strong>) thus reducing<br />

levels of attenti<strong>on</strong>-seeking behavior<br />

while no presessi<strong>on</strong> attenti<strong>on</strong> may have functi<strong>on</strong>ed<br />

as an establishing operati<strong>on</strong> (i.e., deprivati<strong>on</strong>)<br />

<strong>and</strong> thus subsequently increasing<br />

the value of attenti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> the frequency of<br />

attenti<strong>on</strong>-maintained challenging behavior.<br />

A core diagnostic feature for autism spectrum<br />

disorder according to the Diagnostic<br />

<strong>and</strong> Statistical Manual (DSM-IV) is engagement<br />

in compulsive or repetitive behaviors<br />

(American Psychiatric Associati<strong>on</strong>, 1994). Previous<br />

research has dem<strong>on</strong>strated that individuals<br />

with autism can engage in these repetitive<br />

interacti<strong>on</strong>s with tangible items. For example,<br />

Reese <strong>and</strong> colleagues (2003) c<strong>on</strong>ducted a<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>al assessment interview with caregivers<br />

of 100 young children with ASD. They<br />

found that 85% of participants engaged in<br />

perseverative behavior with 30% of the children<br />

engaging in challenging behavior in order<br />

to access perseverative activities. Based <strong>on</strong><br />

these findings, it may be important to c<strong>on</strong>sider<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>s that can successfully decrease<br />

the motivati<strong>on</strong> to engage in such perseverative<br />

activities am<strong>on</strong>g individuals with<br />

ASD.<br />

Over the course of several studies, we have<br />

developed a methodology to evaluate <strong>and</strong><br />

treat challenging behavior by altering the individual’s<br />

motivati<strong>on</strong> to engage in such behavior<br />

(O’Reilly, et al. 2008; O’Reilly et al. 2009;<br />

Lang, et al. 2009; Lang, et al. in press; Rispoli<br />

et al. in press). This methodology involves<br />

c<strong>on</strong>ducting an analogue functi<strong>on</strong>al analysis to<br />

determine the maintaining c<strong>on</strong>sequence for<br />

the individual’s challenging behavior. Next,<br />

we evaluate the child’s m<strong>and</strong>s to determine<br />

when they have reached a level of satiati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Communicati<strong>on</strong> of satiati<strong>on</strong> was defined as<br />

the individual’s rejecti<strong>on</strong> of the maintaining<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sequence (e.g., a highly preferred toy) of<br />

their challenging behavior. Satiati<strong>on</strong> was believed<br />

to be achieved <strong>on</strong>ce the participant<br />

rejected the toy three times. Following the<br />

third rejecti<strong>on</strong>, the child enters into the problematic<br />

situati<strong>on</strong> that was previously associated<br />

with challenging behavior. Results from<br />

these studies dem<strong>on</strong>strated that the participants<br />

engaged in lower levels of challenging<br />

behavior following the satiati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong><br />

when compared to a no presessi<strong>on</strong> access c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

The purpose of this study was to replicate<br />

<strong>and</strong> extend these procedures to classroom settings.<br />

Specifically, we evaluated the influence<br />

of presessi<strong>on</strong> satiati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> levels of challenging<br />

behavior <strong>and</strong> academic engagement for three<br />

boys with autism in their classrooms during<br />

group instructi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Method<br />

Participants<br />

Three boys with autism participated in this<br />

study. Participants were referred to the study<br />

by their teachers or administrators who reported<br />

that participants engaged in challenging<br />

behavior during group instructi<strong>on</strong> when<br />

preferred items were in sight but unavailable.<br />

Preferred items were identified via a preference<br />

assessment described below. Each participant<br />

was assessed using the Vinel<strong>and</strong> Adaptive<br />

Behavior Scales (Sparrow, Balla &<br />

Cicchetti, 1984) <strong>and</strong> the Childhood <strong>Autism</strong><br />

Rating Scale (Schopler, Reichler, & Renner,<br />

1988). Analogue functi<strong>on</strong>al analyses (Iwata, et<br />

al. 1994) were c<strong>on</strong>ducted to assess the functi<strong>on</strong><br />

of each participant’s challenging behav-<br />

608 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


TABLE 1<br />

Participant Characteristics<br />

Participant Gender Age Ethnicity Diagnosis CARS<br />

Jacob Male 5 African American PDD-NOS 33 (mild to<br />

moderate<br />

autism)<br />

Geoffrey Male 6 Asian American <strong>Autism</strong> 30.5 (mild to<br />

moderate<br />

autism)<br />

D<strong>on</strong>ovan Male 6 African American <strong>Autism</strong>, speech<br />

impairment<br />

iors. Table 1 provides participant descripti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with respect to gender, age, ethnicity, diagnosis,<br />

<strong>and</strong> diagnostic assessment results.<br />

Jacob was a 5-year-old African-American<br />

male diagnosed with Pervasive <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

Disorder, Not Otherwise Specified (PDD-<br />

NOS). Jacob attended a self-c<strong>on</strong>tained classroom<br />

in a private school for children with<br />

disabilities. Jacob scored 33 <strong>on</strong> the CARS,<br />

which placed him in the mild to moderate<br />

range <strong>on</strong> the autism spectrum. His overall<br />

adaptive age equivalency <strong>on</strong> the Vinel<strong>and</strong><br />

Adaptive Behavior Scales was 2 years 9<br />

m<strong>on</strong>ths. Jacob communicated sp<strong>on</strong>taneously<br />

using 5 to 6 word phrases. A preferred toy for<br />

Jacob was a toy microph<strong>on</strong>e. When left al<strong>on</strong>e<br />

with the microph<strong>on</strong>e, Jacob would repeatedly<br />

sing the same s<strong>on</strong>g, “Twinkle, Twinkle, Little<br />

Star.” If Jacob was unable to access the microph<strong>on</strong>e<br />

he would engage in challenging behavior<br />

including (a) elopement: moving at least 2<br />

feet away from the instructi<strong>on</strong>al area without<br />

the teacher’s permissi<strong>on</strong>, (b) aggressi<strong>on</strong>: striking<br />

others with h<strong>and</strong> or object, <strong>and</strong> (c) protesting:<br />

loud vocalizati<strong>on</strong>s including “no,”<br />

“stop,” “go away,” or crying.<br />

Geoffrey was a 6-year-old Asian-American<br />

male diagnosed with autism. Geoffrey attended<br />

an inclusive kindergarten class in a<br />

small private school. He received a score of<br />

30.5 <strong>on</strong> the CARS, which placed him in the<br />

mild to moderate range of autism <strong>and</strong> had an<br />

overall age equivalency of 1 year 8 m<strong>on</strong>ths <strong>on</strong><br />

the Vinel<strong>and</strong> Adaptive Behavior Scales. Geoffrey<br />

communicated using two to three word<br />

utterances <strong>and</strong> manual signs. Geoffrey at-<br />

49 (severe<br />

autism)<br />

Vinel<strong>and</strong><br />

adaptive age<br />

equivalent<br />

2 years 9<br />

m<strong>on</strong>ths<br />

Vinel<strong>and</strong> adaptive<br />

behavior composite<br />

st<strong>and</strong>ard score<br />

tended an inclusi<strong>on</strong> classroom for part of his<br />

day. In this classroom, Geoffrey often played<br />

with a plastic ball slide. Geoffrey would watch<br />

the balls roll down the slide <strong>and</strong> count each<br />

ball. When access to the ball slide was prevented,<br />

Geoffrey’s challenging behavior included<br />

(a) h<strong>and</strong> mouthing: placement of fingers<br />

past the plane of the lips in order to suck<br />

or bite, <strong>and</strong> (b) elopement: pushing his body<br />

away from table or teacher in an attempt to<br />

leave the instructi<strong>on</strong>al area.<br />

D<strong>on</strong>ovan was a 6-year-old African-American<br />

male diagnosed with autism <strong>and</strong> speech impairment.<br />

D<strong>on</strong>ovan attended a self-c<strong>on</strong>tained<br />

classroom for students with autism in a public<br />

school. D<strong>on</strong>ovan obtained a score of 49 <strong>on</strong> the<br />

CARS, which placed him in the severe range<br />

of autism. His overall adaptive age equivalency<br />

was 1 year <strong>on</strong> the Vinel<strong>and</strong> Adaptive Behavior<br />

Scales. D<strong>on</strong>ovan communicated requests by<br />

leading adults by the h<strong>and</strong> to items or activities<br />

in the room. Following verbal prompts,<br />

D<strong>on</strong>ovan could request items using vocal approximati<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

D<strong>on</strong>ovan’s preferred toy in the<br />

classroom was a Magna Doodle®. D<strong>on</strong>ovan<br />

would write the capital <strong>and</strong> lower case form of<br />

letters <strong>on</strong> the Magna Doodle®. When he<br />

could not access the Magna Doodle®, D<strong>on</strong>ovan<br />

would engage in challenging behaviors<br />

including (a) elopement: rising from his chair<br />

in an attempt to leave the instructi<strong>on</strong>al area,<br />

(b) aggressi<strong>on</strong>: biting or attempting to bite<br />

others, <strong>and</strong>/or hitting others with his h<strong>and</strong>s,<br />

<strong>and</strong> (c) jumping up <strong>and</strong> down <strong>on</strong> his toes<br />

repetitively.<br />

64<br />

1 year 8 m<strong>on</strong>ths 40<br />

1 year 0 m<strong>on</strong>ths 37<br />

Effects of Presessi<strong>on</strong> Satiati<strong>on</strong> / 609


Assessments<br />

Preference assessment. Classroom teachers<br />

<strong>and</strong> parents were asked to provide a list of<br />

eight toys that they believed the participant<br />

preferred while at school. The participant’s<br />

preference for these items was then assessed<br />

using a paired choice preference assessment<br />

(Fisher et al., 1992). Specifically, participants<br />

were presented with two toys at a time <strong>and</strong> the<br />

toy that the participant selected was recorded.<br />

Each of the eight items was paired with each<br />

of the other items in a r<strong>and</strong>omized sequence<br />

with the locati<strong>on</strong> of each item alternated between<br />

the left <strong>and</strong> right side of the table to<br />

c<strong>on</strong>trol for potential positi<strong>on</strong> biases. Trials<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tinued until each toy had been paired with<br />

all other toys in each possible positi<strong>on</strong>. The<br />

percentage of opportunities in which each toy<br />

was selected was calculated to establish a rank<br />

order of toys from most to least preferred.<br />

Each participant’s most preferred toy was used<br />

in the tangible c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> of the functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

analysis <strong>and</strong> in the presessi<strong>on</strong> satiati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Functi<strong>on</strong>al analysis. Eligibility for participati<strong>on</strong><br />

in this study required evidence that<br />

each child’s challenging behavior was maintained<br />

by positive reinforcement in the form<br />

of access to preferred toys. Analogue functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

analyses were c<strong>on</strong>ducted with each participant<br />

to determine the maintaining c<strong>on</strong>sequence(s)<br />

for his challenging behavior using<br />

individual multielement experimental designs<br />

(Barlow, Nock, & Hersen, 2009). The functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

analysis c<strong>on</strong>sisted of four 5-min c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s:<br />

(a) attenti<strong>on</strong>, (b) tangible, (c) escape,<br />

<strong>and</strong> (d) play. The sequence of these c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

was held c<strong>on</strong>stant across each participant.<br />

Procedures were similar to those described<br />

by Iwata et al. (1994), however, an<br />

al<strong>on</strong>e c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> was not c<strong>on</strong>ducted because of<br />

school policy (children were not allowed to go<br />

unsupervised).<br />

Analogue functi<strong>on</strong>al analyses were c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

in a c<strong>on</strong>ference room or an empty<br />

classroom within each participant’s school.<br />

These rooms c<strong>on</strong>tained a table <strong>and</strong> at least<br />

three chairs <strong>and</strong> were void of extraneous instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

or play materials. During the functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

analysis materials related to the assessment<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s were present. These<br />

included papers for the researcher to “read”<br />

during the attenti<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> of the functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

analysis, the most preferred item identified<br />

via a paired choice preference assessment<br />

(Fisher et al., 1992) for the tangible<br />

phase, <strong>and</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>al materials related to<br />

the participant’s Individualized Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

Program (IEP) goals. Instructi<strong>on</strong>al materials<br />

c<strong>on</strong>sisted of items such as shapes, letters, lacing<br />

cards, picture cards of animals, <strong>and</strong> colored<br />

blocks.<br />

Results of the analogue functi<strong>on</strong>al analyses<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strated that each participant’s challenging<br />

behavior was maintained, at least in<br />

part, by access to their most preferred toy.<br />

(Functi<strong>on</strong>al analysis sessi<strong>on</strong> by sessi<strong>on</strong> data are<br />

available from the corresp<strong>on</strong>ding author<br />

up<strong>on</strong> request). Jacob engaged in challenging<br />

behavior in the tangible (M 77%; range<br />

50% to 100%) <strong>and</strong> attenti<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s (M <br />

19%; range 3% to 33%) of the analogue functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

analysis. Geoffrey’s challenging behavior<br />

occurred primarily in the tangible c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong><br />

(M 22%; range 3% to 43%) with lower<br />

levels of challenging behavior in the dem<strong>and</strong><br />

(M 4%; range 3% to 17%), <strong>and</strong> attenti<strong>on</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s (M 3%; range 3% to 13%). D<strong>on</strong>ovan’s<br />

challenging behavior occurred primarily<br />

in the tangible c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> (M 43%; range<br />

20% to 70%) with lower levels of challenging<br />

behavior in the attenti<strong>on</strong> (M 3%; range 0%<br />

to 10%), <strong>and</strong> play c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s (M 5%; range<br />

0% to 27%). D<strong>on</strong>ovan also showed an increasing<br />

trend in challenging behavior during the<br />

dem<strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> (M 32%; range 3% to<br />

77% of intervals) of the functi<strong>on</strong>al analysis.<br />

Communicati<strong>on</strong> of rejecti<strong>on</strong>. Following the<br />

analogue functi<strong>on</strong>al analysis, behavioral indicators<br />

of satiati<strong>on</strong> were assessed according to<br />

the methodology described by O’Reilly et al<br />

(2009). Teachers <strong>and</strong> parents were asked to<br />

identify how each participant communicated<br />

they no l<strong>on</strong>ger wanted to play with a toy or<br />

engage in an activity. To verify that participants<br />

used these behaviors to communicate<br />

rejecti<strong>on</strong>, each participant was exposed to two<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s: (a) access to a highly preferred<br />

item <strong>and</strong> (b) access to a n<strong>on</strong>-preferred item.<br />

Each c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> lasted 10 min <strong>and</strong> was c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

five times with each participant using<br />

an alternating treatments experimental design<br />

(Barlow et al., 2009).<br />

In the highly preferred item c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, the<br />

participant was presented with a highly pre-<br />

610 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


TABLE 2<br />

Operati<strong>on</strong>alized Definiti<strong>on</strong>s for Target Behaviors<br />

Participant Challenging behavior Rejecting Behavior<br />

Jacob Moving at least 2 feet from instructi<strong>on</strong>al area,<br />

hitting others with h<strong>and</strong> or object, verbal<br />

protesting/crying<br />

Geoffrey H<strong>and</strong> mouthing, moving h<strong>and</strong>s in air<br />

repetitively, pushing away from<br />

table/therapist<br />

D<strong>on</strong>ovan Lifts bottom off chair, biting or attempts to<br />

bite, hitting others with h<strong>and</strong>s, jumping up<br />

<strong>and</strong> down<br />

ferred item, identified during the paired<br />

choice preference assessment. Using a partial<br />

interval recording system, data were collected<br />

<strong>on</strong> the percentage of 10-s intervals in which<br />

the participant engaged in the identified rejecting<br />

behavior. The first author was present<br />

during each sessi<strong>on</strong> but did not interact with<br />

the participant except to re-present the item<br />

to the participant following rejecting behavior.<br />

In the n<strong>on</strong>-preferred item c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, the<br />

participant was presented with a n<strong>on</strong>-preferred<br />

item. This item was the lowest ranked<br />

item according to the results of the paired<br />

choice preference assessment. Procedures<br />

during this c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> were identical to those<br />

used in the highly preferred item c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> data were collected <strong>on</strong> the percentage of<br />

10-s intervals in which the participant engaged<br />

in the identified rejecting behavior. Table 2<br />

provides operati<strong>on</strong>al definiti<strong>on</strong>s for each participant’s<br />

challenging behaviors <strong>and</strong> rejecting<br />

behavior.<br />

Presessi<strong>on</strong> Satiati<strong>on</strong> versus No Presessi<strong>on</strong> Access<br />

C<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

Participants were exposed to two c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

(a) presessi<strong>on</strong> satiati<strong>on</strong> of preferred toy versus<br />

(b) no presessi<strong>on</strong> access to the preferred toy<br />

(i.e., deprivati<strong>on</strong>) prior to 20-min classroom<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong>s. An alternating treatments design<br />

(Barlow et al., 2009) was used to compare the<br />

effects of the two c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> each participant’s<br />

subsequent challenging behavior <strong>and</strong><br />

academic engagement during the 20 min<br />

classroom sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Says “He’s all d<strong>on</strong>e,” or<br />

“Finished”<br />

Pushes item away<br />

Walks 2 feet from item<br />

Setting <strong>and</strong> materials. Classroom sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

were c<strong>on</strong>ducted in the participant’s typical<br />

classroom during group instructi<strong>on</strong>al activities.<br />

These instructi<strong>on</strong>al activities varied across<br />

participants but were held c<strong>on</strong>stant for each<br />

participant. For example, group instructi<strong>on</strong> in<br />

D<strong>on</strong>ovan’s <strong>and</strong> Jacob’s class activities included<br />

listening to a story, watching a video, <strong>and</strong><br />

building with blocks. Group instructi<strong>on</strong> for<br />

Geoffrey focused <strong>on</strong> academic skills such as<br />

completing ph<strong>on</strong>ics worksheets, writing in a<br />

journal, <strong>and</strong> listening to poems. During the<br />

classroom sessi<strong>on</strong>s, three to four students were<br />

seated at a table or <strong>on</strong> the floor with access to<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>al materials. The item maintaining<br />

challenging behavior (i.e., each participant’s<br />

preferred tangible toy) was visible but not accessible<br />

during the classroom sessi<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

Data collecti<strong>on</strong>. Two dependent variables,<br />

challenging behavior <strong>and</strong> academic engagement,<br />

were defined <strong>and</strong> measured in this<br />

study. Challenging behavior was defined individually<br />

for each participant. Operati<strong>on</strong>alized<br />

definiti<strong>on</strong>s of these behaviors are presented in<br />

Table 2. Challenging behavior data were collected<br />

using 10-s partial interval recording.<br />

Academic engagement was defined as being<br />

appropriately involved with the instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

materials (c.f., O’Reilly et al., 2005). Appropriate<br />

involvement required that the participant<br />

engage with the materials in the manner<br />

intended, for example placing puzzle pieces<br />

in a puzzle, looking at picture books, <strong>and</strong><br />

placing pegs into pegboards. Academic engagement<br />

was measured during classroom sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

using 10-s whole interval recording.<br />

Effects of Presessi<strong>on</strong> Satiati<strong>on</strong> / 611


Interobserver agreement. A sec<strong>on</strong>d observer<br />

simultaneously <strong>and</strong> independently recorded<br />

data <strong>on</strong> the target behaviors for at least 30% of<br />

all sessi<strong>on</strong>s for each participant during each<br />

phase of the study. Interobserver agreement<br />

(IOA) was calculated by dividing the total<br />

number of agreements for each interval by the<br />

total number of intervals. IOA scores for each<br />

sessi<strong>on</strong> were added together <strong>and</strong> divided by<br />

the total number of sessi<strong>on</strong>s in which reliability<br />

data were gathered in order to calculate<br />

the overall mean IOA. The mean IOA combined<br />

across all sessi<strong>on</strong>s, dependent variables,<br />

<strong>and</strong> participants was 96% (range, 82% to<br />

100%).<br />

Procedural fidelity. Task analyses of the procedural<br />

steps were created for each phase of<br />

this study. Procedural fidelity was calculated<br />

by dividing the number of steps completed<br />

correctly by the total number of steps in the<br />

procedure <strong>and</strong> multiplying by 100%. Procedural<br />

fidelity was assessed by an independent<br />

observer for 33% of sessi<strong>on</strong>s for each participant<br />

in each phase of the study with 100%<br />

correct implementati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Procedure<br />

Presessi<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s. During the presessi<strong>on</strong><br />

satiati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, participants were<br />

given free access to their preferred tangible<br />

item in an empty c<strong>on</strong>ference room or classroom<br />

within the school. The first author<br />

h<strong>and</strong>ed the preferred item to the participant<br />

<strong>and</strong> instructed him to play with the item. The<br />

first author remained in the room but did not<br />

provide any additi<strong>on</strong>al attenti<strong>on</strong> or instructi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

If the participant engaged in rejecting<br />

behavior, the first author re-presented the<br />

item to the participant. This procedure was<br />

followed for the first <strong>and</strong> sec<strong>on</strong>d instances of<br />

rejecting behavior. Following three rejecti<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

the presessi<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> was terminated <strong>and</strong><br />

the participant immediately entered into the<br />

classroom sessi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

The no presessi<strong>on</strong> access c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> involved<br />

prohibiting the participant from accessing<br />

the preferred tangible prior to the<br />

classroom sessi<strong>on</strong> that day. The participant<br />

participated in all school routines, but did not<br />

have access to his or her preferred tangible<br />

item. Classroom sessi<strong>on</strong>s were held at the<br />

same time of day for each participant. This<br />

allowed the durati<strong>on</strong> of the no presessi<strong>on</strong> access<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> to remain c<strong>on</strong>stant for each<br />

participant.<br />

Classroom sessi<strong>on</strong>s. Classroom sessi<strong>on</strong>s immediately<br />

followed presessi<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

During classroom sessi<strong>on</strong>s, participants were<br />

seated at a table or <strong>on</strong> the floor near three to<br />

four peers. Instructi<strong>on</strong>al materials were available<br />

<strong>and</strong> all students were instructed to interact<br />

with the materials. The first author sat with<br />

the participant <strong>on</strong> the floor or at the table.<br />

Classroom sessi<strong>on</strong>s were characterized by low<br />

levels of dem<strong>and</strong>s <strong>and</strong> high levels of attenti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

which approximated typical classroom c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<strong>and</strong> served to c<strong>on</strong>trol escape-maintained<br />

<strong>and</strong> attenti<strong>on</strong>-maintained challenging behaviors.<br />

The first author modeled appropriate<br />

use of the materials but never prompted the<br />

participant to engage with the instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

materials. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally, the first author provided<br />

attenti<strong>on</strong> (praise) to the participant every<br />

30 sec<strong>on</strong>ds. The first author did not resp<strong>on</strong>d<br />

to instances of challenging behavior<br />

except to physically guide the individual back<br />

to the instructi<strong>on</strong>al area or to block access to<br />

the preferred tangible. The discriminative<br />

stimulus (S D ) for challenging behavior (each<br />

child’s preferred tangible item) was present<br />

but unavailable throughout the classroom sessi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Results<br />

Figure 1 shows the percentages of 10-s intervals<br />

with challenging behavior across classroom sessi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<strong>and</strong> under the two c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s for each<br />

participant. All three children had higher levels<br />

of challenging behavior under the no presessi<strong>on</strong><br />

access c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> lower levels of challenging<br />

behavior following presessi<strong>on</strong> satiati<strong>on</strong>. Jacob<br />

engaged in higher levels of challenging behavior<br />

in the classroom following no presessi<strong>on</strong><br />

access (M 64%; range <str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>% to 95%) than<br />

following the presessi<strong>on</strong> satiati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong><br />

(M 0.2%; range 0% to 1%). Similarly, Geoffrey<br />

engaged in higher levels of challenging behavior<br />

following the no presessi<strong>on</strong> access c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong><br />

(M 16%, range 10% to 23%) compared<br />

to the presessi<strong>on</strong> satiati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> (M 3%;<br />

range 1% to 4%). D<strong>on</strong>ovan engaged in comparatively<br />

higher levels of challenging behavior following<br />

the no presessi<strong>on</strong> access c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> (M <br />

28%, range 20% to 35%) <strong>and</strong> lower levels of<br />

612 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


Figure 1. Percentage of intervals with challenging behavior.<br />

Effects of Presessi<strong>on</strong> Satiati<strong>on</strong> / 613


challenging behavior in the presessi<strong>on</strong> satiati<strong>on</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> (M 4%, range 3% to 6%).<br />

Figure 2 presents the effect of the MO manipulati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>on</strong> academic engagement. Higher<br />

levels of academic engagement following presessi<strong>on</strong><br />

satiati<strong>on</strong> to the preferred tangible<br />

were observed for all three participants. Following<br />

presessi<strong>on</strong> satiati<strong>on</strong> to the microph<strong>on</strong>e<br />

Jacob engaged in very high levels of academic<br />

engagement in the classroom (M 91%;<br />

range 85% to 100%). In the no presessi<strong>on</strong><br />

access c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> Jacob’s academic engagement<br />

in the classroom was c<strong>on</strong>siderably lower<br />

(M 26%; range 5% to <str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>%). In the presessi<strong>on</strong><br />

satiati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> Geoffrey dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

high levels of academic engagement<br />

(M 91%; range 88% to 96%). During the no<br />

presessi<strong>on</strong> access c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, Geoffrey had<br />

lower levels of academic engagement (M <br />

43%; range 26% to 58%). In the presessi<strong>on</strong><br />

satiati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> D<strong>on</strong>ovan dem<strong>on</strong>strated<br />

higher levels of academic engagement (M <br />

79%, range 57% to 91%). During the no presessi<strong>on</strong><br />

access c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, D<strong>on</strong>ovan had lower<br />

levels of academic engagement (M 29%,<br />

range 17% to <str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>%).<br />

A NAP (N<strong>on</strong>-overlap of All Pairs) analysis<br />

(Parker & Vannest, 2009) was c<strong>on</strong>ducted for<br />

each participant’s data across the two c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

of presessi<strong>on</strong> satiati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> no presessi<strong>on</strong><br />

access using the dominance statistic, Mann-<br />

Whitney U, (MW-U) within the NCSS statistical<br />

package (Hintze, 2004). MW-U outputs<br />

smaller <strong>and</strong> larger U values (US <strong>and</strong> UL), <strong>and</strong><br />

NAP equals their difference divided by their<br />

sum: (UL - US)/(UL US). The resulting<br />

NAP value was 1, indicating that 100% percent<br />

of the data showed no overlap between the<br />

two c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s for each participant <strong>on</strong> both<br />

challenging behavior <strong>and</strong> academic engagement.<br />

Therefore the two data clusters were<br />

highly separated.<br />

The n<strong>on</strong>-overlap NAP indices calculated for<br />

the three separate series (<strong>on</strong>e for each participant)<br />

were then combined, using Meta-analysis<br />

methods in the free software package,<br />

WinPEPI (Abrams<strong>on</strong> & Gahlinger, 2001).<br />

NAP values <strong>and</strong> their st<strong>and</strong>ard errors were<br />

entered into WinPEPI, <strong>and</strong> were combined<br />

using a fixed effects model, which assumes<br />

that each individual series is an estimati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

the same true effect. The combining method<br />

was based <strong>on</strong> weighted averages, with the<br />

weights being reciprocals of the variances.<br />

This more heavily weights l<strong>on</strong>ger data series<br />

(Abrams<strong>on</strong>, 2004). The omnibus NAP n<strong>on</strong>overlap<br />

for the three individual series for challenging<br />

behavior <strong>and</strong> all three individual series<br />

for academic engagement was 100%, with<br />

95% c<strong>on</strong>fidence limits 0.57 to 1.43. Therefore,<br />

for our obtained overall NAP of 1 for challenging<br />

behavior <strong>and</strong> NAP of 1 for academic<br />

engagement, we can be 95% sure that the true<br />

NAP is somewhere between 0.57 <strong>and</strong> 1.43.<br />

Discussi<strong>on</strong><br />

The purpose of this study was to evaluate the<br />

effect of presessi<strong>on</strong> satiati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> challenging<br />

behavior <strong>and</strong> academic engagement for three<br />

boys with autism who engaged in tangibly<br />

maintained challenging behavior. Participants<br />

were exposed to <strong>on</strong>e of two c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s prior<br />

to group instructi<strong>on</strong> sessi<strong>on</strong>s in their classroom:<br />

presessi<strong>on</strong> satiati<strong>on</strong> or no presessi<strong>on</strong><br />

access to their preferred tangible item. It was<br />

hypothesized that participants would engage<br />

in lower levels of challenging behavior following<br />

presessi<strong>on</strong> access to the preferred tangible.<br />

This access decreases the reinforcement<br />

value of the tangible item <strong>and</strong> corresp<strong>on</strong>dingly<br />

decreases the frequency of challenging<br />

behavior previously correlated with accessing<br />

the tangible (i.e., toys). It was also hypothesized<br />

that following presessi<strong>on</strong> satiati<strong>on</strong>, participant<br />

academic engagement would increase<br />

as a by-product of the reducti<strong>on</strong> in challenging<br />

behavior based <strong>on</strong> previous study outcomes<br />

(Rispoli et al., in press). Results of the<br />

MO manipulati<strong>on</strong> support these hypotheses.<br />

All participants engaged in lower levels of<br />

challenging behavior <strong>and</strong> higher levels of academic<br />

engagement following presessi<strong>on</strong> satiati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

These results also support previous research<br />

dem<strong>on</strong>strating reducti<strong>on</strong>s in<br />

challenging behavior as a result of presessi<strong>on</strong><br />

manipulati<strong>on</strong>s (e.g. Lang et al., 2009; McComas,<br />

Thomps<strong>on</strong>, & Johns<strong>on</strong>, 2003; O’Reilly,<br />

2008; Rispoli et al., in press).<br />

One explanati<strong>on</strong> for these results is the manipulati<strong>on</strong><br />

of the MO. In this study, satiati<strong>on</strong><br />

may have been achieved by providing the participants<br />

with presessi<strong>on</strong> access to their highly<br />

preferred tangible. By creating a state of satiati<strong>on</strong><br />

the reinforcing value of the tangible may<br />

have been reduced. The participant’s rejec-<br />

614 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


Figure 2. Percentage of intervals with academic engagement.<br />

Effects of Presessi<strong>on</strong> Satiati<strong>on</strong> / 615


ti<strong>on</strong> of the preferred item signaled a reducti<strong>on</strong><br />

in that item’s reinforcing value <strong>and</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>ed<br />

as a behavioral indicator of satiati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

This presessi<strong>on</strong> access may have also c<strong>on</strong>tributed<br />

to the increase in academic engagement<br />

in the subsequent classroom sessi<strong>on</strong>. By<br />

decreasing each child’s motivati<strong>on</strong> to engage<br />

in challenging behavior, the child may have<br />

allocated his behavior towards accessing reinforcement<br />

derived from academic engagement.<br />

With the decrease in the reinforcing<br />

value of the tangible item, the participants<br />

may have had more opportunity to seek out<br />

<strong>and</strong> access novel communities of reinforcement.<br />

Interacti<strong>on</strong>s with the classmates, the<br />

teacher, or instructi<strong>on</strong>al materials may have<br />

become more reinforcing relative to the tangible<br />

item following presessi<strong>on</strong> satiati<strong>on</strong>. Additi<strong>on</strong>ally,<br />

most of the participants’ topographies<br />

of challenging behavior c<strong>on</strong>flicted with<br />

academic engagement. For instance elopement<br />

<strong>and</strong> aggressi<strong>on</strong> are incompatible with<br />

academic engagement. Thus, by reducing the<br />

frequency of challenging behavior there may<br />

have been an increase in opportunities for<br />

academic engagement.<br />

The reducti<strong>on</strong>s in challenging behavior in<br />

the classroom following the presessi<strong>on</strong> satiati<strong>on</strong><br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> suggest that the MO was successfully<br />

isolated <strong>and</strong> manipulated. In all classroom<br />

c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s tangibly maintained<br />

challenging behavior was placed <strong>on</strong> extincti<strong>on</strong><br />

such that the participants were never given<br />

access to the tangible items. By holding c<strong>on</strong>sequences<br />

c<strong>on</strong>stant across the two c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s,<br />

the <strong>on</strong>ly manipulati<strong>on</strong> was presessi<strong>on</strong> access to<br />

the tangible item. Furthermore, classroom activities<br />

were held c<strong>on</strong>stant for each participant<br />

thereby reducing the influence of possible extraneous<br />

variables such as task preference, <strong>on</strong><br />

levels of challenging behavior.<br />

While presessi<strong>on</strong> satiati<strong>on</strong> may explain the<br />

reducti<strong>on</strong> in challenging behavior, other variables<br />

may have also c<strong>on</strong>tributed to the effectiveness<br />

of this interventi<strong>on</strong>. For example, the<br />

effectiveness of the presessi<strong>on</strong> satiati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>on</strong> reducing challenging behavior may<br />

have been enhanced by the c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>al analysis results. Previous research<br />

has shown that MO interventi<strong>on</strong>s may not be<br />

effective when they are not matched to the<br />

functi<strong>on</strong> of challenging behavior (McComas<br />

et al., 2003). In light of this, each participant’s<br />

challenging behavior was first assessed using<br />

analogue functi<strong>on</strong>al analyses. The presessi<strong>on</strong><br />

satiati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> was then carefully designed<br />

to reflect the tangible functi<strong>on</strong> of the<br />

participants’ challenging behavior. Furthermore,<br />

the tangible item used was c<strong>on</strong>firmed<br />

by the results of a paired choice preference<br />

assessment <strong>and</strong> presessi<strong>on</strong> access was determined<br />

by behavioral indicators of satiati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Thus, the MO manipulati<strong>on</strong> was directly<br />

linked to assessment results for each participant.<br />

The decrease in challenging behavior <strong>and</strong><br />

increase in academic engagement reported<br />

here may also depend, in part, <strong>on</strong> the characteristics<br />

of the participants. Each participant<br />

had a diagnosis of an autism spectrum disorder.<br />

It is possible that the MO manipulati<strong>on</strong><br />

addressed a core behavioral characteristic of<br />

autism. The Diagnostic <strong>and</strong> Statistical Manual<br />

(DSM-IV) describes individuals with autism as<br />

often engaging in compulsive or repetitive behaviors.<br />

All participants in this study engaged<br />

with their preferred tangible in a restrictive<br />

<strong>and</strong> repetitive manner. Jacob sang “Twinkle,<br />

twinkle litter star” into his microph<strong>on</strong>e repeatedly<br />

<strong>and</strong> would verbally protest when asked to<br />

sing a different s<strong>on</strong>g. Geoffrey counted each<br />

ball as it exited the plastic ball drop. Finally,<br />

D<strong>on</strong>ovan drew the uppercase <strong>and</strong> lowercase<br />

letters F <strong>and</strong> R <strong>on</strong> the Magna Doodle®. The<br />

presessi<strong>on</strong> access to the toys may have reduced<br />

the value of the participants’ compulsive engagement<br />

with these items thereby allowing<br />

them to engage in other behaviors, including<br />

academic engagement.<br />

Several limitati<strong>on</strong>s of this study must be c<strong>on</strong>sidered<br />

when interpreting these results. First,<br />

the influence of MOs is <strong>on</strong>ly <strong>on</strong>e explanati<strong>on</strong><br />

for the changes in performance. Other behavioral<br />

mechanisms (e.g., schedules of reinforcement)<br />

may have influenced the results<br />

even though the manipulati<strong>on</strong> of the MO fits<br />

c<strong>on</strong>ceptually with the outcomes of this study<br />

<strong>and</strong> supports previous research in this area.<br />

Sec<strong>on</strong>d, the experimental design did not include<br />

a baseline phase. Thus, while the effects<br />

of the presessi<strong>on</strong> satiati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong> appeared<br />

to improve both dependent variables as compared<br />

to the no presessi<strong>on</strong> access c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>, a<br />

comparis<strong>on</strong> against baseline levels of these<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses cannot be made. Third, the durati<strong>on</strong><br />

of presessi<strong>on</strong> satiati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s varied<br />

616 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


across <strong>and</strong> within participants. The mean<br />

lengths of presessi<strong>on</strong> satiati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>diti<strong>on</strong>s were<br />

11, 21, <strong>and</strong> 23 minutes for Jacob, Geoffrey,<br />

<strong>and</strong> D<strong>on</strong>ovan, respectively. Such variability<br />

may present an additi<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong> when<br />

scheduling <strong>and</strong> implementing this interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

in regular public school settings.<br />

Results from this study have several implicati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for practiti<strong>on</strong>ers. First, this interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

may be especially useful when the discriminative<br />

stimulus for challenging behavior cannot<br />

be removed from the envir<strong>on</strong>ment. In such<br />

instances, addressing the motivati<strong>on</strong> to engage<br />

in challenging behavior may provide an<br />

effective means of reducing challenging behavior<br />

(O’Reilly et al., 2008). Classroom<br />

teachers may incorporate periods of free access<br />

to the maintaining c<strong>on</strong>sequence for challenging<br />

behavior in their daily classroom<br />

schedule. If a student is known to engage in<br />

tangibly-maintained challenging behavior<br />

during a particular activity, the teacher may<br />

provide the student with access to the tangible<br />

item for periods of time immediately prior to<br />

the difficult activity.<br />

Sec<strong>on</strong>d, presessi<strong>on</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong>s may reduce<br />

the need for <strong>on</strong>e-<strong>on</strong>-<strong>on</strong>e support in inclusive<br />

classrooms. By abolishing the value of<br />

reinforcement for challenging behavior prior<br />

to a classroom situati<strong>on</strong>, the individual may be<br />

less likely to engage in such behavior <strong>and</strong> thus<br />

require less behavioral support from teachers<br />

<strong>and</strong> staff. Another benefit that is particularly<br />

noteworthy is the effect of this MO manipulati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>on</strong> multiple outcomes. Students with autism<br />

often require multiple interventi<strong>on</strong>s for<br />

decreasing challenging behaviors (Reese et<br />

al., 2003) <strong>and</strong> additi<strong>on</strong>al interventi<strong>on</strong>s for increasing<br />

appropriate skills. Implementing numerous<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>s for multiple students in<br />

a classroom may impede treatment fidelity<br />

<strong>and</strong> overwhelm service providers. Therefore,<br />

the efficiency of this interventi<strong>on</strong> may be appealing<br />

to practiti<strong>on</strong>ers. More research is<br />

needed to examine the use of MOs to affect<br />

multiple resp<strong>on</strong>se classes of behavior simultaneously.<br />

This study highlights several areas that warrant<br />

further empirical attenti<strong>on</strong>. First, researchers<br />

should explore the applicati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

MO interventi<strong>on</strong>s in other applied settings<br />

including the community <strong>and</strong> home envir<strong>on</strong>ments.<br />

Sec<strong>on</strong>d, previous research has shown<br />

that preferences for specific tangible items<br />

may vary across time (Zhou, Iwata, Goff, &<br />

Shore, 2001). As a result, a tangible item that<br />

does not evoke challenging behavior <strong>on</strong>e day<br />

may evoke such behavior <strong>on</strong> another day. For<br />

individuals whose challenging behavior is<br />

maintained by access to multiple tangible<br />

items, it may be helpful to assess preferences<br />

prior to MO manipulati<strong>on</strong>s regularly in order<br />

to enhance interventi<strong>on</strong> effectiveness.<br />

References<br />

Abrams<strong>on</strong>, J. H., & Gahlinger, P. M. (2001). Computer<br />

Programs for Epidemiologists: PEPI v.4.0.<br />

Salt Lake City, UT: Sagebrush Press.<br />

Abrams<strong>on</strong>, J. H. (2004). WINPEPI (PEPI-for-Windows):<br />

computer programs for epidemiologists.<br />

Epidemiologic Perspectives & Innovati<strong>on</strong>s, 1, 1-6. doi:<br />

10.1186/1742-5573-1-6<br />

American Psychiatric Associati<strong>on</strong>. (1994). Diagnostic<br />

<strong>and</strong> statistical manual of mental disorders (4 th ed.).<br />

Washingt<strong>on</strong>, DC: Author.<br />

Barlow, D. H., Nock, M. K., & Hersen, M. (2009).<br />

Single case experimental designs: Strategies for studying<br />

behavior change (3rd ed.). New York: Allyn & Bac<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Carr, E. G., & Smith, C. E. (1995). Biological setting<br />

events for self-injury. Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 1, 94–98.<br />

Fisher, W., Piazza, C., Bowman, L G., Hagopian,<br />

L. P., Owens, J. C., & Slevin, I. (1992). A comparis<strong>on</strong><br />

of two approaches for identifying reinforcers<br />

for pers<strong>on</strong>s with severe <strong>and</strong> profound disabilities.<br />

Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 491-498.<br />

Hintze, J. (2006). NCSS <strong>and</strong> PASS: Number Cruncher<br />

Statistical Systems [Computer software]. Kaysville,<br />

UT: NCSS.<br />

Horner, R. H., Albin, R. W., Sprague, J. R., & Todd,<br />

A. W. (2000). Positive behavior support. In M.<br />

Snell & F. Brown (Eds.), Instructi<strong>on</strong> of students with<br />

severe disabilities (5 th<br />

ed.) (pp.207-243). Upper Saddle<br />

River, NJ: Merrill.<br />

Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman,<br />

K. E., & Richman, G. S. (1994). Toward a functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

analysis of self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior<br />

Analysis, 27, 197–209. (Reprinted from<br />

Analysis <strong>and</strong> Interventi<strong>on</strong> in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>,<br />

2, 1982, 3–20).<br />

Kennedy, C. H., & Meyer, K. A. (1996). Sleep deprivati<strong>on</strong>,<br />

allergy symptoms, <strong>and</strong> negatively reinforced<br />

problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior<br />

Analysis, 29, 133–135.<br />

Kern, L., Choutka, C. M., & Sokol, N. G. (2002).<br />

Assessment-based antecedent interventi<strong>on</strong>s used<br />

in natural settings to reduce challenging behav-<br />

Effects of Presessi<strong>on</strong> Satiati<strong>on</strong> / 617


ior: An analysis of the literature. Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Treatment of Children, 25, 113–130.<br />

Lang, R., O’Reilly, M., Sigafoos, J., Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, G. E.,<br />

Machalicek, W., Rispoli, M., & White, P. (2009).<br />

Enhancing the effectiveness of a play interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

by abolishing the reinforcing value of stereotypy<br />

for a child with autism: A pilot study. Journal of<br />

Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, 889–894.<br />

Lang, R., O’Reilly, M., Sigafoos, J., Machalicek, W.,<br />

Rispoli, M., Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, G., Aguilar, J., & Fragale, C.<br />

(in press). The effects of an abolishing operati<strong>on</strong><br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> comp<strong>on</strong>ent <strong>on</strong> play skills, challenging<br />

behavior <strong>and</strong> stereotypy. Behavior Modificati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Laraway, S., Snycerski, S., Michael, J., & Poling, A.<br />

(2003). Motivating operati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> terms to describe<br />

them: Some further refinements. Journal of<br />

Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 407–414.<br />

Luiselli, J. (2006). Summary <strong>and</strong> future decisi<strong>on</strong>s. In<br />

J. K. Luiselli (Ed.), Antecedent assessment <strong>and</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong>:<br />

Supporting children <strong>and</strong> adults with developmental<br />

disabilities in community settings (pp. 293–<br />

302). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing<br />

Co.<br />

McComas, J. J., Thomps<strong>on</strong>, A., & Johns<strong>on</strong>, L.<br />

(2003). The effects of presessi<strong>on</strong> attenti<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong><br />

problem behavior maintained by different reinforcers.<br />

Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36,<br />

297–307.<br />

O’Reilly, M. F. (1995). Functi<strong>on</strong>al analysis <strong>and</strong> treatment<br />

of escape-maintained aggressi<strong>on</strong> correlated<br />

with sleep deprivati<strong>on</strong>. Journal of Applied Behavior<br />

Analysis, 28, 225–226.<br />

O’Reilly, M. F. (1997). Functi<strong>on</strong>al analysis of episodic<br />

self-injury correlated with recurrent otitis<br />

media. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30,<br />

165–167.<br />

O’Reilly, M. F., Lang, R., Davis, T., Rispoli, M.,<br />

Machalicek, W., Sigafoos, J., et al. (2009). A systematic<br />

examinati<strong>on</strong> of different parameters of<br />

presessi<strong>on</strong> exposure to tangible stimuli that maintain<br />

challenging behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior<br />

Analysis, 42, 773-783.<br />

O’Reilly, M., Sigafoos, J., Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, G., Edrisinha,<br />

C., & Andrews, A. (2005). An examinati<strong>on</strong> of the<br />

effects of a classroom activity schedule <strong>on</strong> levels of<br />

self-injury <strong>and</strong> engagement for a child with severe<br />

autism. Journal of <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>,<br />

35, 305–311.<br />

O’Reilly, M. F., Sigafoos, J., Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, G., Rispoli,<br />

M., Lang, R., Chan, J., et al. (2008). Manipulating<br />

the behavior-altering effect of the motivating operati<strong>on</strong>:<br />

Examinati<strong>on</strong> of the influence <strong>on</strong> challenging<br />

behavior during leisure activities. Research<br />

in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 29, 333–340.<br />

Parker, R., & Vannest, K. (2009). An improved effect<br />

size for single-case research: N<strong>on</strong>overlap of<br />

all pairs. Behavior Therapy 40, 357–367.<br />

Reese, R. M., Richman, D. M., Zarc<strong>on</strong>e, J., & Zarc<strong>on</strong>e,<br />

T. (2003). Individualizing functi<strong>on</strong>al assessments<br />

for children with autism: The c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong><br />

of perseverative behavior <strong>and</strong> sensory disturbances<br />

to disruptive behavior. Focus <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Other <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 18, 87-92.<br />

Rispoli, M., O’Reilly, M., Lang, R., Machalicek, W.,<br />

Davis, T., Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, G., et al. (in press). Effects of<br />

motivating operati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> aberrant behavior <strong>and</strong><br />

academic engagement during classroom instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

for two students with autism. Journal of Applied<br />

Behavior Analysis.<br />

Schopler, E., Reichler, R. J., & Renner, B. R. (1988).<br />

Childhood autism rating scale. Los Angeles, CA:<br />

Western Psychological Services.<br />

Sparrow, S. S., Balla, D. A., & Cicchetti, D. V. (1984).<br />

Vinel<strong>and</strong> adaptive behavior scales. Circle Pines, MN:<br />

American Guidance Service.<br />

Zhou, L., Iwata, B. A., Goff, G. A., & Shore, B. A.<br />

(2001). L<strong>on</strong>gitudinal analysis of leisure-item preferences.<br />

Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34,<br />

179–184.<br />

Received: 20 July 2010<br />

Initial Acceptance: 16 September 2010<br />

Final Acceptance: 21 October 2010<br />

618 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, 2011, <str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>(4), 619-641<br />

© <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Ten-year Cumulative Author Index of the Journals Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Training in Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, <strong>and</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong> 2001, 36(1) through 2010, 45(4)<br />

Stanley H. Zucker <strong>and</strong> Silva Hassert<br />

Ariz<strong>on</strong>a State University<br />

Abstract: This cumulative author index was developed as a service for the readership of Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>. It was prepared as a resource for scholars wishing to access<br />

the 391 articles published in volumes 36–45 of this journal. It also serves as a timely supplement to the 25-year<br />

(1966-1990) cumulative author index published in 1991 <strong>and</strong> the 10-year (1991-2000) cumulative author<br />

index published in 2001.<br />

Since many literature reviews may involve tracking authors <strong>and</strong> their research programs across a variety of<br />

studies, <strong>and</strong> individual authors may not always be “first” authors, this index was prepared with a crossreference<br />

of all authors to each citati<strong>on</strong>. In total, 1163 author citati<strong>on</strong>s are included. It is our hope that this<br />

bibliographic index will help facilitate access to available informati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> research, practice, <strong>and</strong> issues related<br />

to autism, intellectual disabilities, <strong>and</strong> other developmental disabilities.<br />

Aborn, L. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #279.<br />

Acar, C. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #120.<br />

Agran, M. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #369.<br />

Agran, M. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #170.<br />

1. Agran, M., & Krupp, M. (2010). A preliminary<br />

investigati<strong>on</strong> of parents’ opini<strong>on</strong>s about<br />

safety skills instructi<strong>on</strong>: An apparent discrepancy<br />

between importance <strong>and</strong> expectati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

45(2), 303–311.<br />

2. Agran, M., Alper, S., & Wehmeyer, M. (2002).<br />

Access to the general curriculum for students<br />

with significant disabilities: What it means to<br />

teachers. 37(2), 123–133.<br />

3. Agran, M., Sinclair, T., Alper, S., Cavin, M.,<br />

Wehmeyer, M., & Hughes, C. (2005). Using<br />

self-m<strong>on</strong>itoring to increase following-directi<strong>on</strong><br />

skills of students with moderate to severe<br />

disabilities in general educati<strong>on</strong>. 40(1), 3–13.<br />

4. Agran, M., Wehmeyer, M., Cavin, M., & Palmer,<br />

S. (2010). Promoting active engagement in<br />

Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence c<strong>on</strong>cerning this article should<br />

be addressed to Stanley H. Zucker, Mary Lou Fult<strong>on</strong><br />

Teachers College, Ariz<strong>on</strong>a State University, 1050 S.<br />

Forest Ave., PO Box 871811, Tempe, AZ 85287-<br />

1811. E-mail: <str<strong>on</strong>g>etadd</str<strong>on</strong>g>@asu.edu.<br />

the general educati<strong>on</strong> classroom <strong>and</strong> access<br />

to the general educati<strong>on</strong> curriculum for students<br />

with cognitive disabilities. 45(2), 163–<br />

174.<br />

Aguilera, A. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #249.<br />

5. Akmanoglu, N., & Batu, S. (2004). Teaching<br />

pointing to numerals to individuals with autism<br />

using simultaneous prompting. 39(4),<br />

326–336.<br />

6. Akmanoglu-Uludag, N., & Batu, S. (2005).<br />

Teaching naming relatives to individuals with<br />

autism using simultaneous prompting. 40(4),<br />

401–410.<br />

Alant, E. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #35.<br />

Alazetta, L. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #352.<br />

Alberto, P. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #49.<br />

Alberto, P. A. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #329.<br />

Alberto, P. A. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #80.<br />

Algozzine, B. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #178.<br />

Allinder, R. M. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #316.<br />

7. Allor, J. H., Champlin, T. M., Gifford, D. B., &<br />

Mathes, P. (2010). Methods for increasing<br />

the intensity of reading instructi<strong>on</strong> for students<br />

with intellectual disabilities. 45(4),<br />

500–511.<br />

8. Allor, J. H., Mathes, P., Roberts, K., J<strong>on</strong>es F., &<br />

Champlin, T. (2010). Teaching students with<br />

Cumulative Author Index, Volumes 36–45 / 619


moderate intellectual disabilities to read: An<br />

experimental examinati<strong>on</strong> of a comprehensive<br />

reading interventi<strong>on</strong>. 45(1), 3–22.<br />

9. Allor, J., Mathes, P., Champlin, T., & Cheatham,<br />

J. P. (2009). Research-based techniques for<br />

teaching early reading skills to students with<br />

intellectual disabilities. 44(3), 356–366.<br />

Alper, S. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #2.<br />

Alper, S. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #3.<br />

Alper, S. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #301.<br />

Alper, S. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #302.<br />

10. Alpern, C. S., & Zager, D. (2007). Addressing<br />

communicati<strong>on</strong> needs of young adults with<br />

autism in a college-based inclusi<strong>on</strong> program.<br />

42(4), 428–436.<br />

Amos, B. A. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #202.<br />

Anders<strong>on</strong>, H. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #167.<br />

Andrews, A. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #217.<br />

Angell, M. E. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #18.<br />

Angell, M. E. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #19.<br />

11. Angell, M. E., Bailey, R. L., & Lars<strong>on</strong>, L. (2008).<br />

Systematic instructi<strong>on</strong> for social-pragmatic<br />

language skills in lunchroom settings. 43(3)<br />

342–359.<br />

Anth<strong>on</strong>y, A. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #265.<br />

Antoine, K. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #288.<br />

Ant<strong>on</strong>ucci, M. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #217.<br />

Archwamety, T. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #180.<br />

Arens, K. B. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #89.<br />

12. Arens, K., Cress, C. J., & Marvin, C. A. (2005).<br />

Gaze-shift patterns of young children with<br />

developmental disabilities who are at risk for<br />

being n<strong>on</strong>speaking. 40(2), 158–170.<br />

13. Arthur, M. (2003). Socio-communicative variables<br />

<strong>and</strong> behavior states in students with<br />

profound <strong>and</strong> multiple disabilities: Descriptive<br />

data from school settings. 38(2), 200–<br />

219.<br />

Asmus, J. M. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #<str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>.<br />

Aspy, R. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #254.<br />

Aucoin, M. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #330.<br />

Ayres, K. M. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #317.<br />

Ayres, K. M. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #328.<br />

Ayres, K. M. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #156.<br />

Ayres, K. M. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #79.<br />

14. Ayres, K. M., & Lang<strong>on</strong>e, J. (2005). Interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong> with video for students<br />

with autism: A review of literature. 40(2), 183–<br />

196.<br />

15. Ayres, K. M., Lang<strong>on</strong>e, J., Bo<strong>on</strong>, R. T., & Norman,<br />

A. (2006). Computer-based instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

for purchasing skills. 41(3), 253–263.<br />

16. Ayres, K., Maguire, A., & McClim<strong>on</strong>, D. (2009).<br />

Acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong> of chained<br />

tasks taught with computer based video instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

to children with autism. 44(4), 493–<br />

508.<br />

17. Baer, R. M., Flexer, R. W., & Dennis, L. (2007).<br />

Examining the career paths <strong>and</strong> transiti<strong>on</strong><br />

services of students with disabilities exiting<br />

high school. 42(3), 317–329.<br />

Bailey, R. L. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #11.<br />

18. Bailey, R. L., & Angell, M. E. (2005). Improving<br />

feeding skills <strong>and</strong> mealtime behaviors in children<br />

<strong>and</strong> youth with disabilities. 40(1), 80–<br />

98.<br />

19. Bailey, R. L., St<strong>on</strong>er, J. B., Parette, H. P. Jr., &<br />

Angell, M. E. (2006). AAC team percepti<strong>on</strong>s:<br />

Augmentative <strong>and</strong> alternative communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

device use. 41(2), 139–154.<br />

Bak, S. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #204.<br />

Baker, J. N. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #245.<br />

Baker, S. R. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #349.<br />

Balb<strong>on</strong>i, G. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #326.<br />

Bambara, L. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #209.<br />

20. Bambara, L. M., & Gomez, O. N. (2001). Using<br />

a self-instructi<strong>on</strong>al training package to teach<br />

complex problem-solving skills to adults with<br />

moderate <strong>and</strong> severe disabilities. 36(4), 386–<br />

400.<br />

B<strong>and</strong>a, D. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #101.<br />

B<strong>and</strong>a, D. R. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #102.<br />

21. B<strong>and</strong>a, D. R., & Grimmett, E. (2008). Enhancing<br />

social <strong>and</strong> transiti<strong>on</strong> behaviors of pers<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with autism through activity schedules: A review.<br />

43(3), 324–333.<br />

Barnhill, G. P. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #257.<br />

Barnhill, G. P. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #255.<br />

Bart<strong>on</strong>-Arwood, S. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #175.<br />

Bashinski, S. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #368.<br />

Bassette, L. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #43.<br />

Batterman, N. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #73.<br />

Batu, S. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #5.<br />

Batu, S. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #6.<br />

22. Batu, S., Cihak, D., & Gama, R. I. (2008). Caregiver-delivered<br />

home-based instructi<strong>on</strong> using<br />

simultaneous prompting for teaching<br />

home skills to individuals with developmental<br />

disabilities. 43(4), 541–555.<br />

Bayliss, P. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #51.<br />

Beare, P. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #129.<br />

Beck, A. R. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #33.<br />

23. Beck, A. R., St<strong>on</strong>er, J. B., & Bock, S. J. (2008).<br />

Comparis<strong>on</strong> of PECS <strong>and</strong> the use of a VOCA:<br />

A replicati<strong>on</strong>. 43(2), 198–216.<br />

24. Beck, A. R., Thomps<strong>on</strong>, J. R., Clay, S. L.,<br />

Hutchins, M., Vogt, W. P., Romaniak, B., &<br />

Sokolowski, B. (2001). Preservice professi<strong>on</strong>als’<br />

attitudes toward children who use aug-<br />

620 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


mentative/alternative communicati<strong>on</strong>. 36(3),<br />

255–271.<br />

Becker, S. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #256.<br />

Bedesem, P. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #221.<br />

Belfiore, P. J. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #36.<br />

25. Bennett, K., Frain, M. Brady, M. P., Rosenberg,<br />

H., & Surinak, T. (2009). It all depends <strong>on</strong><br />

where you st<strong>and</strong>: Differences between employee’s<br />

<strong>and</strong> supervisor’s evaluati<strong>on</strong>s of work<br />

performance <strong>and</strong> support needs. 44(4), 471–<br />

480.<br />

Berger, S. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #141.<br />

26. Berger<strong>on</strong>, R., Floyd, R. G., & Sh<strong>and</strong>s, E. I.<br />

(2008). States’ eligibility guidelines for mental<br />

retardati<strong>on</strong>: An update <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

part scores <strong>and</strong> unreliability of IQs. 43(1),<br />

123–143.<br />

27. Beyer, J. (2009). <strong>Autism</strong> spectrum disorders<br />

<strong>and</strong> sibling relati<strong>on</strong>ships: Research <strong>and</strong> strategies<br />

44(4), 444–452.<br />

Beyer, J. F. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #325.<br />

28. Bezdek, J., Summers, J., & Turnbull, A. (2010).<br />

Professi<strong>on</strong>als’ attitude <strong>on</strong> partnering with<br />

families of children <strong>and</strong> youth with disabilities.<br />

45(3), 356–365.<br />

Billingsley, F. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #373.<br />

29. Bingham, M. A. Spo<strong>on</strong>er, F., & Browder, D.<br />

(2007). Training paraeducators to promote<br />

the use of augmentative <strong>and</strong> alternative communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

by students with significant disabilities.<br />

42(3), 339–352.<br />

30. Birkan, B. (2005). Using simultaneous prompting<br />

for teaching various discrete tasks to students<br />

with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. 40(1), 68–79.<br />

Birkan, B. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #378.<br />

Birkan, B. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #379.<br />

Birkan, B. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #380.<br />

Blackmountain, L. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #136.<br />

Blair, C. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #132.<br />

31. Blair, K. C., Liaupsin, C. J., Umbreit, J., &<br />

Kwe<strong>on</strong>, G. (2006). Functi<strong>on</strong>-based interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

to support the inclusive placements of<br />

young children in Korea. 41(1), 48–57.<br />

Blais, M. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #214.<br />

32. Blanks, A. B., & Smith, J. D. (2009). Multiculturalism,<br />

religi<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> disability: Implicati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for special educati<strong>on</strong> practiti<strong>on</strong>ers. 44(3),<br />

295–303.<br />

Bock, S. J. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #23.<br />

33. Bock, S. J., St<strong>on</strong>er, J. B., Beck, A. R., Hanley,<br />

L., & Prochnow, J. (2005). Increasing functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

communicati<strong>on</strong> in n<strong>on</strong>-speaking preschool<br />

children: Comparis<strong>on</strong> of PECS <strong>and</strong><br />

VOCA. 40(3), 264–278.<br />

Bodkin, A. E. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #195.<br />

Boisvert, J. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #214.<br />

Bolger, K. E. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #132.<br />

34. B<strong>on</strong>ggat, P. W., & Hall, L. J. (2010). Evaluati<strong>on</strong><br />

of the effects of sensory integrati<strong>on</strong>-based<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> by a preschool special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

teacher. 45(2), 294–302.<br />

Bo<strong>on</strong>, R. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #131.<br />

Bo<strong>on</strong>, R. T. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #15.<br />

35. Bornman, J., Alant, E., & Lloyd, L. L. (2007). A<br />

beginning communicati<strong>on</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong> protocol:<br />

In-service training of health workers.<br />

42(2), 190–208.<br />

36. Bosner, S. M., & Belfiore, P. J. (2001). Strategies<br />

<strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong>s for teaching an adolescent<br />

with Down Syndrome. 36(1), 94–<br />

102.<br />

37. Bouck, E. C. (2009). Functi<strong>on</strong>al curriculum<br />

models for sec<strong>on</strong>dary students with mild<br />

mental impairment. 44(4), 435–443.<br />

38. Bouck, E. C. (2004). State of curriculum for<br />

sec<strong>on</strong>dary students with mild mental retardati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

39(2), 169–176.<br />

39. Bouck, E. C. (2005). Impact of factors <strong>on</strong> curriculum<br />

<strong>and</strong> instructi<strong>on</strong>al envir<strong>on</strong>ments for<br />

sec<strong>on</strong>dary students with mild mental retardati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

40(3), 309–319.<br />

40. Bouck, E. C. (2008). Factors impacting the<br />

enactment of a functi<strong>on</strong>al curriculum in selfc<strong>on</strong>tained<br />

cross-categorical programs.<br />

43(3), 294–310.<br />

41. Bouck, E. C. (2009). No Child Left Behind, the<br />

Individuals with <strong>Disabilities</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong> Act<br />

<strong>and</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>al curricula: A c<strong>on</strong>flict of interest?<br />

44(1), 3–13.<br />

Bouck, E. C. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #390.<br />

42. Bouck, E. C., & Flanagan, S. M. (2010). Functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

curriculum Evidence-based educati<strong>on</strong>?:<br />

C<strong>on</strong>sidering sec<strong>on</strong>dary students with<br />

mild intellectual disabilities. 45(4), 487–499.<br />

43. Bouck, E. C., Bassette, L., Taber-Doughty, T.,<br />

Flanagan, S. M., & Szwed, K. (2009). Pentop<br />

computers as tools for teaching multiplicati<strong>on</strong><br />

to students with mild intellectual disabilities.<br />

44(3), 367–380.<br />

44. Boutot, E. A., & Bryant, D. P. (2005). Social<br />

integrati<strong>on</strong> of students with autism in inclusive<br />

settings. 40(1), 14–23.<br />

45. Boutot, E. A., Guenther, T., & Crozier, S.<br />

(2005). Let’s play: Teaching play skills to<br />

young children with autism. 40(3), 285–292.<br />

Boyd, B. A. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #221.<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>. Boyd, B. A., C<strong>on</strong>roy, M. A., Asmus, J. M.,<br />

McKenney, E. L. W., & Mancil, G. R. (2008).<br />

Descriptive analysis of classroom setting<br />

events <strong>on</strong> the social behaviors of children<br />

with autism spectrum disorder. 43(2), 186–<br />

197.<br />

47. Boyd, C. M., Fraiman, J. L., Hawkins, K. A.,<br />

Labin, J. M., Sutter, M., & Wahl, M. R. (2008).<br />

Cumulative Author Index, Volumes 36–45 / 621


Effects of the STAR interventi<strong>on</strong> program <strong>on</strong><br />

interacti<strong>on</strong>s between campers with <strong>and</strong> without<br />

disabilities during inclusive summer day<br />

camp activities. 43(1), 92–101.<br />

Bozkurt, F. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #153.<br />

48. Bozkurt, F., & Gursel, O. (2005). Effectiveness<br />

of c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay <strong>on</strong> teaching snack <strong>and</strong><br />

drink preparati<strong>on</strong> skills to children with mental<br />

retardati<strong>on</strong>. 40(4), 390–400.<br />

Bradford, E. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #169.<br />

49. Bradford, S., Shippen, M. E., Alberto, P.,<br />

Houchins, D. E., & Flores, M. (2006). Using<br />

systematic instructi<strong>on</strong> to teach decoding<br />

skills to middle school students with moderate<br />

intellectual disabilities. 41(4), 333–343.<br />

Brady, M. P. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #182.<br />

Brady, M. P. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #55.<br />

Brady, M. P. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #25.<br />

50. Brady, M. P., & Rosenberg, H. (2002). Job<br />

observati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> behavior scale: A supported<br />

employment assessment instrument. 37(4),<br />

427–433.<br />

Bray, N. W. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #168.<br />

51. Broekaert, E., Van Hove, G., Bayliss P., &<br />

D’Oosterlinck, F. (2004). The search for an<br />

integrated paradigm of care models for people<br />

with h<strong>and</strong>icaps, disabilities <strong>and</strong> behavioural<br />

disorders at the Department of Orthopedagogy<br />

of Ghent University. 39(3), 206–<br />

216.<br />

Brothers<strong>on</strong>, M. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #142.<br />

Brothers<strong>on</strong>, M. J. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #269.<br />

Browder, D. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #29.<br />

Browder, D. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #245.<br />

Browder, D. M. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #209.<br />

Browder, D. M. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #210.<br />

Browder, D. M. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #178.<br />

Browder, D. M. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #174.<br />

Browder, D. M. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #88.<br />

52. Browder, D. M., Fallin, K., Davis, S., & Karv<strong>on</strong>en,<br />

M. (2003). C<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong> of what may<br />

influence student outcomes <strong>on</strong> alternate assessment.<br />

38(3), 255–270.<br />

53. Brown, J., & Murray, D. (2001). Strategies for<br />

enhancing play skills for children with autism<br />

spectrum disorder. 36(3), 312–317.<br />

Brunet, J. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #100.<br />

54. Bryan, T. Stiles, N., Burstein, K. Ergul, C., &<br />

Chao, P. (2007). “Am I supposed to underst<strong>and</strong><br />

this stuff?” Youth with special health<br />

care needs readiness for transiti<strong>on</strong>. 42(3),<br />

330–338.<br />

Bryant, D. P. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #44.<br />

55. Bucholz, J., Brady, M. P., Duffy, M., Scott, J., &<br />

K<strong>on</strong>tosh, L. G. (2008). Using literacy-based<br />

behavioral interventi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> social stories to<br />

improve work behavior in employees with developmental<br />

disabilities. 43(4), 486–501.<br />

56. Bui, Y. N., & Turnbull, A. (2003). East meets<br />

west: Analysis of pers<strong>on</strong>-centered planning<br />

in the c<strong>on</strong>text of Asian American values.<br />

38(1), 18–31.<br />

Burley, M. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #266.<br />

Burstein, K. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #54.<br />

57. Burt, T. L., Porretta, D. L., & Klein, R. E. (2007).<br />

Use of adapted bicycles <strong>on</strong> the learning of<br />

c<strong>on</strong>venti<strong>on</strong>al cycling by children with mental<br />

retardati<strong>on</strong>. 42(3), 364–382.<br />

58. Camer<strong>on</strong>, D. L., & Cook, B. G. (2007). Attitudes<br />

of preservice teachers enrolled in an<br />

infusi<strong>on</strong> preparati<strong>on</strong> program regarding planning<br />

<strong>and</strong> making adaptati<strong>on</strong>s for included<br />

students with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. 42(3), 353–<br />

363.<br />

59. Campbell, D. J., Reilly, A., & Henley, J. (2008).<br />

Comparis<strong>on</strong> of assessment results of children<br />

with low incidence disabilities. 43(2),<br />

217–225.<br />

Campbell, J. L. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #277.<br />

Campbell, L. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #307.<br />

Çamursoy, I˙. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #380.<br />

60. Cannella, H., Sigafoos, J., O’Reilly, M., de la<br />

Cruz, B., Edrisinha, C., & Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, G. E.<br />

(2006). Comparing video prompting to video<br />

modeling for teaching daily living skills to six<br />

adults with developmental disabilities. 41(4),<br />

344–356.<br />

61. Cannella-Mal<strong>on</strong>e, H. I., O’Reilly, M. F., Sigafoos,<br />

J., & Chan, J. M. (2008). Combined<br />

curricular interventi<strong>on</strong> with brief h<strong>and</strong>s down<br />

to decrease h<strong>and</strong> mouthing <strong>and</strong> the use of<br />

arm splints for a young boy with profound<br />

disabilities. 43(3), 360–366.<br />

62. Carothers, D. E., & Taylor, R. L. (2004). Social<br />

cognitive processing in elementary school<br />

children with Asperger disorder. 39(2), 177-<br />

190.<br />

63. Carpenter, L. B. (2001). Utilizing travel cards<br />

to increase productive student behavior,<br />

teacher collaborati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> parent-school<br />

communicati<strong>on</strong>. 36(3), 318–322.<br />

Carter, E. W. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #169.<br />

Carter, E. W. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #74.<br />

Carter, M. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #152.<br />

Carter, M. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #184.<br />

Carter, S. L. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #375.<br />

64. Carter, S. L. (2007). Review of recent treatment<br />

acceptability research. 42(3), 301–316.<br />

65. Carter, S. L. (2008). A distributive model of<br />

treatment acceptability. 43(4), 411–420.<br />

66. Carter, S. L. (2008). Further c<strong>on</strong>ceptualizati<strong>on</strong><br />

of treatment acceptability. 43(2), 135–143.<br />

Carter, S. L. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #376.<br />

622 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


67. Carter, S. L. (2009). Treatment of pica using a<br />

pica exchange procedure with increasing resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

effort. 44(1), 143–150.<br />

68. Carter, S. L., Wheeler, J. J., & Mayt<strong>on</strong>, M. R.<br />

(2004). Pica: A review of recent assessment<br />

<strong>and</strong> treatment procedures. 39(4), 3<str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>–358.<br />

69. Carter, S., & Wheeler, J. J. (2007). Functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

analysis <strong>and</strong> reducti<strong>on</strong> of inappropriate spitting.<br />

42(1), 59–64.<br />

Cascella, P. W. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #323.<br />

70. Caspar, L. A., & Glidden, L. M. (2001). Sexuality<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> for adults with developmental<br />

disabilities. 36(2), 172–177.<br />

Cavin, M. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #3.<br />

Cavin, M. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #4.<br />

71. Cavkaytar, A. (2007). Turkish parents as<br />

teachers: Teaching parents how to teach<br />

self-care <strong>and</strong> domestic skills to their children<br />

with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. 42(1), 85–93.<br />

Cavkaytar, A. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #267.<br />

72. Cavkaytar, A., & Pollard, E. (2009). Effectiveness<br />

of Parent <strong>and</strong> Therapist Collaborati<strong>on</strong><br />

Program (PTCP) for teaching self-care <strong>and</strong><br />

domestic skills to individuals with autism.<br />

44(3), 381–395.<br />

73. Certo, N. J., Mautz, D., Pumpian, I., Sax, C.,<br />

Smalley, K., Wade, H. A., Noyes, D., Luecking,<br />

R., Wechsler, J., & Batterman, N. (2003).<br />

Review <strong>and</strong> discussi<strong>on</strong> of a model for seamless<br />

transiti<strong>on</strong> to adulthood. 38(1), 3–17.<br />

Chadsey, J. G. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #75.<br />

Chambers, C. R. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #77.<br />

74. Chambers, C. R., Hughes, C., & Carter, E. W.<br />

(2004). Parent <strong>and</strong> sibling perspectives <strong>on</strong><br />

the transiti<strong>on</strong> to adulthood. 39(1), 79–94.<br />

Champlin, T. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #9.<br />

Champlin, T. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #8.<br />

Champlin, T. M. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #7.<br />

Chan, J. M. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #61.<br />

Chan, J. M. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #217.<br />

Chan, J. M. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #198.<br />

Chan, J. M. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #218.<br />

75. Chan, M., & Chadsey, J. G. (2006). High<br />

school teachers’ percepti<strong>on</strong>s of school-towork<br />

transiti<strong>on</strong> practices in Taiwan. 41(3),<br />

280–289.<br />

Cheatham, J. P. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #9.<br />

76. Chen, L., & Zhang, D. (2003). Transiti<strong>on</strong> services<br />

in Taiwan: Comparis<strong>on</strong> between service<br />

need <strong>and</strong> services received. 38(3), 334–<br />

340.<br />

77. Childre, A. L., & Chambers, C. R. (2005). Family<br />

Percepti<strong>on</strong>s of student centered planning<br />

<strong>and</strong> IEP meetings. 40(3), 217–233.<br />

Chitiyo, M. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #376.<br />

Chittooran, M. M. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #252.<br />

Chou, Y. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #258.<br />

Chun, E. J. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #110.<br />

Cihak, D. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #22.<br />

Cihak, D. F. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #131.<br />

78. Cihak, D. F., & Gama, R. I. (2008). N<strong>on</strong>c<strong>on</strong>tingent<br />

escape access to self-reinforcement to<br />

increase task engagement for students with<br />

moderate to severe disabilities. 43(4), 556–<br />

568.<br />

79. Cihak, D. F., Wright, R., & Ayres, K. M. (2010).<br />

Use of self-modeling static-picture prompts<br />

via a h<strong>and</strong>held compute to facilitate selfm<strong>on</strong>itoring<br />

in the general educati<strong>on</strong> classroom.<br />

45(1), 136–149.<br />

80. Cihak, D., Kessler, K., & Alberto, P. A. (2008).<br />

Use of a h<strong>and</strong>held prompting system to transiti<strong>on</strong><br />

independently through vocati<strong>on</strong>al task<br />

for students with moderate to severe intellectual<br />

disabilities. 43(1), 102–110.<br />

Clay, S. L. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #24.<br />

81. Cobigo, V. Morin, D., & Lachapelle, Y. (2007).<br />

Assessing work tasks preferences am<strong>on</strong>g<br />

pers<strong>on</strong>s with intellectual disabilities: An integrative<br />

review of literature. 42(3), 286–300.<br />

82. Cobigo, V., Morin, D., & Lachapelle, Y. (2009).<br />

Field test of a method to assess work task<br />

preferences. 44(4), 561–572.<br />

Cockram, J. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #225.<br />

Coffin, A. Bi. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #254.<br />

Cohen, S. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #207.<br />

Collins, B. C. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #2<str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>.<br />

Collins, B. C. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #130.<br />

Collins, B. C. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #344.<br />

Collins, B. C. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #148.<br />

Collins, B. C. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #165.<br />

Collins, B. C. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #145.<br />

Collins, B. C. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #176.<br />

83. Colozzi, G. A., Ward, L. W., & Crotty, K. E.<br />

(2008). Comparis<strong>on</strong> of simultaneous promting<br />

precedures in 1:1 <strong>and</strong> small group instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

to teach play skills to preschool students<br />

with pervasive developmental disorder<br />

<strong>and</strong> developmental disabilities. 43(2), 226–<br />

248.<br />

C<strong>on</strong>roy, J. W. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #327.<br />

C<strong>on</strong>roy, M. A. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #<str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>.<br />

84. C<strong>on</strong>yers, C., Martin, T. L., Martin, G. L., & Yu,<br />

D. (2002). The 1983 AAMR manual, the 1992<br />

AAMR manual, or the <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Act: Which do researchers use? 37(3),<br />

310–316.<br />

Cook, B. G. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #58.<br />

Cook, C. C. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #142.<br />

Copel<strong>and</strong>, S. R. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #171<br />

Cumulative Author Index, Volumes 36–45 / 623


Copel<strong>and</strong>, S. R. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #169<br />

Copel<strong>and</strong>, S. R. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #170.<br />

85. Copel<strong>and</strong>, S., & Hughes, C. (2002). Effects of<br />

goal setting <strong>on</strong> task performance of pers<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. 37(1), 40–54.<br />

86. Cordes, T. L., & Howard, R. W. (2005). C<strong>on</strong>cepts<br />

of work, leisure <strong>and</strong> retirement in adults<br />

with an intellectual disability. 40(2), 99–108.<br />

87. Cote, D., Pierce, T., Higgins, K., Miller, S.,<br />

T<strong>and</strong>y, R., & Sparks, S. (2010). Increasing<br />

skill performances of problem solving in students<br />

with intellectual disabilities. 45(4), 512–<br />

524.<br />

Courtade, G. R. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #174.<br />

88. Courtade, G., Browder, D. M., Spo<strong>on</strong>er, F., &<br />

DiBiase, W. (2010). Training teachers to use<br />

an inquiry-based task analysis to teach science<br />

to students with moderate <strong>and</strong> severe<br />

disabilities. 45(3), 378–399.<br />

Craig-Unkefer, L. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #213.<br />

Craig-Unkefer, L. A. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #361.<br />

Cramer, K. M. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #331.<br />

Cramer, K. M. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #332.<br />

Cramer, S. F. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #107.<br />

Cress, C. J. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #320.<br />

Cress, C. J. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #12.<br />

89. Cress, C. J., Arens, K. B., & Zajicek, A. K.<br />

(2007). Comparis<strong>on</strong> of engagement patterns<br />

of young children with developmental disabilities<br />

between structured <strong>and</strong> free play. 42(2),<br />

152–164.<br />

Crethar, H. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #141.<br />

90. Crites, S. A., & Dunn C. (2004). Teaching social<br />

problem solving to individuals with mental<br />

retardati<strong>on</strong>. 39(4), 301–309.<br />

Crompt<strong>on</strong>, A. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #259.<br />

Cr<strong>on</strong>in, B. A. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #237.<br />

Crotty, K. E. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #83.<br />

Crozier, S. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #45.<br />

Crozier, S. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #352.<br />

Czarnecki, D. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #350.<br />

D’Oosterlinck, F. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #51.<br />

Danbrook, M. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #307.<br />

91. Davies, D. K., Stock, S. E., & Wehmeyer, M. L.<br />

(2001). Enhancing independent internet access<br />

for individuals with mental retardati<strong>on</strong><br />

through use of a specialized web browser: A<br />

pilot study. 36(1), 107–115.<br />

92. Davies, D. K., Stock, S. E., & Wehmeyer, M. L.<br />

(2002). Enhancing independent task performance<br />

for individuals with mental retardati<strong>on</strong><br />

through use of a h<strong>and</strong>held self-directed visual<br />

<strong>and</strong> audio prompting system. 37(2),<br />

209–218.<br />

93. Davies, D. K., Stock, S. E., & Wehmeyer, M. L.<br />

(2003a). Applicati<strong>on</strong> of computer simulati<strong>on</strong><br />

to teach ATM access to individuals with intellectual<br />

disabilities. 38(4), 451–456.<br />

94. Davies, D. K., Stock, S. E., & Wehmeyer, M. L.<br />

(2003b). Utilizati<strong>on</strong> of computer technology<br />

to facilitate m<strong>on</strong>ey management by individuals<br />

with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. 38(1), 106–112.<br />

95. Davies, S., & Hastings, R. P. (2003). Computer<br />

technology in clinical psychology services for<br />

people with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>: A review.<br />

38(3), 341–352.<br />

Davis, A. K. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #367.<br />

Davis, S. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #52.<br />

Davis, T. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #217.<br />

Davis, T. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #198.<br />

Davis, T. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #218.<br />

Dazzi, C. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #286.<br />

de la Cruz, B. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #60.<br />

96. Delgado, C. E. F. (2009). Fourth grade outcomes<br />

of children with a preschool history of<br />

developmental disability. 44(4), 573–579.<br />

Delgado, C. F. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #311.<br />

Delgado, C. F. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #309.<br />

Dennis, L. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #17.<br />

97. Devlin, P. (2008). Enhancing the job performance<br />

of employees with disabilities using<br />

the Self-Determined Career Development<br />

Model. 43(4), 502–513.<br />

Devlin, S. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #195.<br />

98. Devlin, S. D., &. Harber, M. M. (2004). Collaborati<strong>on</strong><br />

am<strong>on</strong>g parents <strong>and</strong> professi<strong>on</strong>als<br />

with discrete trial training in the treatment for<br />

<strong>Autism</strong>. 39(4), 291–300.<br />

DiBiase, W. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #88.<br />

Didden, R. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #217.<br />

99. Dincer, B., & Erbas, D. (2010). Descripti<strong>on</strong> of<br />

communicati<strong>on</strong> breakdown repair strategies<br />

produced by n<strong>on</strong>verbal students with developmental<br />

disabilities. 45(3), 400–409.<br />

100. Di<strong>on</strong>, É., Wagner, S., & Brunet, J. (2002).<br />

High school inclusi<strong>on</strong> of adolescents with<br />

mental retardati<strong>on</strong>: A multiple case study.<br />

37(3), 253–261.<br />

Dix<strong>on</strong>, A. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #181.<br />

Doggett, R. A. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #195.<br />

101. Dogoe, M. S., B<strong>and</strong>a, D., & Lock, R. H.<br />

(2010). Acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> generalizati<strong>on</strong> of the<br />

picture exchange communicati<strong>on</strong> system behaviors<br />

across settings, pers<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>and</strong> stimulus<br />

classes with three students with autism.<br />

45(2), 216–229.<br />

102. Dogoe, M., & B<strong>and</strong>a, D. R. (2009). Review of<br />

recent research using c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay to<br />

teach chained tasks to pers<strong>on</strong>s with developmental<br />

disabilities. 44(2), 177–186.<br />

624 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


103. Dorminy, K. P., Luscre, D., & Gast, D. L.<br />

(2009). Teaching organizati<strong>on</strong>al skills to children<br />

with high functi<strong>on</strong>ing autism <strong>and</strong><br />

asperger’s syndrome. 44(4), 538–550.<br />

Doubet, S. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #274.<br />

Downs, A. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #105.<br />

104. Downs, A., Downs, R. C., Fossum, M., & Rau,<br />

K. (2008). Effectiveness of discrete trial<br />

teaching with preschool students with developmental<br />

disabilities. 43(4), 443–453.<br />

Downs, R. C. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #104.<br />

105. Downs, R. C., & Downs, A. (2010). Practices<br />

in early interventi<strong>on</strong> for children with autism:<br />

A comparis<strong>on</strong> with Nati<strong>on</strong>al Research Council<br />

recommended practices. 45(1), 150–159.<br />

Doy<strong>on</strong>, M. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #214.<br />

Duffy, M. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #55.<br />

Dunn, C. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #90.<br />

Dunn, W. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #256.<br />

Dyches, T. T. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #139.<br />

Dyches, T. T. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #205.<br />

106. Dyches, T. T., & Prater, M. A. (2005). Characteristics<br />

of developmental disability in children’s<br />

ficti<strong>on</strong>. 40(3), 202–216.<br />

107. Dyches, T. T., Prater, M. A., & Cramer, S. F.<br />

(2001). Characterizati<strong>on</strong>s of mental retardati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> autism in children’s literature. 36(3),<br />

230–242.<br />

108. Dyches, T. T., Prater, M., & Leininger, M.<br />

(2009). Juvenile literature <strong>and</strong> the portrayal of<br />

developmental disabilities. 44(3), 304–317.<br />

109. Dym<strong>on</strong>d, S. K., & Russell, D. L. (2004). Impact<br />

of grade <strong>and</strong> disability <strong>on</strong> the instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

c<strong>on</strong>text of inclusive classrooms. 39(2),<br />

127–140.<br />

110. Dym<strong>on</strong>d, S., Renzaglia, A., & Chun, E. J.<br />

(2008). Inclusive high school service learning<br />

programs: Methods for <strong>and</strong> barriers to including<br />

students with disabilities. 43(1), 20–<br />

36.<br />

111. Eaves, R. C., Woods-Groves, S., Williams,<br />

T. O., Jr., & Fall, A. (2006). Reliability <strong>and</strong><br />

validity of the Pervasive <strong>Developmental</strong> Disorders<br />

Rating Scale <strong>and</strong> the Gilliam <strong>Autism</strong><br />

Rating Scale. 41(3), 300–314.<br />

112. Edeh, O. M. (2006). Cross-cultural investigati<strong>on</strong><br />

of interest-based training <strong>and</strong> social interpers<strong>on</strong>al<br />

problem solving in students with<br />

mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. 41(2), 163–176.<br />

Edrisinha, C. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #60.<br />

113. Eisenman, L., Tanverdi, A., Perringt<strong>on</strong>, C., &<br />

Geiman, A. (2009). Sec<strong>on</strong>dary <strong>and</strong> postsec<strong>on</strong>dary<br />

community activities of youth with significant<br />

intellectual disabilities. 44(2), 168–<br />

176.<br />

114. Elgie, S., & Hastings, R. P. (2002). Staff definiti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

of challenging behavior. 37(2), 202–<br />

208.<br />

115. Ellerd, D. A., Morgan, R. L., & Salzberg, C. L.<br />

(2002). Comparis<strong>on</strong> of two approaches for<br />

identifying job preferences am<strong>on</strong>g pers<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with disabilities using video CD-ROM. 37(3),<br />

300–309.<br />

116. Ellerd, D. A., Morgan, R., & Salzberg, C. L.<br />

(2006). Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence between video CD-<br />

ROM <strong>and</strong> community-based job preferences<br />

for individuals with developmental disabilities.<br />

41(1), 81–94.<br />

Elliott, T. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #279.<br />

Elmensdorp, S. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #300.<br />

117. Embregts, P. J. C. M. (2003). Using self-management,<br />

video feedback, <strong>and</strong> graphic feedback<br />

to improve social behavior of youth with<br />

mild mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. 38(3), 283–295.<br />

118. Erbas, D. (2005). Resp<strong>on</strong>ses to communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

breakdowns by n<strong>on</strong>verbal children with<br />

developmental disabilities. 40(2), 145–157.<br />

119. Erbas, D. (2010). A collaborative approach to<br />

implement positive behavior support plans<br />

for children with problem behaviors: A comparis<strong>on</strong><br />

of c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong> versus c<strong>on</strong>sultati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> feedback approach. 45(1), 94–106.<br />

Erbas, D. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #99.<br />

120. Erbas, D., Ozen, A., & Acar, C. (2004). Comparis<strong>on</strong><br />

of two approaches for identifying reinforcers<br />

in teaching figure coloring to students<br />

with Down syndrome. 39(3), 253–264.<br />

121. Erbas, D., Ozen, A., Turan, Y., & Halle, J. W.<br />

(2006). Effect of adapted “Cover Write”<br />

method to teach spelling to students with<br />

developmental disabilities. 41(4), 357–364.<br />

122. Erbas, D., Tekin-Iftar, E., & Yucesoy, S.<br />

(2006). Teaching special educati<strong>on</strong> teachers<br />

how to c<strong>on</strong>duct functi<strong>on</strong>al analysis in natural<br />

settings. 41(1), 28–36.<br />

123. Erbas, D.,Yucesoy, S., Turan, Y., & Ostrosky,<br />

M. M. (2006). Turkish special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

teachers’ implementati<strong>on</strong> of functi<strong>on</strong>al analysis<br />

in classroom settings. 41(2), 155–162.<br />

124. Erez, G., & Peled, I. (2001). Cogniti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

metacogniti<strong>on</strong>: Evidence of higher thinking in<br />

problem solving of youth with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

36(1), 83–93.<br />

Ergenek<strong>on</strong>, Y. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #356.<br />

Ergul, C. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #54.<br />

Erkan, M. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #378.<br />

125. Ersoy, G., Tekin-Iftar, E., & Kircaali-Iftar, G.<br />

(2009). Effects of antecedent prompt <strong>and</strong> test<br />

procedure <strong>on</strong> teaching simulated menstrual<br />

care skills to females with developmental disabilities.<br />

44(1), 54–66.<br />

Esch, J. W. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #282.<br />

Cumulative Author Index, Volumes 36–45 / 625


126. Ezell, D., & Klein, C. (2003). Impact of portfolio<br />

assessment <strong>on</strong> locus of c<strong>on</strong>trol of students<br />

with <strong>and</strong> without disabilities. 38(2),<br />

220–228.<br />

127. Ezell, D., & Klein-Ezell, C. E. (2003).<br />

M.A.G.I.C. W.O.R.K.S. 38(4), 441–450.<br />

Fall, A. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #111.<br />

Fallin, K. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #52.<br />

Feldman, M. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #342.<br />

Feldman, M. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #307.<br />

128. Fergus<strong>on</strong>, H., Myles, B. S., & Hagiwara, T.<br />

(2005). Using a pers<strong>on</strong>al digital assistant to<br />

enhance the independence of an adolescent<br />

with Asperger syndrome. 40(1), 60–67.<br />

Fidler, D. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #388.<br />

129. Firman, K. B., Beare, P., & Lloyd, R. (2002).<br />

Enhancing self-management in students with<br />

mental retardati<strong>on</strong>: Extrinsic versus intrinsic<br />

procedures. 37(2), 163–171.<br />

130. Fiscus, R. S., Schuster, J., Morse, T. E., &<br />

Collins, B. C. (2002). Teaching elementary<br />

students with cognitive disabilities food preparati<strong>on</strong><br />

skills while embedding instructive<br />

feedback in the prompt <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>sequent<br />

event. 37(1), 55–69.<br />

Flanagan, S. M. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #43.<br />

Flanagan, S. M. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #42.<br />

131. Fletcher, D., Bo<strong>on</strong>, R., & Cihak, D. F. (2010).<br />

Effects of the TOUCHMATH program compared<br />

to a number line strategy to teach additi<strong>on</strong><br />

facts to middle school students with<br />

moderate intellectual disabilities. 45(3), 449–<br />

458.<br />

Fletcher, K. L. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #310.<br />

Fletcher, K. L. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #312.<br />

Fletcher, K. L. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #168.<br />

Fletcher, K. L. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #311.<br />

132. Fletcher, K. L., Blair, C., Scott, M. S., &<br />

Bolger, K. E. (2004). Specific patterns of cognitive<br />

abilities in young children with mild<br />

mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. 39(3), 270–189.<br />

Flexer, R. W. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #17.<br />

Flexer, R. W. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #193.<br />

Flores, M. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #49.<br />

133. Flores, M. M., & Ganz, J. B. (2009). Effects of<br />

direct instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the reading comprehensi<strong>on</strong><br />

of students with autism <strong>and</strong> developmental<br />

disabilities. 44(1), 39–53.<br />

Flowers, C. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #178.<br />

Floyd, R. G. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #26.<br />

Fogal, T. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #336.<br />

Fossum, M. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #104.<br />

134. Fowler, C. H., K<strong>on</strong>rad, M. Walker, A. R., Test,<br />

D. W., & Wood, W. M. (2007). Self-determinati<strong>on</strong><br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>s’ effects <strong>on</strong> the academic<br />

performance of students with developmental<br />

disabilities. 42(3), 270–285.<br />

135. Fox, R., Holtz, C. A., & Moist, A. M. (2009). A<br />

community-based accomodati<strong>on</strong> program<br />

for adults with autism <strong>and</strong> mental retardati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

44(1), 118–126.<br />

Foy, J. B. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #331.<br />

Foy, J. B. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #332.<br />

Fraiman, J. L. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #47.<br />

Frain, M. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #25.<br />

Franco, J. H. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #218.<br />

136. Frankl<strong>and</strong>, H. C., Turnbull, A. P., Wehmeyer,<br />

M. L., & Blackmountain, L. (2004). An explorati<strong>on</strong><br />

of the self-determinati<strong>on</strong> c<strong>on</strong>struct <strong>and</strong><br />

disability as it relates to the Dine (Navajo)<br />

culture. 39(3), 191–205.<br />

Fredrick, L. D. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #329.<br />

Freeman, A. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #266.<br />

Fridel, S. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #208.<br />

137. Friend, A., Summers, J. A., & Turnbull, A. P.<br />

(2009). Impacts of family support in early<br />

childhood interventi<strong>on</strong> research.44(4), 453–<br />

470.<br />

Gadzichowski, M. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #277.<br />

Gama, R. I. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #22.<br />

Gama, R. I. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #78.<br />

Ganz, J. B. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #133.<br />

138. Ganz, J. B., & Sigafoos, J. (2005). Self m<strong>on</strong>itoring:<br />

Are young adults with MR <strong>and</strong> autism<br />

able to utilize cognitive strategies independently?<br />

40(1), 24–33.<br />

Garcia, L. A. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #240.<br />

Garner, N. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #367.<br />

Gartin, B. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #252.<br />

Gartin, B. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #388.<br />

139. Gartin, B. C., Murdick, N. L., Thomps<strong>on</strong>, J. R.,<br />

& Dyches, T. T. (2002). Issues <strong>and</strong> challenges<br />

facing educators who advocate for<br />

students with disabilities. 37(1), 3–13.<br />

140. Gasc<strong>on</strong>, H., & Morin, P. (2010). Deinstituti<strong>on</strong>alisati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> adaptati<strong>on</strong> of adults with intellectual<br />

disabilities: Results from Quebec.<br />

45(3), 366–377.<br />

Gast, D. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #157.<br />

Gast, D. L. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #239.<br />

Gast, D. L. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #328.<br />

Gast, D. L. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #103.<br />

Gast, D. L. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #236.<br />

Gast, D. L. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #156.<br />

Gast, D. L. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #238.<br />

141. Gaudet, L., Pulos, S., Crethar, H., & Berger,<br />

S. (2002). Psychosocial c<strong>on</strong>cerns of adults<br />

with developmental disabilities: Perspectives<br />

626 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


of the self, family member, <strong>and</strong> provider.<br />

37(1), 23–26.<br />

Geiman, A. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #113.<br />

142. Geisthardt, C. L., Brothers<strong>on</strong>, M., & Cook,<br />

C. C. (2002). Friendships of children with disabilities<br />

in the home envir<strong>on</strong>ment. 37(3), 235–<br />

252.<br />

Getzel, E. E. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #348.<br />

Gifford, D. B. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #7.<br />

143. Gillespie, M. (2003). Cardiovascular fitness of<br />

young Canadian children with <strong>and</strong> without<br />

mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. 38(3), 296–301.<br />

144. Girolami, P. A., & Scotti, J. R. (2001). Use of<br />

analog functi<strong>on</strong>al analysis in assessing the<br />

functi<strong>on</strong> of mealtime problem behaviors.<br />

36(2), 207–223.<br />

Glaeser, B. C. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #284.<br />

Glaeser, B. C. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #285.<br />

Glaubman, R. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #208.<br />

Glidden, L. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #199.<br />

Glidden, L. M. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #70.<br />

145. Godsey, J. R., Schuster, J. W., Lingo, A. S.,<br />

Collins, B. C., & Kleinert, H. L. (2008). Peerimplemented<br />

time delay procedures <strong>on</strong> the<br />

acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of chained tasks by students with<br />

moderate <strong>and</strong> severe disabilities. 43(1), 111–<br />

122.<br />

1<str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>. Goeke, J. L. (2003). Parents speak out: Facial<br />

plastic surgery for children with Down syndrome.<br />

38(3), 323–333.<br />

Gomez, O. N. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #20.<br />

G<strong>on</strong>zalez, L. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #188.<br />

Goodman, R. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #381.<br />

147. Graetz, J. E., Mastropieri, M. A., & Scruggs,<br />

T. E. (2009). Decreasing inappropriate behaviors<br />

for adolescents with autism spectrum<br />

disorder using modified social stories. 44(1),<br />

91–104.<br />

Gragoudas, S. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #202.<br />

148. Graves, T. B., Collins, B. C., Schuster, J. W.,<br />

& Kleinert, H. (2005). Using video prompting<br />

to teach cooking skills to sec<strong>on</strong>dary students<br />

with moderate disabilities. 40(1), 34–<str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>.<br />

149. Greef, A. P., & van der Walt, K. (2010). Resilience<br />

in families with an autistic child. 45(3),<br />

347–355.<br />

Greer, R. D. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #297.<br />

Grenwelge, C. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #386.<br />

Grider, K. L. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #358.<br />

Grider, K. M. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #358.<br />

150. Griffin, M., McMillan, E. D., & Hodapp, R. M.<br />

(2010). Family perspectives <strong>on</strong> post-sec<strong>on</strong>dary<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> for students with intellectual<br />

disabilities. 45(3), 339–3<str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>.<br />

Griffiths, D. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #342.<br />

Grigal, M. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #260.<br />

151. Grigal, M., Neubert, D. A., & Mo<strong>on</strong>, M. S.<br />

(2001). Public school programs for students<br />

with significant disabilities in post-sec<strong>on</strong>dary<br />

settings. 36(3), 244–254.<br />

Grimmett, E. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #21.<br />

Grisham-Brown, J. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #2<str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>.<br />

Griswold, D. E. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #257.<br />

Griswold, D. E. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #255.<br />

Grossman, B. G. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #254.<br />

152. Grunsell, J., & Carter, M. (2002). Behavior<br />

chain interrupti<strong>on</strong> strategy: Generalizati<strong>on</strong> to<br />

out-of-routine c<strong>on</strong>texts. 37(4), 378–390.<br />

Grupe, L. A. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #168.<br />

Guenther, T. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #45.<br />

Gursel, O. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #48.<br />

153. Gursel, O., Tekin-Iftar, E., & Bozkurt, F.<br />

(2006). Effectiveness of simultaneous<br />

prompting in small group: The opportunity of<br />

acquiring n<strong>on</strong>-target skills through observati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

learning <strong>and</strong> instructive feedback.<br />

41(3), 225–243.<br />

Gustafs<strong>on</strong>, M. R. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #236.<br />

Guth, C. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #171.<br />

Guy, B. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #211.<br />

154. Hager, K. D., & Slocum, T. A. (2008). Utah’s<br />

alternative assessment: Evidence regarding<br />

six aspects of validity. 43(2), 144–161.<br />

Hagiwara, M. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #229.<br />

Hagiwara, T. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #257.<br />

Hagiwara, T. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #255.<br />

Hagiwara, T. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #256.<br />

Hagiwara, T. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #128.<br />

Hall, L. J. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #34.<br />

155. Hall, L. J., & Strickett, T. (2002). Peer relati<strong>on</strong>ships<br />

of preadolescent students with disabilities<br />

who attend a separate school. 37(4),<br />

399–409.<br />

Halle, J. W. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #121.<br />

156. Hamm<strong>on</strong>d, D. L., Whatley, A. D., Ayres, K. M.,<br />

& Gast, D. (2010). Effectiveness of video<br />

modeling to teach iPod use to students with<br />

moderate intellectual disabilities. 45(4), 525–<br />

538.<br />

157. Hamm<strong>on</strong>d, D., & Gast, D. L. (2010). Descriptive<br />

analysis of single subject research designs:<br />

1983–2007. 45(2), 187–202.<br />

Hanley, L. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #33.<br />

Hanline, M. F. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #374.<br />

Hansen, C. D. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #212.<br />

158. Hansen, D. L., & Morgan, R. L. (2008). Teaching<br />

grocery store purchasing skills to students<br />

with intellectual disabilities using a<br />

Cumulative Author Index, Volumes 36–45 / 627


computer-based instructi<strong>on</strong> program. 43(4),<br />

431–442.<br />

Harapiak, S. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #381.<br />

Harber M. M. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #98.<br />

Haring, K. A. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #215.<br />

Harris, K. C. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #300.<br />

Hart, D. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #382.<br />

159. Hart, D., Mele-McCarthy, J., Pasternack,<br />

R. H., Zimbrich, K., & Parker, D. R. (2004).<br />

Community college: A pathway to success<br />

for youth with learning, cognitive, <strong>and</strong> intellectual<br />

disabilities in sec<strong>on</strong>dary settings.<br />

39(1), 54–66.<br />

Hastings, R. P. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #95.<br />

Hastings, R. P. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #114.<br />

Hastings, R. P. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #343.<br />

160. Hastings, R. P., & No<strong>on</strong>e, S. J. (2005). Selfinjurious<br />

behavior <strong>and</strong> functi<strong>on</strong>al analysis:<br />

Ethics <strong>and</strong> evidence. 40(4), 335–342.<br />

Hawkins, K. A. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #47.<br />

Heath, M. A. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #205.<br />

Heck, R. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #226.<br />

Heck, R. H. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #261.<br />

Heflin, L. J. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #329.<br />

Heller, K. W. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #329.<br />

Hendricks<strong>on</strong>, J. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #315.<br />

Henley, J. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #59.<br />

Henry, S. A. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #254.<br />

Herold, J. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #229.<br />

Hess, J. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #274.<br />

161. Hetzr<strong>on</strong>i, O. E., & Roth, T. (2003). Effects of a<br />

positive support approach to enhance communicative<br />

behaviors of children with mental<br />

retardati<strong>on</strong> who have challenging behaviors.<br />

38(1), 95–105.<br />

162. Hetzr<strong>on</strong>i, O. E., & Shavit, P. (2002). Comparis<strong>on</strong><br />

of two instructi<strong>on</strong>al strategies for acquiring<br />

form <strong>and</strong> sound of Hebrew letters by<br />

students with mild mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. 37(3),<br />

273–282.<br />

Hiebert, R. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #381.<br />

163. Hiemstra, S. J., Vlaskamp, C., & Wiersma,<br />

L. A. (2007). Individual focus in an activity<br />

centre: An observati<strong>on</strong>al study am<strong>on</strong>g pers<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with profound <strong>and</strong> multiple disabilities.<br />

42(1), 14–23.<br />

Higgins, K. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #87.<br />

Hodapp, R. M. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #150.<br />

Hoekstra, S. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #229.<br />

Holtz, C. A. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #135.<br />

Hopkins, S. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #198.<br />

Hopps, S. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #214.<br />

164. Horn, C. (2010). Resp<strong>on</strong>se cards: An effective<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> for students with disabilities.<br />

45(1), 116–123.<br />

165. Horn, C., Schuster, J. W., & Collins, B. C.<br />

(2006). Use of resp<strong>on</strong>se cards to teach telling<br />

time to students with moderate <strong>and</strong> severe<br />

disabilities. 41(4), 382–391.<br />

Horvat, M. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #319.<br />

Houchins, D. E. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #175.<br />

Houchins, D. E. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #49.<br />

Hourcade, J. J. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #273.<br />

Hourcade, J. J. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #271.<br />

166. Hourcade, J. J., & Parette, P. (2001). Providing<br />

assistive technology informati<strong>on</strong> to professi<strong>on</strong>als<br />

<strong>and</strong> families of children with<br />

MRDD: Interactive CD-ROM technology.<br />

36(3), 272–279.<br />

167. Hourcade, J. J., Parette, P., & Anders<strong>on</strong>, H.<br />

(2003). Accountability in collaborati<strong>on</strong>: A<br />

framework for evaluati<strong>on</strong>. 38(4), 398–404.<br />

Howard, R. W. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #86.<br />

Huang, A. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #376.<br />

Huang, T. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #377.<br />

Huds<strong>on</strong>, J. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #258.<br />

Huebner, R. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #188.<br />

Huer, M. B. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #273.<br />

Huer, M. B. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #269.<br />

Huettig, C. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #298.<br />

168. Huffman, L. F., Fletcher, K. L., Bray, N. W., &<br />

Grupe, L. A. (2004). Similarities <strong>and</strong> differences<br />

in additi<strong>on</strong> strategies of children with<br />

<strong>and</strong> without mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. 39(4), 317–<br />

325.<br />

Huggins, A. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #257.<br />

Huggins, A. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #256.<br />

Hughes, C. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #85.<br />

Hughes, C. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #74.<br />

Hughes, C. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #3.<br />

169. Hughes, C., Carter, E. W., Hughes, T., Bradford,<br />

E., & Copel<strong>and</strong>, S. R. (2002). Effects of<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>al versus n<strong>on</strong>-instructi<strong>on</strong>al roles <strong>on</strong><br />

social interacti<strong>on</strong>s of high school students.<br />

37(2), 1<str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>–162.<br />

170. Hughes, C., Copel<strong>and</strong>, S. R., Agran, M., Wehmeyer,<br />

M. L., Rodi, M. S., & Presley, J. A.<br />

(2002). Using self-m<strong>on</strong>itoring to improve performance<br />

in general educati<strong>on</strong> high school<br />

classes. 37(3), 262–272.<br />

171. Hughes, C., Copel<strong>and</strong>, S. R., Guth, C., Rung,<br />

L. L., Hwang, B., Kleeb, G., & Str<strong>on</strong>g, M.<br />

(2001). General educati<strong>on</strong> students’ perspectives<br />

<strong>on</strong> their involvement in a high<br />

school peer buddy program. 36(4), 343–356.<br />

Hughes, T. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #169.<br />

Hupp, S. C. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #185.<br />

628 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


172. Hurst, M., & Jolivette, K. (2006). Effects of<br />

private versus public assessment <strong>on</strong> the<br />

reading fluency of middle school students<br />

with mild disabilities. 41(2), 185–198.<br />

Hutchins, M. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #24.<br />

Hwang, B. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #171.<br />

Hwang, B. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #187.<br />

Hyde, M. S. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #192.<br />

Ivester, J. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #385.<br />

173. Ivey, J. K. (2007). Outcomes for students with<br />

<strong>Autism</strong> Spectrum Disorders: What is important<br />

<strong>and</strong> likely according to teachers? 42(1),<br />

3–13<br />

James<strong>on</strong>, M. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #228.<br />

174. Jimenez, B. A., Browder, D. M., & Courtade,<br />

G. R. (2008). Teaching an algebraic equati<strong>on</strong><br />

to high school students with moderate developmental<br />

disabilities. 43(2), 266–278.<br />

Jobe, B. M. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #199.<br />

Johns<strong>on</strong>, J. W. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #227.<br />

Johns<strong>on</strong>, J. W. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #228.<br />

Johns<strong>on</strong>, J. W. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #358.<br />

Johnst<strong>on</strong>, S. S. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #259.<br />

Jolivette, K. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #172.<br />

175. Jolivette, K., Lingo, A. S., Houchins, D. E.,<br />

Bart<strong>on</strong>-Arwood, S., & Shippen, M. E. (2006).<br />

Building math fluency for students with developmental<br />

disabilities <strong>and</strong> attenti<strong>on</strong> difficulties<br />

using Great Leaps Math. 41(4), 392–400.<br />

J<strong>on</strong>es, E. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #266.<br />

J<strong>on</strong>es, F. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #8.<br />

176. J<strong>on</strong>es, K. L., Collins, B. C., Schuster, J. W., &<br />

Kleinert, H. (2009). Presenting chained <strong>and</strong><br />

discrete tasks as n<strong>on</strong>targeted informati<strong>on</strong><br />

when teaching discrete academic skills<br />

through small group instructi<strong>on</strong>. 44(1), 127–<br />

142.<br />

Jordan, L. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #194.<br />

177. Joseph, L. M., & McCachran, M. (2003).<br />

Comparis<strong>on</strong> of a word study ph<strong>on</strong>ics technique<br />

between students with moderate to<br />

mild mental retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> struggling readers<br />

without disabilities. 38(2), 192–199.<br />

Kane, M. T. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #292.<br />

Karv<strong>on</strong>en, M. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #52.<br />

178. Karv<strong>on</strong>en, M., Flowers, C., Browder, D. M.,<br />

Wakeman, S. Y., & Algozzine, B. (2006).<br />

Case study of the influences <strong>on</strong> alternate assessment<br />

outcomes for students with disabilities.<br />

41(2), 95–110.<br />

179. Katims, D. S. (2001). Literacy assessment<br />

<strong>and</strong> students with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>: An exploratory<br />

investigati<strong>on</strong>. 36(4), 363–372.<br />

Katsiyannis, A. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #385.<br />

180. Katsiyannis, A., Zhang, D., & Archwamety, T.<br />

(2002). Placement <strong>and</strong> exit patterns for students<br />

with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>: An analysis of<br />

nati<strong>on</strong>al trends. 37(2), 134–145.<br />

181. Katsiyannis, A., Zhang, D., Woodruff, N., &<br />

Dix<strong>on</strong>, A. (2005).Transiti<strong>on</strong> supports to students<br />

with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>: An examinati<strong>on</strong><br />

of data from the Nati<strong>on</strong>al L<strong>on</strong>gitudinal<br />

Transiti<strong>on</strong> Study 2. 40(2), 109–116.<br />

182. Keller, C. L., Brady, M. P., & Taylor, R. L.<br />

(2005). Using self evaluati<strong>on</strong> to improve student<br />

teacher interns’ use of specific praise.<br />

40(4), 368–376.<br />

183. Kellow, J. T., & Parker, R. I. (2002). Selfpercepti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

of adequacy of support am<strong>on</strong>g<br />

pers<strong>on</strong>s with mental retardati<strong>on</strong> living in suburban<br />

versus rural communities. 37(3), 328–<br />

338.<br />

Kelly, M. L. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #318.<br />

184. Kemp, C., & Carter, M. (2006). Active <strong>and</strong><br />

passive task related behavior, directi<strong>on</strong> following<br />

<strong>and</strong> the inclusi<strong>on</strong> of children with disabilities.<br />

41(1), 14–27.<br />

Kercher, K. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #228.<br />

Kessler, K. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #80.<br />

185. Kim, O., & Hupp, S. C. (2005). Teacher interacti<strong>on</strong><br />

styles <strong>and</strong> task engagement of elementary<br />

students with cognitive disabilities.<br />

40(3), 293–308.<br />

186. Kim, Y. (2010). Pers<strong>on</strong>al safety programs for<br />

children with intellectual disabilities. 45(2),<br />

312–319.<br />

187. Kim, Y., Yang, Y., & Hwang, B. (2001). Generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

effects of script-based interventi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>on</strong> language expressi<strong>on</strong> of preschool<br />

children with language disorders. 36(4), 411–<br />

423.<br />

Kircaali-Iftar, G. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #345.<br />

Kircaali-Iftar, G. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #125.<br />

Kleeb, G. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #171.<br />

Klein, C. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #126.<br />

Klein, R. E. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #57.<br />

Kleinert, H. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #344.<br />

Kleinert, H. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #148.<br />

Kleinert, H. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #176.<br />

Kleinert, H. L. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #145.<br />

188. Kleinert, J. O., G<strong>on</strong>zalez, L., Schuster, J. W.,<br />

& Huebner, R. (2007). Comparis<strong>on</strong> of syntax<br />

training for students with developmental disabilities<br />

utilizing clinician-directed versus<br />

self-determined sessi<strong>on</strong> paradigms. 42(1),<br />

65–84.<br />

Klein-Ezell, C. E. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #127.<br />

189. Knight, M. G., Ross, D. E., Taylor, R. L., &<br />

Ramasamy, R. (2003). C<strong>on</strong>stant time delay<br />

<strong>and</strong> interspersal of known items to teach<br />

Cumulative Author Index, Volumes 36–45 / 629


sight words to students with mental retardati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> learning disabilities. 38(2), 179–191.<br />

K<strong>on</strong>rad, M. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #134.<br />

190. K<strong>on</strong>rad, M., Trela, K., & Test, D. W. (2006).<br />

Using IEP goals <strong>and</strong> objectives to teach<br />

paragraph writing to high school students<br />

with physical <strong>and</strong> cognitive disabilities. 41(2),<br />

111–124.<br />

K<strong>on</strong>tosh, L. G. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #55.<br />

K<strong>on</strong>ukman, F. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #378.<br />

K<strong>on</strong>ukman, F. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #379.<br />

K<strong>on</strong>ukman, F. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #380.<br />

Kozen, A. A. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #223.<br />

191. Kozub, F. M. (2003). Explaining physical activity<br />

in individuals with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>: An<br />

exploratory study. 38(3), 302–313.<br />

192. Krajewski, J. J., Hyde, M. S., & O’Keeffe,<br />

M. K. (2002). Teen attitudes toward individuals<br />

with mental retardati<strong>on</strong> from 1987 to 1998:<br />

Impact of resp<strong>on</strong>dent gender <strong>and</strong> school<br />

variables. 37(1), 27–39.<br />

193. Kreiner, J., & Flexer, R. W. (2009). Assessment<br />

of leisure preferences for students with<br />

severe developmental disabilities <strong>and</strong> communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

difficulties. 44(2), 280–294.<br />

194. Kretlow, A., Lo, Y., White, R. B., & Jordan, L.<br />

(2008). Teaching test-taking strategies to improve<br />

the academic achievement of students<br />

with mild mental disabilities. 43(3), 397–408.<br />

Krupp, M. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #1.<br />

195. Kuhn, L. R., Bodkin, A. E., Devlin, S., &<br />

Doggett, R. A. (2008). Using pivotal resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

training with peers in special educati<strong>on</strong> to<br />

facilitate play in two children with autism.<br />

43(1), 37–45.<br />

Kwe<strong>on</strong>, G. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #31.<br />

Labin, J. M. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #47.<br />

Lachapelle, Y. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #81.<br />

Lachapelle, Y. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #82.<br />

196. Lambrechts, G., Petry, K., & Maes, B. (2008).<br />

Staff variables that influence resp<strong>on</strong>ses to<br />

challenging behaviour of clients with intellectual<br />

disability: A review. 43(4), 454–473.<br />

Lance, G. D. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #368.<br />

Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, G. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #217.<br />

Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, G. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #198.<br />

Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, G. E. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #60.<br />

L<strong>and</strong>mark, L. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #386.<br />

Lang, R. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #217.<br />

Lang, R. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #218.<br />

197. Lang, R., Machalicek, W., O’Reilly, M., Sigafoos,<br />

J., Rispoli, M., Shogren, K., & Regester,<br />

A. (2009). Review of teacher involvement in<br />

the applied interventi<strong>on</strong> research for children<br />

with autism spectrum disorders. 44(4), 481–<br />

492.<br />

198. Lang, R., O’Reilly, M. Sigafoos, J., Machalicek,<br />

W., Rispoli, M., Shogren, K., Chan,<br />

J. M., Davis, T., Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, G., & Hopkins, S.<br />

(2010). Review of teacher involvement in the<br />

applied interventi<strong>on</strong> research for children<br />

with autism spectrum disorders. 45(2), 268–<br />

283.<br />

Lang<strong>on</strong>e, J. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #317.<br />

Lang<strong>on</strong>e, J. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #14.<br />

Lang<strong>on</strong>e, J. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #15.<br />

Langthorne, P. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #217.<br />

Lars<strong>on</strong>, L. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #11.<br />

Lattin, D. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #369.<br />

199. Lawrence, E. R., Glidden, L., & Jobe, B. M.<br />

(2006). Keeping them happy: Job satisfacti<strong>on</strong>,<br />

pers<strong>on</strong>ality, <strong>and</strong> attitudes toward disability<br />

in predicting counselor job retenti<strong>on</strong>.<br />

41(1), 70–80.<br />

Lawrence, M. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #367.<br />

Leake, D. W. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #318.<br />

Lee, A. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #245.<br />

200. Lee, E., & Lee, S. (2009). Effects of instructi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

rubrics <strong>on</strong> class engagement behaviors<br />

<strong>and</strong> the achievement of less<strong>on</strong> objectives<br />

by students with mild mental retardati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> their typical peers. 44(3), 396–408.<br />

Lee, H. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #258.<br />

201. Lee, M. S. H., Nguyen, D., Yu, C. T., Thorsteinss<strong>on</strong>,<br />

J. R., Martin, T. L., & Martin, G. L.<br />

(2008). Discriminati<strong>on</strong> skills predict effective<br />

preference assessment methods for adults<br />

with developmental disabilities. 43(3), 388–<br />

396.<br />

Lee, S. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #200.<br />

202. Lee, S., Amos, B. A., Gragoudas, S., Lee, Y.,<br />

Shogren, K. A., Theoharis, R., & Wehmeyer,<br />

M. L. (2006). Curriculum augmentati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

adaptati<strong>on</strong> strategies to promote access to<br />

general curriculum for students with intellectual<br />

<strong>and</strong> developmental disabilities. 41(3),<br />

199–212.<br />

203. Lee, S., Post<strong>on</strong>, D., & Post<strong>on</strong>, AJ. (2007).<br />

Less<strong>on</strong>s learned through implementing a<br />

positive behavioral support interventi<strong>on</strong> at<br />

home: A case study <strong>on</strong> self-management<br />

with a student with autism <strong>and</strong> his mother.<br />

42(4), 418–427.<br />

204. Lee, S., Yoo, S., & Bak, S. (2003). Characteristics<br />

of friendships between children with<br />

<strong>and</strong> without mild disabilities. 38(2), 157–166.<br />

Lee, Y. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #202.<br />

Leeper, D. C. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #372.<br />

Lehtinen, U. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #303.<br />

Leimkuhl, T. T. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #265.<br />

630 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


Leininger, M. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #108.<br />

205. Leininger, M., Dyches, T. T., Prater, M., &<br />

Heath, M. A. (2010). Newbery award winning<br />

books 1975–2009: How do they portray disabilities?<br />

45(4), 583–596.<br />

Liaupsin, C. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #355.<br />

Liaupsin, C. J. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #31.<br />

206. Lifshitz, H. (2001). Aging phenomena am<strong>on</strong>g<br />

people with mental retardati<strong>on</strong> in community<br />

residence in Israel. 36(1), 30–41.<br />

207. Lifshitz, H., Nissim, S., & Cohen, S. (2008).<br />

Attitudes of Israeli teachers <strong>and</strong> paraprofesi<strong>on</strong>als<br />

towards the new definiti<strong>on</strong> of ID <strong>and</strong><br />

their willingness to cope with special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

law changes. 43(4), 514–528.<br />

208. Lifshitz, H., Weiss, I., Fridel, S., & Glaubman,<br />

R. (2009). Why do individuals with intellectual<br />

disability turn to religi<strong>on</strong>: A comparis<strong>on</strong> between<br />

adolescents <strong>and</strong> adults. 44(2), 196–<br />

206.<br />

209. Lim, L., Browder, D. M., & Bambara, L.<br />

(2001). Effects of sampling opportunities <strong>on</strong><br />

preference development for adults with severe<br />

disabilities. 36(2), 188–195.<br />

210. Lim, L., Lin, C., & Browder, D. M. (2002).<br />

Applicati<strong>on</strong>s of moti<strong>on</strong> study in developmental<br />

disabilities: A review. 37(1), 105–118.<br />

Lin, C. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #210.<br />

211. Lindsey, P., Wehmeyer, M. L., Guy, B., &<br />

Martin, J. (2001). Age of majority <strong>and</strong> Mental<br />

retardati<strong>on</strong>: A positi<strong>on</strong> statement of the <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

<strong>on</strong> mental Retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>. 36(1), 3–15.<br />

Lingo, A. S. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #175.<br />

Lingo, A. S. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #145.<br />

Lloyd, L. L. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #35.<br />

Lloyd, R. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #129.<br />

Lo, Y. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #194.<br />

Lock, R. H. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #101.<br />

212. Logan, K. R., Hansen, C. D., Nieminen, P. K.,<br />

& Wright, E. H. (2001). Student support<br />

teams: Helping students succeed in general<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> classrooms or working to place<br />

students in special educati<strong>on</strong>? 36(3), 280–<br />

292.<br />

213. L<strong>on</strong>cola, J. A., & Craig-Unkefer, L. (2005).<br />

Teaching social communicati<strong>on</strong> skills to<br />

young urban children with autism. 40(3),<br />

243–263.<br />

214. Loranger, M., Blais, M., Hopps, S., Pèpin, M.,<br />

Boisvert, J., & Doy<strong>on</strong>, M. (2002). Applicati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

of measures of speed of mental operati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

am<strong>on</strong>g children with intellectual deficiency.<br />

37(2), 184–192.<br />

215. Lovett, D. L., & Haring, K. A. (2003). Family<br />

percepti<strong>on</strong>s of transiti<strong>on</strong>s in early interventi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

38(4), 370–377.<br />

Lubin, J. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #288.<br />

Lubin, J. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #287.<br />

Luecking, R. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #73.<br />

Luscre, D. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #103.<br />

216. Lustig, D. C. (2002). Family coping in families<br />

with a child with a disability. 37(1), 14–22.<br />

Maccubbin, E. M. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #277.<br />

Machalicek, W. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #197.<br />

Machalicek, W. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #198.<br />

217. Machalicek, W. O’Reilly, M., Chan, J. M.,<br />

Lang, R., Rispoli, M., Davis, T., Shogren, K.,<br />

Sigafoos, J., Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, G., Ant<strong>on</strong>ucci, M.,<br />

Langthorne, P., Andrews, A., & Didden, R.<br />

(2009). Using videoc<strong>on</strong>ferencing to c<strong>on</strong>duct<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>al analysis of challenging behavior<br />

<strong>and</strong> develop classroom support plans for students<br />

with autism. 44(2), 207–217.<br />

218. Machalicek, W., O’Reilly, M. R., Rispoli, M.,<br />

Davis, T., Lang, R. Franco, J. H., & Chan,<br />

J. M. (2010). Training teachers to assess the<br />

challenging behaviors of student with autism<br />

using video tele-c<strong>on</strong>ferencing. 45(2), 203–<br />

215.<br />

Maes, B. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #281.<br />

Maes, B. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #196.<br />

Maguire, A. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #16.<br />

Mallow, L. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #293<br />

219. Mancil, G. R. (2006). Functi<strong>on</strong>al communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

training: A review of the literature related<br />

to children with <strong>Autism</strong>. 41(3), 213–224.<br />

Mancil, G. R. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #<str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>.<br />

220. Mancil, G. R. (2009). Milieu therapy as a communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>: A review of the literature<br />

related to children with autism spectrum<br />

disorder. 44(1), 105–117.<br />

Mancil, G. R. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #305.<br />

221. Mancil, G. R., Boyd, B. A., & Bedesem, P.<br />

(2009). Parental stress <strong>and</strong> autism: Are there<br />

useful coping strategies. 44(4), 523–537.<br />

Manser, S. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #229.<br />

Martin, G. L. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #84.<br />

Martin, G. L. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #381.<br />

Martin, G. L. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #308.<br />

Martin, G. L. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #350.<br />

Martin, G. L. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #201.<br />

Martin, G. L. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #307.<br />

Martin, J. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #211.<br />

Martin, T. G. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #381.<br />

Martin, T. L. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #381.<br />

Martin, T. L. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #84.<br />

Martin, T. L. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #350.<br />

Martin, T. L. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #201.<br />

Marvin, C. A. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #12.<br />

Cumulative Author Index, Volumes 36–45 / 631


Mastropieri, M. A. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #147.<br />

Mathes, P. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #9.<br />

Mathes, P. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #7.<br />

Mathes, P. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #8.<br />

222. Mattie, H. (2001). Generalizati<strong>on</strong> effects of<br />

cognitive strategies c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong> training for<br />

adults with moderate to severe disabilities.<br />

36(2), 178–187.<br />

223. Mattie, H. D., & Kozen, A. A. (2007). C<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong><br />

of behavior states <strong>and</strong> patterns in IEP<br />

development <strong>and</strong> daily planning: A multiple<br />

case study approach involving students with<br />

multiple disabilities. 42(1), 38–47.<br />

Mautz, D. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #73.<br />

Mayt<strong>on</strong>, M. R. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #375.<br />

Mayt<strong>on</strong>, M. R. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #68.<br />

Mayt<strong>on</strong>, M. R. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #376.<br />

224. Mayt<strong>on</strong>, M. R., Wheeler, J. J., Menendez,<br />

A. L., & Zhang, J. (2010). An analysis of<br />

evidence-based practices in the educati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> treatment of learners with autism spectrum<br />

disorders. 45(4), 539–551.<br />

225. McAfee, J. K., Cockram, J., & Wolfe, P. S.<br />

(2001). Police reacti<strong>on</strong>s to crimes involving<br />

pers<strong>on</strong>s with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. 36(2), 160–<br />

171.<br />

McCachran, M. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #177.<br />

McClim<strong>on</strong>, D. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #16.<br />

McCormick, L. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #261.<br />

226. McCormick, L., No<strong>on</strong>an, M., Ogata, V., &<br />

Heck, R. (2001). Co-teacher relati<strong>on</strong>ship <strong>and</strong><br />

program quality: Implicati<strong>on</strong>s for preparing<br />

teachers for inclusive preschool settings.<br />

36(2), 119–132.<br />

McCrimm<strong>on</strong>, A. W. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #248.<br />

McD<strong>on</strong>nell, A. P. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #259.<br />

227. McD<strong>on</strong>nell, J., Johns<strong>on</strong>, J. W., Polychr<strong>on</strong>is,<br />

S., & Riesen, T. (2002). Effects of embedded<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> students with moderate disabilities<br />

enrolled in general educati<strong>on</strong> classes.<br />

37(4), 363–377.<br />

228. McD<strong>on</strong>nell, J., Johns<strong>on</strong>, J. W., Polychr<strong>on</strong>is,<br />

S., Riesen, T., James<strong>on</strong>, M., & Kercher, K.<br />

(2006). Comparis<strong>on</strong> of <strong>on</strong>e-to-<strong>on</strong>e embedded<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> in general educati<strong>on</strong> classes<br />

with small group instructi<strong>on</strong> in special educati<strong>on</strong><br />

classes. 41(2), 125–138.<br />

McKenney, E. L.W. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #<str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>.<br />

McMillan, E. D. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #15.<br />

McQueen-Fuentes, G. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong><br />

#342.<br />

Meadan, H. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #274.<br />

229. Meadan, H., Thomps<strong>on</strong>, J. R., Hagiwara, M.,<br />

Herold, J., Hoekstra, S., & Manser, S. (2009).<br />

Evaluating the acceptability <strong>and</strong> effective-<br />

ness of family assessment portfolios. 44(3),<br />

421–430.<br />

230. Mechling, L., & O’Brien, E. (2010). Computerbased<br />

video instructi<strong>on</strong> to teach students<br />

with intellectual disabilities to use public bus<br />

transportati<strong>on</strong>. 45(2), 230–241.<br />

231. Mechling, L. C. (2007). Assistive technology<br />

as a self-management tool for prompting students<br />

with intellectual disabilities to initiate<br />

<strong>and</strong> complete daily tasks: A literature review.<br />

42(3), 252–269.<br />

232. Mechling, L. C. (2008). High tech cooking: A<br />

literature review of evolving technologies for<br />

teaching a functi<strong>on</strong>al skill. 43(4), 474–485.<br />

233. Mechling, L. C. (2008). Thirty year review of<br />

safety skill instructi<strong>on</strong> for pers<strong>on</strong>s with intellectual<br />

disability. 43(3), 311–323.<br />

234. Mechling, L. C., & Ortega-Hurnd<strong>on</strong>, F.<br />

(2007). Computer-based video instructi<strong>on</strong> to<br />

teach young adults with moderate intellectual<br />

disabilities to perform multiple step, job tasks<br />

in a generalized setting. 42(1), 24–37.<br />

235. Mechling, L. C., & Stephens, E. (2009). Comparis<strong>on</strong><br />

of self-prompting of cooking skills via<br />

picture-based cookbooks <strong>and</strong> video recipes.<br />

44(2), 218–236.<br />

236. Mechling, L. C., Gast, D. L., & Gustafs<strong>on</strong>,<br />

M. R. (2009). Use of video modeling to teach<br />

extinguishing of cooking related fires to individuals<br />

with moderate intellectual disabilities.<br />

44(1), 67–79.<br />

237. Mechling, L. C., Pridgen, L. S., & Cr<strong>on</strong>in, B. A.<br />

(2005). Computer-based video instructi<strong>on</strong> to<br />

teach students with intellectual disabilities to<br />

verbally resp<strong>on</strong>d to questi<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> make purchases<br />

in fast food restaurants. 40(1), 47–59.<br />

238. Mechling, L., Gast, D. L., & Seid, N. H.<br />

(2010). Evaluati<strong>on</strong> of a pers<strong>on</strong>al digital assistant<br />

as a self-prompting device for increasing<br />

multi-step task completi<strong>on</strong> by studens with<br />

moderate intellectual disabilities. 45(3), 422–<br />

439.<br />

239. Mechling, L. C., & Gast, D. L. (2003). Multimedia<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> to teach grocery word associati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<strong>and</strong> store locati<strong>on</strong>: A study of generalizati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

38(1), 62–76.<br />

Mele-McCarthy, J. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #159.<br />

240. Menchetti, B. M., & Garcia, L. A. (2003). Pers<strong>on</strong>al<br />

employment outcomes of pers<strong>on</strong>-centered<br />

career planning. 38(2), 145–156.<br />

Menendez, A. L. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #376.<br />

Menendez, A. L. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #224.<br />

241. Millar, D. S. (2003). Age of majority, transfer<br />

of rights <strong>and</strong> guardianship: C<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for families <strong>and</strong> educators. 38(4), 378–397.<br />

242. Millar, D. S. (2007). “I never put it together”:<br />

The disc<strong>on</strong>nect between self-determinati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> guardianship - implicati<strong>on</strong>s for practice.<br />

42(2), 119–129.<br />

632 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


243. Millar, D. S. (2008). Self-determinati<strong>on</strong> in relati<strong>on</strong><br />

to having or not having a legal guardian:<br />

Case studies of two school-aged young<br />

adults with developmental disabilities. 43(3),<br />

279–293.<br />

244. Millar, D. S. (2009). Comparis<strong>on</strong> of transiti<strong>on</strong>related<br />

IEP c<strong>on</strong>tent for young adults with disabilities<br />

who do or do not have a legal guardian.<br />

44(2), 151–167.<br />

Miller, S. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #87.<br />

245. Mims, P. J., Browder, D., Baker, J. N., Lee, A.,<br />

& Spo<strong>on</strong>er, F. (2009). Increasing comprehensi<strong>on</strong><br />

of students with significant intellectual<br />

disabilities <strong>and</strong> visual impairments during<br />

shared stories 44(3), 409–420.<br />

2<str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>. Miracle, S. A., Collins, B. C., Schuster, J. W.,<br />

& Grisham-Brown, J. (2001). Peer- verses<br />

teacher-delivered instructi<strong>on</strong>: Effects <strong>on</strong> acquisiti<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> maintenance. 36(4), 373–385.<br />

247. Moberg, S. (2003). Educati<strong>on</strong> for all in the<br />

North <strong>and</strong> the South: Teachers’ attitudes towards<br />

inclusive educati<strong>on</strong> in Finl<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Zambia. 38(4), 417–428.<br />

Moist, A. M. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #135.<br />

M<strong>on</strong>tgomery, J. W. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #301.<br />

M<strong>on</strong>tgomery, J. W. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #302.<br />

248. M<strong>on</strong>tgomery, J., McCrimm<strong>on</strong>, A. W.,<br />

Schwean, V. L., & Saklofske, D. H. (2010).<br />

Emoti<strong>on</strong>al intelligence in Asperger Syndrome:<br />

Implicati<strong>on</strong>s of diss<strong>on</strong>ance between<br />

intellect <strong>and</strong> affect. 45(4), 566–582.<br />

M<strong>on</strong>toya, L. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #386.<br />

Mo<strong>on</strong>, M. S. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #151.<br />

Mo<strong>on</strong>, S. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #260.<br />

249. Moreno, J., Aguilera, A., & Saldana, D.<br />

(2008). Do parents prefer special schools for<br />

their children with <strong>Autism</strong>? 43(2), 162–173.<br />

Morgan, R. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #116.<br />

Morgan, R. L. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #115.<br />

Morgan, R. L. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #158.<br />

Morin, D. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #81.<br />

Morin, D. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #82.<br />

Morin, P. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #140.<br />

Morse, T. E. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #130.<br />

250. Morse, T. E., & Schuster, J. W. (2004). Simultaneous<br />

prompting: A review of the literature.<br />

39(2), 153–168.<br />

251. Mostert, M. P. (2003). Meta-analyses in mental<br />

retardati<strong>on</strong>. 38(2), 229–249.<br />

Murdick, N. L. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #139.<br />

252. Murdick, N., Shore, P., Chittooran, M. M., &<br />

Gartin, B. (2004). Cross-cultural comparis<strong>on</strong><br />

of the c<strong>on</strong>cept of “otherness” <strong>and</strong> its impact<br />

<strong>on</strong> pers<strong>on</strong>s with disabilities. 39(4), 310–316.<br />

Murray, D. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #53.<br />

253. Mutua, N. K. (2001). Importance of parents’<br />

expectati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> beliefs in the educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

participati<strong>on</strong> of children with mental retardati<strong>on</strong><br />

in Kenya. 36(2), 148–159.<br />

Myles, B. S. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #128.<br />

254. Myles, B. S. Grossman, B. G., Aspy, R.,<br />

Henry, S. A., & Coffin, A. B. (2007). Planning<br />

a comprehensive program for students with<br />

autism spectrum disorders using evidencebased<br />

practices. 42(4), 398–409.<br />

255. Myles, B. S., Barnhill, G. P., Hagiwara, T.,<br />

Griswold, D. E., & Simps<strong>on</strong>, R. L. (2001). A<br />

synthesis of studies <strong>on</strong> the intellectual, academic,<br />

social/emoti<strong>on</strong>al <strong>and</strong> sensory characteristics<br />

of children <strong>and</strong> youth with Asperger<br />

syndrome. 36(3), 304–311.<br />

256. Myles, B. S., Hagiwara, T., Dunn, W., Rinner,<br />

L., Reese, M., Huggins, A., & Becker, S.<br />

(2004). Sensory issues in children with<br />

Asperger syndrome <strong>and</strong> autism. 39(4), 283–<br />

290.<br />

257. Myles, B. S., Huggins, A., Rome-Lake, M.,<br />

Hagiwara, T., Griswold, D. E., & Barnhill,<br />

G. P. (2003). Written language profile of children<br />

<strong>and</strong> youth with Asperger syndrome:<br />

From research to practice. 38(4), 362–369.<br />

258. Myles, B. S., Lee, H., Smith, S. M., Tien, K.,<br />

Chou, Y., & Huds<strong>on</strong>, J. (2007). A large-scale<br />

study of the characteristics of Asperger syndrome.<br />

42(4), 448–459.<br />

Nakaya, A. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #263.<br />

Nakken, H. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #359.<br />

Nefdt, N. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #300.<br />

Nels<strong>on</strong>, A. R. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #259.<br />

259. Nels<strong>on</strong>, C., McD<strong>on</strong>nell, A. P., Johnst<strong>on</strong>, S. S.,<br />

Crompt<strong>on</strong>, A., & Nels<strong>on</strong>, A. R. (2007). Keys to<br />

play: A strategy to increase the social interacti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

of young children with <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

their typically developing peers. 42(2), 165–<br />

181.<br />

Neubert, D. A. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #151.<br />

260. Neubert, D., Mo<strong>on</strong>, M. S., & Grigal, M. (2004).<br />

Activities of students with significant disabilities<br />

receiving services in postsec<strong>on</strong>dary settings.<br />

39(1), 16–25.<br />

Nguyen, D. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #201.<br />

Nieminen, P. K. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #212.<br />

Nissim, S. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #207.<br />

No<strong>on</strong>an, M. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #226.<br />

261. No<strong>on</strong>an, M., McCormick, L., & Heck, R. H.<br />

(2003). The co-teacher relati<strong>on</strong>ship scale:<br />

Applicati<strong>on</strong>s for professi<strong>on</strong>al development.<br />

38(1), 113–120.<br />

No<strong>on</strong>e, S. J. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #160.<br />

Norman, A. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #15.<br />

Noyes, D. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #73.<br />

Cumulative Author Index, Volumes 36–45 / 633


262. Noyes, D. A., & Sax, C. L. (2004). Changing<br />

systems for transiti<strong>on</strong>: Students, families, <strong>and</strong><br />

professi<strong>on</strong>als working together. 39(1), 35–<br />

44.<br />

O’Brien, E. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #230.<br />

O’Keeffe, M. K. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #192.<br />

O’Reilly, M. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #60.<br />

O’Reilly, M. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #197.<br />

O’Reilly, M. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #217.<br />

O’Reilly, M. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #198.<br />

O’Reilly, M. F. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #61.<br />

O’Reilly, M. F. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #218.<br />

Ogata, V. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #226.<br />

Ohtake, Y. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #264.<br />

263. Ohtake, Y., Wehmeyer, M., Uchida, N., Nakaya,<br />

A., & Yanagihara, M. (2010). Enabling a<br />

prelinguistic communicator with autism to<br />

use picture card as a strategy for repairing<br />

listener misunderst<strong>and</strong>ings: A case study.<br />

45(3), 410–421.<br />

264. Okada, S., Ohtake, Y., & Yanagihara, M.<br />

(2008). Effects of perspective sentences in<br />

Social Stories <strong>on</strong> improving the adaptive behaviors<br />

of students with autism spectrum disorders<br />

<strong>and</strong> related disabilities. 43(1), <str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>–60.<br />

265. Oliver, M. N., Anth<strong>on</strong>y, A., Leimkuhl, T. T., &<br />

Skillman, G. D. (2002). Attitudes toward acceptable<br />

socio-sexual behaviors for pers<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>: Implicati<strong>on</strong>s for normalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> community integrati<strong>on</strong>. 37(2),<br />

193–201.<br />

Openden, D. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #300.<br />

Ortega-Hurnd<strong>on</strong>, F. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #234.<br />

266. Osborn, J., Freeman, A., Burley, M., Wils<strong>on</strong>,<br />

R., J<strong>on</strong>es, E., & Rychener, S. (2007). Effect of<br />

tutoring <strong>on</strong> reading achievement for students<br />

with cognitive disabilities, specific learning<br />

disabilities, <strong>and</strong> students receiving Title I services.<br />

42(4), <str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>7–474.<br />

Ostrosky, M. M. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #123.<br />

Owen, F. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #342.<br />

267. Ozcan, N., & Cavkaytar, A. (2009). Parents as<br />

teachers: Teaching parents how to teach toilet<br />

skills to their children with autism <strong>and</strong><br />

mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. 44(2), 237–243.<br />

Özen, A. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #120.<br />

Özen, A. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #121.<br />

Özen, A. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #380.<br />

Palmer, S. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #4.<br />

Palmer, S. B. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #366.<br />

Parette, H. P. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #389.<br />

Parette, H. P. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #336.<br />

268. Parette, H. P., & Peters<strong>on</strong>-Karlan, G. R.<br />

(2007). Facilitating student achievement with<br />

assistive technology. 42(4), 387–397.<br />

269. Parette, H. P., Huer, M. B., & Brothers<strong>on</strong>, M.<br />

(2001). Related service pers<strong>on</strong>nel percepti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

of team AAC decisi<strong>on</strong>-making across<br />

cultures. 36(1), 69–82.<br />

270. Parette, H. P., St<strong>on</strong>er, J. B., & Watts, E. H.<br />

(2009). Assistive technology user group perspectives<br />

of early childhood professi<strong>on</strong>als.<br />

44(2), 257–270.<br />

271. Parette, H. P., Wojcik, B. W., Peters<strong>on</strong>-Karlan,<br />

G., & Hourcade, J. J. (2005). Assistive technology<br />

for students with mild disabilities:<br />

What’s cool <strong>and</strong> what’s not. 40(3), 320–331.<br />

Parette, Jr., H. P. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #19.<br />

Parette, P. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #166.<br />

Parette, P. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #313.<br />

Parette, P. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #167.<br />

272. Parette, P., & Scherer, M. (2004). Assistive<br />

technology use <strong>and</strong> stigma. 39(3), 217–226.<br />

273. Parette, P., Huer, M. B., & Hourcade, J. J.,<br />

(2003). Using assistive technology focus<br />

groups with families across cultures. 38(4),<br />

429–440.<br />

274. Parette, P., Meadan, H., Doubet, S., & Hess,<br />

J. (2010). Supporting families of young children<br />

with disabilities using technology. 45(4),<br />

552–565.<br />

275. Park, J., & Turnbull, A. P. (2001). Cross-cultural<br />

competency <strong>and</strong> special educati<strong>on</strong>:<br />

Percepti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> experiences of Korean parents<br />

of children with special needs. 36(2)<br />

133–147.<br />

Parker, D. R. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #159.<br />

276. Parker, M., & Schuster, J. W. (2002). Effectiveness<br />

of simultaneous prompting <strong>on</strong> the<br />

acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of observati<strong>on</strong>al <strong>and</strong> instructive<br />

feedback stimuli when teaching a heterogeneous<br />

group of high school students. 37(1),<br />

89–104.<br />

Parker, R. I. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #183.<br />

277. Pasnak, R., Maccubbin, E. M., Campbell,<br />

J. L., & Gadzichowski, M. (2004). Learning<br />

set instructi<strong>on</strong> in seriati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> the oddity principle<br />

for a child with severe mental disabilities.<br />

39(4), 337–345.<br />

Pasternack, R. H. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #159.<br />

Patt<strong>on</strong>, J. R. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #324.<br />

Patt<strong>on</strong>, J. R. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #325.<br />

Patt<strong>on</strong>, J. R. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #288.<br />

Patt<strong>on</strong>, J. R. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #287.<br />

278. Payne E. M., & Sitlingt<strong>on</strong>, P. L. (2008). Guardianship:<br />

Its role in the transiti<strong>on</strong> process for<br />

students with developmental disabilities.<br />

43(1), 3–19.<br />

279. Pearman, E., Elliott, T., & Aborn, L. (2004).<br />

Transiti<strong>on</strong> services model: Partnership for<br />

student success. 39(1), 26–34.<br />

Peled, I. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #124.<br />

634 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


Pèpin, M. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #214.<br />

Perner, D. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #280.<br />

Perner, D. E. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #389.<br />

Perner, D. E. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #391.<br />

Perner, D. E. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #390.<br />

Perras, C. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #313.<br />

Perras, C. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #388.<br />

Perras, C. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #389.<br />

Perras, C. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #391.<br />

Perras, C. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #390.<br />

280. Perras, C., Sharpe, V., Perner, D., & Zucker,<br />

S. H. (2003). Research to practice in cognitive<br />

disabilities/mental retardati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>Autism</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> related disabilities. 38(4), 355–357.<br />

Perringt<strong>on</strong>, C. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #113.<br />

Peters<strong>on</strong>-Karlan, G. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #271.<br />

Peters<strong>on</strong>-Karlan, G. R. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong><br />

#268.<br />

Petry, K. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #196.<br />

281. Petry, K., & Maes, B. (2007). Descripti<strong>on</strong> of<br />

the support needs of people with profound<br />

intellectual <strong>and</strong> multiple disabilities using the<br />

2002 AAMR system: An overview of literature.<br />

42(2), 130–143.<br />

282. Petursdottir, A. I., Esch, J. W., Sautter, R. A.,<br />

& Stewart, K. K. (2010). Characteristics <strong>and</strong><br />

hypothesized functi<strong>on</strong>s of challenging behavior<br />

in a community-based sample. 45(1),<br />

81–93.<br />

283. Petursdottir, A., & Sigurdardottir, Z. G.,<br />

(2006). Increasing the skills of children with<br />

developmental disabilities through staff training<br />

in behavioral teaching techniques. 41(3),<br />

264–279.<br />

Pierce, T. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #87.<br />

284. Piers<strong>on</strong>, M. R., & Glaeser, B. C. (2005). Extensi<strong>on</strong><br />

of research <strong>on</strong> social skills training<br />

using comic strip c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>s to students<br />

without <strong>Autism</strong>. 40(3), 279–284.<br />

285. Piers<strong>on</strong>, M. R., & Glaeser, B. C. (2007). Using<br />

comic strip c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>s to increase social<br />

satisfacti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> decrease l<strong>on</strong>eliness in students<br />

with autism spectrum disorder. 42(4),<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>0–<str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>6.<br />

286. Pino, O., & Dazzi, C. (2005). Evaluati<strong>on</strong> of<br />

c<strong>on</strong>textual variability in the predicti<strong>on</strong> of reinforcer<br />

effectiveness. 40(2), 117–130.<br />

Pollard, E. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #72.<br />

Polloway, E. A. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #324.<br />

Polloway, E. A. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #325.<br />

287. Polloway, E. A., Lubin, J., Smith, J. D., &<br />

Patt<strong>on</strong>, J. R. (2010). Mild intellectual disabilities:<br />

Legacies <strong>and</strong> trends in c<strong>on</strong>cepts <strong>and</strong><br />

educati<strong>on</strong>al practices. 45(1), 54–68.<br />

288. Polloway, E. A., Patt<strong>on</strong>, J. R., Smith, J. D.,<br />

Lubin, J., & Antoine, K. (2009). State guidelines<br />

for mental retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> developmental<br />

disabilities: A revisitati<strong>on</strong> of previous analyses<br />

in light of changes in the field. 44(1),<br />

14–24.<br />

Polychr<strong>on</strong>is, S. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #227.<br />

Porretta, D. L. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #57.<br />

289. Post, M., & Storey, K. (2002). Review of using<br />

auditory prompting systems with pers<strong>on</strong>s<br />

who have moderate to severe disabilities.<br />

37(3), 317–327.<br />

Post<strong>on</strong>, AJ. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #203.<br />

Post<strong>on</strong>, D. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #203.<br />

290. Post<strong>on</strong>, D. J., & Turnbull, A. P. (2004). Role of<br />

spirituality <strong>and</strong> religi<strong>on</strong> in family quality of life<br />

for families of children with disabilities. 39(2),<br />

95–108.<br />

Prater, M. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #106.<br />

Prater, M. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #108.<br />

Prater, M. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #205.<br />

Prater, M. A. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #107.<br />

Presley, J. A. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #170.<br />

Pridgen, L. S. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #237.<br />

Prochnow, J. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #33.<br />

291. Prupas, A., & Reid, G. (2001). Effects of exercise<br />

frequency <strong>on</strong> the stereotypic behaviors<br />

of children with developmental disabilities.<br />

36(2), 196–206.<br />

Pulos, S. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #141.<br />

Pumpian, I. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #73.<br />

Pyfer, J. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #298.<br />

Ramasamy, R. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #189.<br />

292. Rao, S., & Kane, M. T. (2009).Teaching students<br />

with cognitive impairment chained<br />

mathematical task of decimal subtracti<strong>on</strong> using<br />

simultaneous prompting. 44(2), 244–256.<br />

293. Rao, S., & Mallow, L. (2009). Using simultaneous<br />

prompting procedure to promote recall<br />

of multiplicati<strong>on</strong> facts by middle school<br />

students with cognitive impairment. 44(1),<br />

80–90.<br />

Rau, K. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #104.<br />

Reese, M. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #256.<br />

Regester, A. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #197.<br />

Reid, G. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #291.<br />

Reilly, A. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #59.<br />

Reiter, S. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #360.<br />

Renzaglia, A. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #110.<br />

Richter, S. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #362.<br />

Riesen, T. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #227.<br />

Riesen, T. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #228.<br />

Rinner, L. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #256.<br />

Cumulative Author Index, Volumes 36–45 / 635


Rispoli, M. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #197.<br />

Rispoli, M. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #217.<br />

Rispoli, M. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #198.<br />

Rispoli, M. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #218.<br />

Roberts, K. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #8.<br />

Roberts, K. D. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #318.<br />

Robins<strong>on</strong>, S. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #300.<br />

Rodi, M. S. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #170.<br />

Rogan, P. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #349.<br />

294. Roll-Petterss<strong>on</strong>, L. (2001). Teacher percepti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

of supports <strong>and</strong> resources needed in<br />

regard to pupils with special educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

needs in Sweden. 36(1), 42–54.<br />

295. Roll-Petterss<strong>on</strong>, L. (2008). Teacher’s perceived<br />

efficacy <strong>and</strong> the inclusi<strong>on</strong> of a pupil<br />

with dyslexia or mild mental retardati<strong>on</strong>: Findings<br />

from Sweden. 43(2), 174–185.<br />

Romaniak, B. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #24.<br />

Rome-Lake, M. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #257.<br />

Rosenberg, H. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #50.<br />

Rosenberg, H. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #25.<br />

Ross, D. E. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #189.<br />

296. Ross, D. E. (2002). Replacing faulty c<strong>on</strong>versati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

exchanges for children with autism<br />

by establishing a functi<strong>on</strong>ally equivalent alternative<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se. 37(4), 343–362.<br />

297. Ross, D. E., Singer-Dudek, J., & Greer, R. D.<br />

(2005). Teacher performance rate <strong>and</strong> accuracy<br />

scale: Training as evaluati<strong>on</strong>. 40(4),<br />

411–423.<br />

298. Roth, K., Pyfer, J. & Huettig, C. (2007). Transiti<strong>on</strong><br />

in physical recreati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> students with<br />

cognitive disabilities: Graduate <strong>and</strong> parent<br />

perspectives. 42(1), 94–106.<br />

Roth, T. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #161.<br />

299. Ruef, M., & Turnbull, A. P. (2001). Stakeholder<br />

opini<strong>on</strong>s <strong>on</strong> accessible informati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

products helpful in building positive, practical<br />

soluti<strong>on</strong>s to behavioral challenges of individuals<br />

with mental retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong>/or autism.<br />

36(4), 441–456.<br />

300. Ruef, M., Nefdt, N., Openden, D., Elmensdorp,<br />

S., Harris, K. C., & Robins<strong>on</strong>, S. (2009).<br />

Learning by doing: A collaborative model for<br />

training teacher-c<strong>and</strong>idate graduate students<br />

in <strong>Autism</strong>. 44(3), 343–355.<br />

Ruijssenaars, W. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #341.<br />

Rung, L. L. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #171.<br />

Russell, D. L. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #109.<br />

Rychener, S. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #266.<br />

301. Ryndak, D. L., Ward, T., Alper, S., M<strong>on</strong>tgomery,<br />

J. W., & Storch, J. F. (2010). L<strong>on</strong>g-term<br />

outcomes of services for two pers<strong>on</strong>s with<br />

significant disabilities with differing educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

experiences: A qualitative c<strong>on</strong>sider-<br />

ati<strong>on</strong> of the impact of educati<strong>on</strong>al experiences.<br />

45(3), 323–338.<br />

302. Ryndak, D. L., Ward, T., Alper, S., Storch,<br />

J. F., & M<strong>on</strong>tgomery, J. W. (2010). L<strong>on</strong>g-term<br />

outcomes of services in inclusive <strong>and</strong> selfc<strong>on</strong>tained<br />

settings for siblings with comparable<br />

significant disabilities. 45(1), 38–53.<br />

Saklofske, D. H. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #248.<br />

Saldana, D. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #249.<br />

Sales, C. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #342.<br />

303. Saloviita, T., & Lehtinen, U. (2001). Paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

staff teaching adults with mental<br />

retardati<strong>on</strong>. 36(1), 103–106.<br />

Salzberg, C. L. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #115.<br />

Salzberg, C. L. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #116.<br />

304. S<strong>and</strong>ies<strong>on</strong>, R. (2006). Pathfinding in the research<br />

forest: The pearl harvesting method<br />

for effective informati<strong>on</strong> retrieval. 41(4), 401–<br />

409.<br />

Sautter, R. A. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #282.<br />

Sax, C. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #73.<br />

Sax, C. L. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #262.<br />

305. Schaefer Whitby, P. J., & Mancil, G. R.<br />

(2009). Academic achievement profiles of<br />

children with high functi<strong>on</strong>ing autism <strong>and</strong><br />

Asperger Syndrome: A review of the literature.<br />

44(4), 551–560.<br />

Schaller, J. L. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #377.<br />

Scherer, M. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #272.<br />

306. Schlosser, R. W., Walker, E., & Sigafoos, J.<br />

(2006). Increasing opportunities for requesting<br />

in children with developmental disabilities<br />

residing in group homes through pyramidal<br />

training. 41(3), 244–252.<br />

Schuster, J. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #130.<br />

Schuster, J. W. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #276.<br />

Schuster, J. W. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #344.<br />

Schuster, J. W. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #2<str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>.<br />

Schuster, J. W. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #250.<br />

Schuster, J. W. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #148.<br />

Schuster, J. W. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #165.<br />

Schuster, J. W. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #188.<br />

Schuster, J. W. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #145.<br />

Schuster, J. W. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #176.<br />

307. Schwartzman, L. J. V., Vause, T., Martin,<br />

G. L., Yu, C. T., Campbell, L., Danbrook, M.,<br />

& Feldman, M. (2009). Predicting the learning<br />

ability of children with autism: The assessment<br />

of basic learning abilities test versus<br />

parents’ predicti<strong>on</strong>s. 44(2), 271–279.<br />

308. Schwartzman, L., Martin, G. L., Yu, C. T., &<br />

Whiteley, J. (2004). Choice, degree of preference,<br />

<strong>and</strong> happiness indices with pers<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with intellectual disabilities: A surprising finding.<br />

39(3), 265–269.<br />

636 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


Schwean, V. L. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #248.<br />

Scott, J. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #55.<br />

Scott, M. S. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #132.<br />

309. Scott, M. S., & Delgado, C. F. (2006). Predicting<br />

poor achievement in early grade school<br />

using kindergarten scores <strong>on</strong> simple cognitive<br />

tasks. 41(1), 37–47.<br />

310. Scott, M. S., & Fletcher, K. L. (2001). Classificati<strong>on</strong><br />

of three-year-old children with <strong>and</strong><br />

without mild learning problems: A cognitive<br />

differentiati<strong>on</strong>. 36(4), 401–410.<br />

311. Scott, M. S., Delgado, C. F., Tu, S., &<br />

Fletcher, K. L. (2005). Selecting <strong>and</strong> validating<br />

tasks from a kindergarten screening battery<br />

that best predict third grade educati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

placement. 40(4), 377–389.<br />

312. Scott, M. S., Tu, S., & Fletcher, K. L. (2003).<br />

Cross validating a new preschool screening<br />

test. 38(2), 167–178.<br />

Scotti, J. R. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #144.<br />

Scruggs, T. E. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #147.<br />

Seid, N. H. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #238.<br />

Sh<strong>and</strong>s, E. I. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #26.<br />

Sharpe, V. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #280.<br />

313. Sharpe, V., Perras, C., Parette, P., & Zucker,<br />

S. H. (2001). Many roads, different directi<strong>on</strong>s:<br />

Strategies for success. 36(3), 227–229.<br />

Shavit, P. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #162.<br />

314. Shinde, S. K., & Sym<strong>on</strong>s, F. (2007). Educator<br />

percepti<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> ratings of pain in school-age<br />

children with mental retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> developmental<br />

disabilities. 42(2), 224–229.<br />

Shippen, M. E. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #175.<br />

Shippen, M. E. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #49.<br />

Shogren, K. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #197.<br />

Shogren, K. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #217.<br />

Shogren, K. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #198.<br />

Shogren, K. A. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #202.<br />

Shogren, K. A. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #370.<br />

315. Shokoohi-Yekta, M., & Hendricks<strong>on</strong>, J.<br />

(2010). Friendships with peers with severe<br />

disabilities: American <strong>and</strong> Iranian sec<strong>on</strong>dary<br />

students’ ideas about being a friend. 45(1),<br />

23–37.<br />

Shore, P. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #252.<br />

316. Siegel, E., & Allinder, R. M. (2005). Review of<br />

assessment procedures for students with<br />

moderate <strong>and</strong> severe disabilities. 40(4), 343–<br />

351.<br />

Sigafoos, J. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #138.<br />

Sigafoos, J. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #306.<br />

Sigafoos, J. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #60.<br />

Sigafoos, J. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #61.<br />

Sigafoos, J. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #197.<br />

Sigafoos, J. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #217.<br />

Sigafoos, J. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #198.<br />

Sigurdardottir, Z. G. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #283.<br />

317. Simps<strong>on</strong>, A., Lang<strong>on</strong>e, J., & Ayres, K. M.<br />

(2004). Embedded video <strong>and</strong> computer<br />

based instructi<strong>on</strong> to improve social skills for<br />

students with autism. 39(3), 240–252.<br />

Simps<strong>on</strong>, R. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #370.<br />

Simps<strong>on</strong>, R. L. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #255.<br />

Sinclair, T. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #3.<br />

Singer-Dudek, J. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #297.<br />

Singh, H. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #332.<br />

Sitlingt<strong>on</strong>, P. L. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #278.<br />

Skillman, G. D. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #265.<br />

318. Skouge, J. R., Kelly, M. L., Roberts, K. D.,<br />

Leake, D. W., & Stodden, R. A. (2007). Technologies<br />

for self-determinati<strong>on</strong> for youth with<br />

developmental disabilities. 42(4), 475–482.<br />

Slocum, T. A. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #154.<br />

319. Smail, K., & Horvat, M. (2006). Relati<strong>on</strong>ship of<br />

muscular strength <strong>on</strong> work performance in<br />

high school students with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

41(4), 410–419.<br />

Smalley, K. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #73.<br />

320. Smidt, M. L., & Cress, C. J. (2004). Mastery<br />

behaviors during social <strong>and</strong> object play in<br />

toddlers with physical impairment. 39(2),<br />

141–152.<br />

321. Smith, J. D. (2003). Ab<strong>and</strong><strong>on</strong>ing the myth of<br />

mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. 38(4), 358–361.<br />

322. Smith, J. D. (2007). Mental retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

the problem of “Normality”: Self-determinati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> identity choice. 42(4), 410–417.<br />

Smith, J. D. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #324.<br />

Smith, J. D. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #288.<br />

Smith, J. D. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #32.<br />

Smith, J. D. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #391.<br />

Smith, J. D. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #287.<br />

323. Smith, S. E., & Cascella, P. W. (2007). Ratings<br />

of communicati<strong>on</strong> competence by siblings of<br />

pers<strong>on</strong>s with Down Syndrome. 42(2), 182–<br />

189.<br />

Smith, S. M. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #258.<br />

Smith, T. E.C. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #370.<br />

324. Smith, T. L., Polloway, E. A., Smith, J. D., &<br />

Patt<strong>on</strong>, J. R. (2007). Self-determinati<strong>on</strong> for<br />

pers<strong>on</strong>s with developmental disabilities: Ethical<br />

c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong>s for teachers. 42(2), 144–<br />

151.<br />

325. Smith, T., Polloway, E. A., Patt<strong>on</strong>, J. R., &<br />

Beyer, J. F. (2008). Individuals with intellectual<br />

<strong>and</strong> developmental disabilities in the<br />

criminal justice system <strong>and</strong> implicati<strong>on</strong>s for<br />

transiti<strong>on</strong> planning. 43(4), 421–430.<br />

Sokolowski, B. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #24.<br />

Cumulative Author Index, Volumes 36–45 / 637


Sparks, S. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #87.<br />

326. Spencer, V. G., & Balb<strong>on</strong>i, G. (2003). Can<br />

students with mental retardati<strong>on</strong> teach their<br />

peers? 38(1), 32–61.<br />

Spevack, S. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #381.<br />

Spillane, A. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #363.<br />

Spo<strong>on</strong>er, F. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #29.<br />

Spo<strong>on</strong>er, F. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #245.<br />

Spo<strong>on</strong>er, F. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #88.<br />

327. Spreat, S., & C<strong>on</strong>roy, J. W. (2003). Analysis of<br />

mental retardati<strong>on</strong> service provisi<strong>on</strong> for class<br />

members <strong>and</strong> n<strong>on</strong>-class members. 38(1),<br />

121–127.<br />

328. Spriggs, A., Gast, D. L., & Ayres, K. M.<br />

(2007). Using picture activity schedule books<br />

to increase <strong>on</strong>-schedule <strong>and</strong> <strong>on</strong>-task behaviors.<br />

42(2), 209–223.<br />

329. Stafford, A. M., Alberto, P. A., Fredrick L. D.,<br />

Heflin, L. J., & Heller, K. W. (2002). Preference<br />

variability <strong>and</strong> the instructi<strong>on</strong> of choice<br />

making with students with severe intellectual<br />

disabilities. 37(1), 70–88.<br />

330. Stanish, H. I., & Aucoin, M. (2007). Usefulness<br />

of a perceived exerti<strong>on</strong> scale for m<strong>on</strong>itoring<br />

exercise intensity in adults with intellectual<br />

disabilities. 42(2), 230–251.<br />

331. Starr, E. M., Foy, J. B., & Cramer, K. M.<br />

(2001). Parental percepti<strong>on</strong>s of the educati<strong>on</strong><br />

of children with pervasive developmental disorders.<br />

36(1), 55–68.<br />

332. Starr, E. M., Foy, J. B., Cramer, K. M., Singh,<br />

H. (2006). How are schools doing? Parental<br />

percepti<strong>on</strong>s of children with autism spectrum<br />

disorders, Down syndrome, <strong>and</strong> learning disabilities:<br />

A comparative analysis. 41(4), 315–<br />

332.<br />

Stecker, P. M. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #384.<br />

Stephens, E. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #235.<br />

333. Stephens<strong>on</strong>, J. (2006). Music therapy <strong>and</strong> the<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> of students with severe disabilities.<br />

41(3), 290–299.<br />

Stewart, K. K. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #282.<br />

Stiles, N. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #54.<br />

Stock, S. E. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #91.<br />

Stock, S. E. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #92.<br />

Stock, S. E. (2003a). See citati<strong>on</strong> #93.<br />

Stock, S. E. (2003b). See citati<strong>on</strong> #94.<br />

Stodden, R. A. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #318.<br />

334. Stodden, R. A., & Whelley, T. (2004). Postsec<strong>on</strong>dary<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> youth with intellectual<br />

disabilities: An introducti<strong>on</strong>. 39(1), 6–15.<br />

335. Stodden, R. A., & Zucker, S. H. (2004). Transiti<strong>on</strong><br />

of youth with disabilities to postsec<strong>on</strong>dary<br />

educati<strong>on</strong>. 39(1), 3–5.<br />

St<strong>on</strong>er, J. B. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #33.<br />

St<strong>on</strong>er, J. B. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #19.<br />

St<strong>on</strong>er, J. B. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #23.<br />

St<strong>on</strong>er, J. B. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #270.<br />

336. St<strong>on</strong>er, J. B., Parette, H. P., Watts, E. H.,<br />

Wojcik, B. W., & Fogal, T. (2008). Preschool<br />

teacher percepti<strong>on</strong>s of assistive technology<br />

<strong>and</strong> professi<strong>on</strong>al development resp<strong>on</strong>ses.<br />

43(1), 77–91.<br />

St<strong>on</strong>er, Karen. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #342.<br />

Storch, J. F. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #301.<br />

Storch, J. F. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #302.<br />

Storey, K. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #289.<br />

Strickett, T. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #155.<br />

Str<strong>on</strong>g, M. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #171.<br />

Summers, J. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #28.<br />

Summers, J. A. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #137.<br />

Surinak, T. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #25.<br />

Sutter, M. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #47.<br />

Sym<strong>on</strong>s, F. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #314.<br />

Szwed, K. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #43.<br />

Taber-Doughty, T. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #43.<br />

337. Tachibana, T. (2005). Attitudes of Japanese<br />

adults towards pers<strong>on</strong>s with intellectual disability:<br />

An exploratory analysis of openended<br />

descripti<strong>on</strong>s of resp<strong>on</strong>dents’ experiences<br />

<strong>and</strong> opini<strong>on</strong>s. 40(4), 352–359.<br />

338. Tachibana, T. (2006). Attitudes of Japanese<br />

adults towards pers<strong>on</strong>s with intellectual disability:<br />

Effect of percepti<strong>on</strong>s c<strong>on</strong>cerning intellectual<br />

disability. 41(1), 58–69.<br />

339. Tachibana, T., & Watanabe, K. (2004a). Attitudes<br />

of Japanese adults toward pers<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with intellectual disability: Relati<strong>on</strong>ship between<br />

attitudes <strong>and</strong> demographic variables.<br />

39(2), 109–128.<br />

340. Tachibana, T., & Watanabe, K. (2004b). Attitudes<br />

of Japanese adults toward pers<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with intellectual disability: Comparis<strong>on</strong>s over<br />

time <strong>and</strong> across countries. 39(3), 227–239.<br />

341. Tadema, A. C., Vlaskamp, C., & Ruijssenaars,<br />

W. (2008). Implementati<strong>on</strong> of a programme<br />

for students with profound intellectual<br />

<strong>and</strong> multiple disabilities in schools: Three<br />

case studies. 43(4), 529–540.<br />

T<strong>and</strong>y, R. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #87.<br />

Tanverdi, A. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #113.<br />

342. Tardif-Williams, C. Y., Owen, F., Feldman, M.,<br />

Tarulli, D., Griffiths, D., Sales, C., McQueen-<br />

Fuentes, G., & St<strong>on</strong>er, K. (2007). Comparis<strong>on</strong><br />

of interactive computer-based <strong>and</strong> classroom-based<br />

training <strong>on</strong> human rights awareness<br />

in pers<strong>on</strong>s with intellectual disabilities.<br />

42(1), 48–58.<br />

Targett, P. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #364.<br />

Tarulli, D. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #342.<br />

638 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


343. Taunt, H. M., & Hastings, R. P. (2002). Positive<br />

impact of children with developmental<br />

disabilities <strong>on</strong> their families: A preliminary<br />

study. 37(4), 410–420.<br />

344. Taylor, P., Collins, B. C., Schuster, J. W., &<br />

Kleinert, H. (2002). Teaching laundry skills to<br />

high school students with disabilities: Generalizati<strong>on</strong><br />

of targeted skills <strong>and</strong> n<strong>on</strong>targeted<br />

informati<strong>on</strong>. 37(2), 172–183.<br />

Taylor, R. L. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #189.<br />

Taylor, R. L. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #62.<br />

Taylor, R. L. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #182.<br />

345. Tekin, E., & Kircaali-Iftar, G. (2002). Comparis<strong>on</strong><br />

of the effectiveness <strong>and</strong> efficiency of two<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>se prompting procedures delivered<br />

by sibling tutors. 37(3), 283–299.<br />

3<str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>. Tekin-Iftar, E. (2003). Effectiveness of peer<br />

delivered simultaneous prompting <strong>on</strong> teaching<br />

community signs to students with developmental<br />

disabilities. 38(1), 77–94.<br />

Tekin-Iftar, E. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #122.<br />

Tekin-Iftar, E. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #153.<br />

347. Tekin-Iftar, E. (2008). Parent-delivered community-based<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> with simultaneous<br />

prompting for teaching community skills to<br />

children with developmental disabilities.<br />

43(2), 249–265.<br />

Tekin-Iftar, E. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #125.<br />

Test, D. W. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #190.<br />

Test, D. W. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #134.<br />

Test, D. W. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #362.<br />

Theoharis, R. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #202.<br />

348. Thoma, C. A., & Getzel, E. E. (2005). “Selfdeterminati<strong>on</strong><br />

is what it’s all about”: What<br />

post-sec<strong>on</strong>dary students with disabilities tell<br />

us are important c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong>s for success.<br />

40(3), 234–242.<br />

349. Thoma, C. A., Rogan, P., & Baker, S. R.<br />

(2001). Student involvement in transiti<strong>on</strong><br />

planning: Unheard voices. 36(1), 16–29.<br />

Thomas, R. A. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #375.<br />

Thomps<strong>on</strong>, J. R. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #24.<br />

Thomps<strong>on</strong>, J. R. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #139.<br />

Thomps<strong>on</strong>, J. R. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #229.<br />

350. Thoms<strong>on</strong>, K. M., Czarnecki, D., Martin, T. L.,<br />

Yu, C. T., & Martin, G. L. (2007). Predicting<br />

optimal preference assessment methods for<br />

individuals with developmental disabilities.<br />

42(1), 107–118.<br />

Thorsteinss<strong>on</strong>, J. R. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #201.<br />

Tien, K. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #258.<br />

351. Tien, K. (2008). Effectiveness of the picture<br />

exchange communicati<strong>on</strong> system as a functi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

communicati<strong>on</strong> interventi<strong>on</strong> for individuals<br />

with <strong>Autism</strong> Specturm Disorders: A practice-based<br />

research synthesis. 43(1), 61–76.<br />

Tincani, M. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #353.<br />

352. Tincani, M., Crozier, S., & Alazetta, L. (2006).<br />

The picture exchange communicati<strong>on</strong> system:<br />

Effects <strong>on</strong> m<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>and</strong> speech development<br />

for school-aged children with <strong>Autism</strong>.<br />

41(2), 177–184.<br />

353. Travers, J., & Tincani, M. (2010). Sexuality<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> for individuals with autism spectrum<br />

disorders: Critical issues & decisi<strong>on</strong><br />

making guidelines. 45(2), 284–293.<br />

Trela, K. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #190.<br />

Tsai, S. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #377.<br />

Tu, S. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #213.<br />

Tu, S. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #311.<br />

Turan, Y. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #121.<br />

Turan, Y. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #123.<br />

Turnbull, A. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #56.<br />

Turnbull, A. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #28.<br />

Turnbull, A. P. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #275.<br />

Turnbull, A. P. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #299.<br />

Turnbull, A. P. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #136.<br />

Turnbull, A. P. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #290.<br />

Turnbull, A. P. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #137.<br />

Uchida, N. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #263.<br />

354. Umbarger, G. (2007). State of evidence regarding<br />

complimentary <strong>and</strong> alternative medical<br />

treatments for autism spectrum disorders.<br />

42(4), 437–447.<br />

Umbreit, J. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #31.<br />

Umbreit, J. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #355.<br />

355. Underwood, M., Umbreit, J., & Liaupsin, C.<br />

(2009). Efficacy of a systematic process for<br />

designing functi<strong>on</strong>-based interventi<strong>on</strong>s for<br />

adults in a community setting. 44(1), 25–38.<br />

Uphold, N. M. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #362.<br />

356. Uysal, A., & Ergenek<strong>on</strong>, Y. (2010). Social<br />

skills instructi<strong>on</strong> carried out by teachers<br />

working at private special educati<strong>on</strong> instituti<strong>on</strong>s.<br />

45(3), 459–<str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>6.<br />

van der Walt, K. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #149.<br />

Van Hove, G. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #51.<br />

357. Van Laarhoven T., & Van Laarhoven-Myers,<br />

T. (2006). Comparis<strong>on</strong> of three video-based<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>al procedures for teaching daily<br />

living skills to pers<strong>on</strong>s with developmental<br />

disabilities. 41(4), 365–383.<br />

358. Van Laarhoven, T., Zurita, L. M., Johns<strong>on</strong>,<br />

J. W., Grider, K. M., & Grider, K. L. (2009).<br />

Comparis<strong>on</strong> of self, other, <strong>and</strong> subjective<br />

video models for teaching daily living skills to<br />

individuals with developmental disabilities.<br />

44(4), 509–522.<br />

Van Laarhoven-Myers, T. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong><br />

#357.<br />

Vause, T. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #307.<br />

Cumulative Author Index, Volumes 36–45 / 639


Vlaskamp, C. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #341.<br />

359. Vlaskamp, C., & Nakken, H. (2008). Therapeutic<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>s in the support of people<br />

with profound intellectual <strong>and</strong> multiple disabilities.<br />

43(3), 334–341.<br />

Vlaskamp, C. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #163.<br />

360. Vogel, G., & Reiter, S. (2003). Spiritual dimensi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

of bar/bat mitzvah cerem<strong>on</strong>ies for Jewish<br />

children with developmental disabilities.<br />

38(3), 314–322.<br />

Vogt, W. P. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #24.<br />

Wade, H. A. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #73.<br />

Wagner, S. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #100.<br />

Wahl, M. R. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #47.<br />

Wakeman, S. Y. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #178.<br />

361. Walberg, J. L., & Craig-Unkefer, L. A. (2010).<br />

An examinati<strong>on</strong> of the effects of a social communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the play behaviors<br />

of children with autism spectrum disorder.<br />

45(1), 69–80.<br />

Walker, A. R. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #134.<br />

362. Walker, A., Uphold, N. M., Richter, S., & Test,<br />

D. W. (2010). Review of the literature <strong>on</strong> community-based<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> across grade levels.<br />

45(2), 242–267.<br />

Walker, E. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #306.<br />

Wang, M. H. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #377.<br />

363. Wang, P., & Spillane, A. (2009). Evidencebased<br />

social skills interventi<strong>on</strong>s for children<br />

with autism: A meta-analysis. 44(3), 318–<br />

342.<br />

Ward, L. W. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #83.<br />

Ward, T. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #301.<br />

Ward, T. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #302.<br />

Watanabe, K. (2004a). See citati<strong>on</strong> #339.<br />

Watanabe, K. (2004b). See citati<strong>on</strong> #340.<br />

Watts, E. H. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #336.<br />

Watts, E. H. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #270.<br />

Wechsler, J. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #73.<br />

364. Wehman, P., & Targett, P. (2002). Supported<br />

employment: Challenges of new staff recruitment,<br />

selecti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> retenti<strong>on</strong>. 37(4), 434–<br />

4<str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>.<br />

Wehmeyer, M. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> 2.<br />

Wehmeyer, M. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #3.<br />

Wehmeyer, M. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #263.<br />

Wehmeyer, M. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #4.<br />

Wehmeyer, M. L. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #170.<br />

Wehmeyer, M. L. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #211.<br />

Wehmeyer, M. L. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #91.<br />

Wehmeyer, M. L. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #92.<br />

365. Wehmeyer, M. L. (2003). Defining mental retardati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> ensuring access to the general<br />

curriculum. 38(3), 271–282.<br />

Wehmeyer, M. L. (2003a). See citati<strong>on</strong> #93.<br />

Wehmeyer, M. L. (2003b). See citati<strong>on</strong> #94.<br />

Wehmeyer, M. L. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #136.<br />

Wehmeyer, M. L. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #202.<br />

366. Wehmeyer, M. L., & Palmer, S. B. (2003).<br />

Adult outcomes for students with cognitive<br />

disabilities three years after high school: The<br />

impact of self-determinati<strong>on</strong>. 38(2), 131–144.<br />

367. Wehmeyer, M. L., Garner, N., Yeager, D.,<br />

Lawrence, M., & Davis, A. K. (2006). Infusing<br />

self-determinati<strong>on</strong> into 18–21 services for<br />

students with intellectual or developmental<br />

disabilities: A multi-stage, multiple comp<strong>on</strong>ent<br />

model. 41(1), 3–13.<br />

368. Wehmeyer, M. L., Lance, G. D., & Bashinski,<br />

S. (2002). Promoting access to the general<br />

curriculum for students with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>:<br />

A multi-level model. 37(3), 223–234.<br />

369. Wehmeyer, M. L., Lattin, D., & Agran, M.<br />

(2001). Achieving access to the general curriculum<br />

for students with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>:<br />

A curriculum decisi<strong>on</strong>-making model. 36(4),<br />

327–342.<br />

370. Wehmeyer, M. L., Shogren, K. A., Zager, D.,<br />

Smith, T. E. C., & Simps<strong>on</strong>, R. (2010). Research-based<br />

principles <strong>and</strong> practices for<br />

educating students with <strong>Autism</strong>: Self-determinati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> social interacti<strong>on</strong>s. 45(4), 475–<br />

486.<br />

371. Weir, C. (2004). Pers<strong>on</strong>-centered <strong>and</strong> collaborative<br />

supports for college success. 39(1),<br />

67–73.<br />

Weiss, I. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #208.<br />

372. Werts, M., Zigm<strong>on</strong>d, N., & Leeper, D. C.<br />

(2001). Paraprofessi<strong>on</strong>al proximity <strong>and</strong> academic<br />

engagement: Students with disabilities<br />

in primary aged classrooms. 36(4), 424–<br />

440.<br />

373. West, E. A., & Billingsley, F. (2005). Improving<br />

the system of least prompts: A comparis<strong>on</strong><br />

of procedural variati<strong>on</strong>. (2005). 40(2),<br />

131–144.<br />

374. Whal<strong>on</strong>, K., & Hanline, M. F. (2008). Effects of<br />

a reciprocal interventi<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> the questi<strong>on</strong> generati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> resp<strong>on</strong>ding of children with autism<br />

spectrum disorder. 43(3), 367–387.<br />

Whatley, A. D. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #156.<br />

Wheeler, J. J. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #68.<br />

Wheeler, J. J. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #69.<br />

Wheeler, J. J. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #224.<br />

Wheeler, J. J. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #387.<br />

375. Wheeler, J. J., Carter, S. L., Mayt<strong>on</strong>, M. R., &<br />

Thomas, R. A. (2002). Structural analysis of<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong>al variables <strong>and</strong> their effects <strong>on</strong><br />

640 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


task-engagement <strong>and</strong> self-aggressi<strong>on</strong>. 37(4),<br />

391–398.<br />

376. Wheeler, J., Mayt<strong>on</strong>, M. R., Carter, S. L.,<br />

Chitiyo, M., Menendez, A. L., & Huang, A.<br />

(2009). An assessment of treatment integrity<br />

in behavioral interventi<strong>on</strong> studies c<strong>on</strong>ducted<br />

with pers<strong>on</strong>s with mental retardati<strong>on</strong>. 44(2),<br />

187–195.<br />

Whelley, T. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #334.<br />

White, R. B. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #194.<br />

Whiteley, J. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #308.<br />

Wiersma, L. A. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #163.<br />

Williams, T. O., Jr. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #111.<br />

Wils<strong>on</strong>, R. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #266.<br />

Wojcik, B. W. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #271.<br />

Wojcik, B. W. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #336.<br />

Wolfe, P. S. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #225.<br />

Wood, W. M. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #134.<br />

Woodruff, N. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #181.<br />

Woods-Groves, S. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #111.<br />

Wright, E. H. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #212.<br />

Wright, R. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #79.<br />

Yanagihara, M. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #264.<br />

Yanagihara, M. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #263.<br />

Yanardag˘ , M. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #379.<br />

Yanardag˘ , M. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #380.<br />

377. Yang, N. K., Schaller, J. L., Huang, T., Wang,<br />

M. H., & Tsai, S. (2003). Enhancing appropriate<br />

social behaviors for children with <strong>Autism</strong><br />

in general educati<strong>on</strong> classrooms: An analysis<br />

of six cases. 38(4), 405–416.<br />

Yang, Y. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #187.<br />

Yeager, D. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #367.<br />

378. Yılmaz, I., Birkan, B., K<strong>on</strong>ukman, F., & Erkan,<br />

M. (2005). Using a c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay procedure<br />

to teach aquatic play skills to children<br />

with autism. 40(2), 171–182.<br />

379. Yılmaz, I˙., K<strong>on</strong>ukman, F., Birkan, B., &<br />

Yanardag˘ , M. (2010). Effects of most to least<br />

prompting <strong>on</strong> teaching simple progressi<strong>on</strong><br />

swimming skill for children with autism. 45(3),<br />

440–448.<br />

380. Yılmaz, I˙., K<strong>on</strong>ukman, F., Birkan, B., Özen, A.,<br />

Yanardag˘,M.,&Çamursoy, I˙. (2010). Effects<br />

of c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay procedure <strong>on</strong> the Halliwick’s<br />

method of swimming rotati<strong>on</strong> skills for<br />

children with <strong>Autism</strong>. 45(1), 124–135.<br />

Yoo, S. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #204.<br />

Yu, C. T. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #308.<br />

Yu, C. T. (2008). See citati<strong>on</strong> #201.<br />

Yu, C. T. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #307.<br />

Yu, C.T. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #350.<br />

Yu, D. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #84.<br />

381. Yu, D. C. T., Spevack, S., Hiebert, R., Martin,<br />

T. L., Goodman, R., Martin, T. G., Harapiak,<br />

S., & Martin, G. L. (2002). Happiness indices<br />

am<strong>on</strong>g pers<strong>on</strong>s with profound <strong>and</strong> severe<br />

disabilities during leisure <strong>and</strong> work activities:<br />

A comparis<strong>on</strong>. 37(4), 421–426.<br />

Yucesoy, S. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #122.<br />

Yucesoy, S. (2006). See citati<strong>on</strong> #123.<br />

382. Zafft, C., Hart, D., & Zimbrich, K. (2004). College<br />

career c<strong>on</strong>necti<strong>on</strong>: A study of youth with<br />

intellectual disabilities <strong>and</strong> the impact of<br />

postsec<strong>on</strong>dary educati<strong>on</strong>. 39(1), 45–53.<br />

Zager, D. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #10.<br />

Zager, D. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #370.<br />

Zajicek, A. K. (2007). See citati<strong>on</strong> #89.<br />

Zhang, D. (2002). See citati<strong>on</strong> #180.<br />

Zhang, D. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #76.<br />

383. Zhang, D. (2001). Self-determinati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> inclusi<strong>on</strong>:<br />

Are students with mild mental retardati<strong>on</strong><br />

more self-determined in regular classrooms?<br />

36(4), 357–362.<br />

Zhang, D. (2005). See citati<strong>on</strong> #181.<br />

384. Zhang, D., & Stecker, P. M. (2001). Student<br />

involvement in transiti<strong>on</strong> planning: Are we<br />

there yet? 36(3), 293–303.<br />

385. Zhang, D., Ivester, J., & Katsiyannis, A.<br />

(2005). Teachers’ view of transiti<strong>on</strong> services<br />

in South Carolina: Results from a statewide<br />

survey. 40(4), 360–367.<br />

386. Zhang, D., L<strong>and</strong>mark, L., Grenwelge, C., &<br />

M<strong>on</strong>toya, L. (2010). Culturally diverse parents’<br />

perspectives <strong>on</strong> self-determinati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

45(2), 175–186.<br />

Zhang, J. (2010). See citati<strong>on</strong> #224.<br />

387. Zhang, J., & Wheeler, J. J. (2010). Mercury<br />

<strong>and</strong> autism: A review. 45(1), 107–115.<br />

Zigm<strong>on</strong>d, N. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #372.<br />

Zimbrich, K. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #159.<br />

Zimbrich, K. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #382.<br />

Zucker, S. H. (2001). See citati<strong>on</strong> #313.<br />

Zucker, S. H. (2003). See citati<strong>on</strong> #280.<br />

Zucker, S. H. (2004). See citati<strong>on</strong> #335.<br />

388. Zucker, S. H., Perras, C., Gartin B., & Fidler,<br />

D. (2005). Best practices for practiti<strong>on</strong>ers. 40<br />

(3), 199–201.<br />

389. Zucker, S. H., Perras, C., Parette, H. P., &<br />

Perner, D. E. (2007). Research to practice in<br />

Cognitive <strong>Disabilities</strong>/Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>Autism</strong>,<br />

& related disabilities. 42(3), 383–386.<br />

390. Zucker, S. H., Perras, C., Perner, D. E., &<br />

Bouck, E. C. (2010). Research to practice in<br />

<strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>.<br />

45(4), 471–474.<br />

391. Zucker, S. H., Perras, C., Perner, D. E., &<br />

Smith, J. D. (2009). Best Practices in Cognitive<br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>/Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong>, <strong>Autism</strong>, &<br />

related disabilities. 44(3), 291–294.<br />

Zurita, L. M. (2009). See citati<strong>on</strong> #358.<br />

Cumulative Author Index, Volumes 36–45 / 641


INDEX—VOLUME <str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>, 2011<br />

AUTHORS<br />

Abenis-Cintr<strong>on</strong>, Anna. See Hausmann-Stabile,<br />

Carolina ............................... 3<br />

Agran, Martin. Providing choice making in employment<br />

programs: The beginning or end of<br />

self-determinati<strong>on</strong>? ..................... 565<br />

Alber-Morgan, Sheila R. See Everhart, Julie M. 556<br />

Alberto, Paul A. See Waugh, Rebecca E. . . . 528<br />

Alex<strong>and</strong>er, Melissa. Developing the social<br />

skills of young adult special olympics athletes<br />

................................. 297<br />

Ali, Emad. Effectiveness of combining tangible<br />

symbols with the picture exchange communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

system to teach requesting skills to<br />

children with multiple disabilities including<br />

visual impairment ...................... 425<br />

Angell, Maureen E. See Bailey, Rita L. ...... 352<br />

Ayres, Kevin M. See Bramlett, Virginia ...... 454<br />

Ayres, Kevin. I can identify Saturn but I can’t<br />

brush my teeth: What happens when the<br />

curricular focus for students with severe disabilities<br />

shifts .......................... 11<br />

Baer, Robert. Disproporti<strong>on</strong>ality in transiti<strong>on</strong><br />

services: A descriptive study ............ 172<br />

Bailey, Rita L. Improving literacy skills in students<br />

with complex communicati<strong>on</strong> needs<br />

who use augmentative/alternative communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

systems ........................ 352<br />

Bambara, Linda M. See Papay, Clare ....... 78<br />

B<strong>and</strong>a, Devender R. See Dogoe, Maud ..... 204<br />

B<strong>and</strong>a, Devender. Review of video prompting<br />

studies with pers<strong>on</strong>s with developmental disabilities<br />

............................... 514<br />

Basbigill, Abby R. See Tullis, Christopher A. . 576<br />

Bassette, Laura. See Taber-Doughty, Teresa . 499<br />

Behrmann, Michael M. See Evmenova, Anna<br />

S...................................... 315<br />

Bobzien, J<strong>on</strong>na. See Gear, Sabra Bostian . . . 40<br />

Bo<strong>on</strong>, Richard. See Waters, Hugh E. ....... 544<br />

Bo<strong>on</strong>e, R<strong>and</strong>all. See Travers, Jas<strong>on</strong> C. ..... 326<br />

Bouck, Emily C. A snapshot of sec<strong>on</strong>dary educati<strong>on</strong><br />

for students with mild intellectual disabilities<br />

................................ 399<br />

Bouck, Emily C. See Taber-Doughty, Teresa . 499<br />

Bramlett, Virginia. Effects of computer <strong>and</strong><br />

classroom simulati<strong>on</strong> to teach students with<br />

various excepti<strong>on</strong>alities to locate apparel<br />

sizes ................................. 454<br />

Browder, Diane. Using shared stories <strong>and</strong> individual<br />

resp<strong>on</strong>ses modes to promote comprehensi<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> engagement in literacy for<br />

students with multiple, severe disabilities . 339<br />

Brown, Jennifer. See Kroeger, K. A. ........ 470<br />

Calzada, Esther. See Hausmann-Stabile, Carolina<br />

................................. 3<br />

Cannella-Mal<strong>on</strong>e, Helen. See Tullis, Christopher<br />

A. ................................ 576<br />

Carter, Mark. See Stephens<strong>on</strong>, Jennifer ..... 276<br />

Cihak, David F. See Bramlett, Virginia ...... 454<br />

Collins, Belva. Additi<strong>on</strong> of functi<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>tent<br />

during core c<strong>on</strong>tent instructi<strong>on</strong> with students<br />

with moderate disabilities ............... 22<br />

Collins, Belva. See Smith, Bethany ......... 251<br />

Courtemanche, Andrea B. See Leaf, Justin . . 186<br />

Cuhadar, Selmin. Effectiveness of instructi<strong>on</strong><br />

performed through activity schedules <strong>on</strong> the<br />

leisure skills of children with <strong>Autism</strong> ...... 386<br />

Darcy, Cynthia. See Werts, Margaret ....... 134<br />

Daviso III, Alfred. See Baer, Robert ......... 172<br />

Dent<strong>on</strong>, Stephen J. See Alex<strong>and</strong>er, Melissa . 297<br />

Diken, Ibrahim H. See Cuhadar, Selmin ..... 386<br />

Djuric-Zdravkovic, Aleks<strong>and</strong>ra. Arithmetic operati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

<strong>and</strong> attenti<strong>on</strong> in children with intellectual<br />

disabilities ....................... 214<br />

Dogoe, Maud S. See B<strong>and</strong>a, Devender ..... 514<br />

Dogoe, Maud. Teaching generalized reading<br />

of product warning labels to young adults<br />

with <strong>Autism</strong> using the c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay<br />

procedure ............................. 204<br />

Dots<strong>on</strong>, Wesley H. See Leaf, Justin ......... 186<br />

Douglas, Karen H. See Ayres, Kevin ........ 11<br />

Douglas, Karen H. See Bramlett, Virginia .... 454<br />

Dummer, Gail M. See Alex<strong>and</strong>er, Melissa . . . 297<br />

Everhart, Julie M. Effects of computer-based<br />

practice <strong>on</strong> the acquisiti<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> maintenance<br />

of basic academic skills for children with<br />

moderate to intensive educati<strong>on</strong> needs .... 556<br />

Evmenova, Anna S. Research-based strategies<br />

for teaching c<strong>on</strong>tent to students with<br />

intellectual disabilities: Adapted videos .... 315<br />

642 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


Feinstein, Rita. See Dogoe, Maud .......... 204<br />

Flanagan, Sara M. See Taber-Doughty, Teresa 499<br />

Fleming, Courtney V. See Tullis, Christopher<br />

A. .................................... 576<br />

Flexer, Robert W. See Baer, Robert ........ 172<br />

Flores, Margaret M. See Ganz, Jennifer ..... 596<br />

Fredrick, Laura D. See Waugh, Rebecca E. . 528<br />

Galloway, Carey Creech. See Collins, Belva . 22<br />

Ganz, Jennifer. Effects of a treatment package<br />

<strong>on</strong> imitated <strong>and</strong> sp<strong>on</strong>taneous verbal requests<br />

in children with autism ........... 596<br />

Gast, David L. See Southall, C<strong>and</strong>ice ....... 155<br />

Gear, Sabra Bostian. Teaching social skills to<br />

enhance work performance in a child care<br />

setting ................................ 40<br />

Hager, Karen L. See Collins, Belva ......... 22<br />

Hassert, Silva. See Zucker, Stanley H. ...... 619<br />

Hausmann-Stabile, Carolina. Gan<strong>and</strong>o c<strong>on</strong>fianza:<br />

Research focus groups with immigrant<br />

Mexican mothers ...................... 3<br />

Higgins, Kyle. See Travers, Jas<strong>on</strong> C. ....... 326<br />

Hoffman, Elin Meyers. See Werts, Margaret . 134<br />

Horrocks, Erin L. See Morgan, Robert L. .... 52<br />

Hourcade, Jack J. See Pool, Juli .......... 267<br />

Hunnicutt, Jenny R. See Mechling, Linda . . . 369<br />

Japundza-Milisavljevic, Mirjana. See Djuric-<br />

Zdravkovic, Aleks<strong>and</strong>ra ................. 214<br />

Jasper, Andrea D. See Taber-Doughty, Teresa 499<br />

Johns<strong>on</strong>, Valerie A. See Leaf, Justin ........ 186<br />

Judge, Shar<strong>on</strong>. See Gear, Sabra Bostian . . . 40<br />

Kang, Soye<strong>on</strong>. See Rispoli, M<strong>and</strong>y ......... 607<br />

Klein, Pnina S. See Lifshitz, Hefziba ........ 106<br />

Kleinert, Harold. See Smith, Bethany ....... 251<br />

Koutromanos, George. See Zisimopoulos,<br />

Dimitrios ............................... 238<br />

Kroeger, K. A. Placebo medicati<strong>on</strong> use for behavior<br />

management in an adult with autism . 470<br />

Krupp, Michael. See Agran, Martin ........ 565<br />

Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, Giulio. See Rispoli, M<strong>and</strong>y ....... 607<br />

Lang, Russell. See Rispoli, M<strong>and</strong>y ......... 607<br />

Lashley, Erin E. See Ganz, Jennifer ........ 596<br />

Leaf, Justin. Effects of no-no prompting <strong>on</strong><br />

teaching expressive labeling of facial expressi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

to children with <strong>and</strong> without a pervasive<br />

developmental disorder .......... 186<br />

Lee, Angela. See Browder, Diane ......... 339<br />

Lifshitz, Hefziba. Mediati<strong>on</strong> between staff-elderly<br />

pers<strong>on</strong>s with intellectual disability with<br />

Alzheimer disease as a means of enhancing<br />

their daily functi<strong>on</strong>ing ................... 106<br />

Lock, Robin H. See Dogoe, Maud ......... 204<br />

Lowrey, K. Alisa. See Ayres, Kevin ........ 11<br />

Macesic-Petrovic, Dragana. See Djuric-<br />

Zdravkovic, Aleks<strong>and</strong>ra ................. 214<br />

MacFarl<strong>and</strong>, Stephanie Z. See Ali, Emad . . . 425<br />

Matuszny, Rose Marie. See B<strong>and</strong>a, Devender 514<br />

Mechling, Linda. Computer-based video selfmodeling<br />

to teach receptive underst<strong>and</strong>ing<br />

of prepositi<strong>on</strong>s by students with intellectual<br />

disabilities ............................ 369<br />

Mechling, Linda. Review of Twenty-first century<br />

portable electr<strong>on</strong>ic devices for pers<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with moderate intellectual disabilities <strong>and</strong><br />

autism spectrum disorders .............. 479<br />

Mechling, Linda. Use of a h<strong>and</strong>-held pers<strong>on</strong>al<br />

digital assistant (PDA) to self-prompt pedestrian<br />

travel by young adults with moderate<br />

intellectual disabilities .................. 220<br />

Miller, Susan. See Travers, Jas<strong>on</strong> C. ........ 326<br />

Mims, Pam. See Browder, Diane .......... 339<br />

Mims, Pamela. See Skibo, Holly ........... 124<br />

Mo<strong>on</strong>, Sherril. Percepti<strong>on</strong>s of supported employment<br />

providers: What students with developmental<br />

disabilities, families, <strong>and</strong> educators<br />

need to know for transiti<strong>on</strong> planning . 94<br />

Morgan, Robert L. Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence between<br />

video-based preference assessment <strong>and</strong><br />

subsequent community job performance .. 52<br />

Munde, Vera. Determining alertness in individuals<br />

with profound intellectual <strong>and</strong> multiple<br />

disabilities: The reliability of an observati<strong>on</strong><br />

list ................................... 116<br />

Nakken, Han. See Munde, Vera ........... 116<br />

Neubert, Debra A. See Mo<strong>on</strong>, Sherril ...... 94<br />

O’Reilly, Mark F. See Rispoli, M<strong>and</strong>y ....... 607<br />

Oppenheim, Misty L. See Leaf, Justin ...... 186<br />

Papay, Clare. Postsec<strong>on</strong>dary educati<strong>on</strong> for<br />

transiti<strong>on</strong>-age students with intellectual <strong>and</strong><br />

other developmental disabilities: A nati<strong>on</strong>al<br />

survey ................................ 78<br />

Park, Ju Hee. See Everhart, Julie M. ........ 556<br />

Parker, Richard. See Rispoli, M<strong>and</strong>y ....... 607<br />

Payne, Daniel. See Tullis, Christopher A. .... 576<br />

Pierce, Tom. See Travers, Jas<strong>on</strong> C. ........ 326<br />

Pool, Juli. <strong>Developmental</strong> screening: A review<br />

of c<strong>on</strong>temporary practice ............... 267<br />

Queen, Rachel McMahan. See Baer, Robert . 172<br />

Raver, Shar<strong>on</strong> A. See Gear, Sabra Bostian . 40<br />

Richter, Shar<strong>on</strong>. Effects of multimedia social<br />

stories <strong>on</strong> knowledge of adult outcomes <strong>and</strong><br />

opportunities am<strong>on</strong>t transiti<strong>on</strong>-ages youth<br />

with significant cognitive disabilities ...... 410<br />

Rispoli, M<strong>and</strong>y. Effects of presessi<strong>on</strong> satiati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>on</strong> challenging behavior <strong>and</strong> academic engagement<br />

for children with autism during<br />

classroom instructi<strong>on</strong> ................... 607<br />

Ruijssenaars, Wied. See Munde, Vera ...... 116<br />

Index, Volume <str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g> / 643


Runes, S<strong>and</strong>ra. See Hausmann-Stabile, Carolina<br />

................................... 3<br />

Schuster, John W. See Smith, Bethany ..... 251<br />

Seid, Nicole H. See Mechling, Linda ....... 220<br />

Sheld<strong>on</strong>, Jan B. See Leaf, Justin .......... 186<br />

Sherman, James A. See Leaf, Justin ....... 186<br />

Sievers, Courtney. See Ayres, Kevin ....... 11<br />

Sigafoos, Jeff. See Rispoli, M<strong>and</strong>y ......... 607<br />

Sigafoos, Jeff. See Zisimopoulos, Dimitrios . 238<br />

Sim<strong>on</strong>sen, M<strong>on</strong>ica L. See Mo<strong>on</strong>, Sherril .... 94<br />

Skibo, Holly. Teaching number identificati<strong>on</strong> to<br />

students with severe disabilities using resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

cards .......................... 124<br />

Smeltzer, Ashley. See Alex<strong>and</strong>er, Melissa . . 297<br />

Smith, Bethany. Using simultaneous prompting<br />

to teach restaurant words <strong>and</strong> classificati<strong>on</strong>s<br />

as n<strong>on</strong>-target informati<strong>on</strong> to sec<strong>on</strong>dary students<br />

with moderate to severe disabilities . 251<br />

Southall, C<strong>and</strong>ice. Self-management procedures:<br />

A comparis<strong>on</strong> across the <strong>Autism</strong><br />

spectrum ............................. 155<br />

Spo<strong>on</strong>er, Fred. See Skibo, Holly ........... 124<br />

Stephens<strong>on</strong>, Jennifer. Use of multisensory envir<strong>on</strong>ments<br />

in schools for students with severe<br />

disabilities: Percepti<strong>on</strong>s from schools . 276<br />

St<strong>on</strong>er, Julia B. See Bailey, Rita L. ......... 352<br />

Taber-Doughty, Teresa. Video modeling <strong>and</strong><br />

prompting: A comparis<strong>on</strong> of two strategies<br />

for teaching cooking skills to students with<br />

mild intellectual disabilities .............. 499<br />

T<strong>and</strong>y, Richard. See Travers, Jas<strong>on</strong> C. ..... 326<br />

Test, David. See Richter, Shar<strong>on</strong> .......... 410<br />

Tom, Kinsey. See Taber-Doughty, Teresa . . 499<br />

Travers, Jas<strong>on</strong> C. Emergent literacy skills of<br />

preschool students with <strong>Autism</strong>: A comparis<strong>on</strong><br />

of teacher-led <strong>and</strong> computer-assisted<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> ............................. 326<br />

Tullis, Christopher A. Review of the choice <strong>and</strong><br />

preference assessment literature for individuals<br />

with severe to profound disabilities .. 576<br />

Umbreit, John. See Ali, Emad ............. 425<br />

Vlaskamp, Carla. See Munde, Vera ........ 116<br />

Waters, Hugh E. Teaching M<strong>on</strong>ey Computati<strong>on</strong><br />

Skills to High School Students with Mild<br />

Intellectual <strong>Disabilities</strong> via the TouchMath©<br />

Program: A Multi-Sensory Approach ...... 544<br />

Waugh, Rebecca E. Simultaneous prompting:<br />

An instructi<strong>on</strong>al strategy for skill acquisiti<strong>on</strong><br />

....................................... 528<br />

Werts, Margaret. Acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of instructive<br />

feedback: Relati<strong>on</strong> to target stimulus ..... 134<br />

Wheeler, John J. See Zhang, Jie .......... 62<br />

Whinnery, Keith W. See Whinnery, Stacie B. . 436<br />

Whinnery, Stacie B. Effects of functi<strong>on</strong>al mobility<br />

training for adults with severe disabilities<br />

................................... 436<br />

Wu, Pei-Fang. See Tullis, Christopher A. .... 576<br />

Yeager, Am<strong>and</strong>a. See Tullis, Christopher A. . 576<br />

Yokotani, Kenji. Avoidant attachment style indicates<br />

job adaptati<strong>on</strong> of people with high<br />

functi<strong>on</strong>al Autistic spectrum disorders ..... 291<br />

Zayas, Luis H. See Hausmann-Stabile, Carolina<br />

................................... 3<br />

Zhang, Jie. A meta-analysis of peer-mediated<br />

interventi<strong>on</strong>s for young children with autism<br />

spectrum disorders ..................... 62<br />

Zisimopoulos, Dimitrios. Using video prompting<br />

<strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay to teach an Internet<br />

search basic skill to students with intellectual<br />

disabilities .................... 238<br />

Zucker, Stanley H. Ten-year Cumulative Author<br />

Index of the Journals Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

Training in Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training<br />

in <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>, <strong>and</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong> 2001, 36(1) through 2010, 45(4)<br />

....................................... 619<br />

TITLES<br />

A meta-analysis of peer-mediated interventi<strong>on</strong>s<br />

for young children with autism spectrum<br />

disorders. Jie Zhang <strong>and</strong> John J.<br />

Wheeler .............................. 62<br />

A snapshot of sec<strong>on</strong>dary educati<strong>on</strong> for students<br />

with mild intellectual disabilities. Emily<br />

C. Bouck ............................. 399<br />

Acquisiti<strong>on</strong> of instructive feedback: Relati<strong>on</strong> to<br />

target stimulus. Margaret Werts, Elin Meyers<br />

Hoffman, <strong>and</strong> Cynthia Darcy ............ 134<br />

Additi<strong>on</strong> of functi<strong>on</strong>al c<strong>on</strong>tent during core c<strong>on</strong>tent<br />

instructi<strong>on</strong> with students with moderate<br />

disabilities. Belva Collins, Karen L. Hager,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Carey Creech Galloway ............ 22<br />

Arithmetic operati<strong>on</strong>s <strong>and</strong> attenti<strong>on</strong> in children<br />

with intellectual disabilities. Aleks<strong>and</strong>ra<br />

Djuric-Zdravkovic, Mirjana Japundza-<br />

Milisavljevic, <strong>and</strong> Dragana Macesic-<br />

Petrovic .............................. 214<br />

Avoidant attachment style indicates job adaptati<strong>on</strong><br />

of people with high functi<strong>on</strong>al Autistic<br />

spectrum disorders. Kenji Yokotani ....... 291<br />

Computer-based video self-modeling to teach<br />

receptive underst<strong>and</strong>ing of prepositi<strong>on</strong>s by<br />

students with intellectual disabilities. Linda<br />

Mechling <strong>and</strong> Jenny R. Hunnicutt ........ 369<br />

Corresp<strong>on</strong>dence between video-based preference<br />

assessment <strong>and</strong> subsequent community<br />

job performance. Robert L. Morgan <strong>and</strong><br />

Erin L. Horrocks ....................... 52<br />

644 / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


Determining alertness in individuals with profound<br />

intellectual <strong>and</strong> multiple disabilities:<br />

The reliability of an observati<strong>on</strong> list. Vera<br />

Munde, Carla Vlaskamp, Wied Ruijssenaars<br />

<strong>and</strong> Han Nakken ...................... 116<br />

Developing the social skills of young adult special<br />

olympics athletes. Melissa Alex<strong>and</strong>er,<br />

Gail M. Dummer, Ashley Smeltzer, <strong>and</strong> Stephen<br />

J. Dent<strong>on</strong> ........................ 297<br />

<strong>Developmental</strong> screening: A review of c<strong>on</strong>temporary<br />

practice. Juli Pool <strong>and</strong> Jack J. Hourcade<br />

................................. 267<br />

Disproporti<strong>on</strong>ality in transiti<strong>on</strong> services: A descriptive<br />

study. Robert Baer, Alfred Daviso<br />

III, Rachel McMahan Queen, <strong>and</strong> Robert W.<br />

Flexer ................................ 172<br />

Effectiveness of combining tangible symbols<br />

with the picture exchange communicati<strong>on</strong><br />

system to teach requesting skills to children<br />

with multiple disabilities including visual impairment.<br />

Emad Ali, Stephanie Z. MacFarl<strong>and</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> John Umbreit ................ 425<br />

Effectiveness of instructi<strong>on</strong> performed through<br />

activity schedules <strong>on</strong> the leisure skills of children<br />

with <strong>Autism</strong>. Selmin Cuhadar <strong>and</strong> Ibrahim<br />

H. Diken .......................... 386<br />

Effects of a treatment package <strong>on</strong> imitated <strong>and</strong><br />

sp<strong>on</strong>taneous verbal requests in children with<br />

autism. Jennifer Ganz, Margaret M. Flores,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Erin E. Lashley .................... 596<br />

Effects of computer <strong>and</strong> classroom simulati<strong>on</strong><br />

to teach students with various excepti<strong>on</strong>alities<br />

to locate apparel sizes. Virginia Bramlett,<br />

Kevin M. Ayres, Karen H. Douglas, <strong>and</strong><br />

David F. Cihak ......................... 454<br />

Effects of computer-based practice <strong>on</strong> the acquisiti<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> maintenance of basic academic<br />

skills for children with moderate to<br />

intensive educati<strong>on</strong> needs. Julie M. Everhart,<br />

Sheila R. Alber-Morgan, <strong>and</strong> Ju Hee<br />

Park .................................. 556<br />

Effects of functi<strong>on</strong>al mobility training for adults<br />

with severe disabilities. Stacie B. Whinnery<br />

<strong>and</strong> Keith W. Whinnery .................. 436<br />

Effects of multimedia social stories <strong>on</strong> knowledge<br />

of adult outcomes <strong>and</strong> opportunities<br />

am<strong>on</strong>t transiti<strong>on</strong>-ages youth with significant<br />

cognitive disabilities. Shar<strong>on</strong> Richter <strong>and</strong><br />

David Test ............................. 410<br />

Effects of no-no prompting <strong>on</strong> teaching expressive<br />

labeling of facial expressi<strong>on</strong>s to children<br />

with <strong>and</strong> without a pervasive developmental<br />

disorder. Justin Leaf, Misty L. Oppenheim,<br />

Wesley H. Dots<strong>on</strong>, Valerie A. Johns<strong>on</strong>, Andrea<br />

B. Courtemanche, Jan B. Sheld<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong><br />

James A. Sherman ..................... 186<br />

Effects of presessi<strong>on</strong> satiati<strong>on</strong> <strong>on</strong> challenging<br />

behavior <strong>and</strong> academic engagement for children<br />

with autism during classroom instructi<strong>on</strong>.<br />

M<strong>and</strong>y Rispoli, Mark F. O’Reilly, Jeff<br />

Sigafoos, Russell Lang, Soye<strong>on</strong> Kang, Giulio<br />

Lanci<strong>on</strong>i, <strong>and</strong> Richard Parker ......... 607<br />

Emergent literacy skills of preschool students<br />

with <strong>Autism</strong>: A comparis<strong>on</strong> of teacher-led<br />

<strong>and</strong> computer-assisted instructi<strong>on</strong>. Jas<strong>on</strong> C.<br />

Travers, Kyle Higgins, Tom Pierce, R<strong>and</strong>all<br />

Bo<strong>on</strong>e, Susan Miller, <strong>and</strong> Richard T<strong>and</strong>y . 326<br />

Gan<strong>and</strong>o c<strong>on</strong>fianza: Research focus groups<br />

with immigrant Mexican mothers. Carolina<br />

Hausmann-Stabile, Luis H. Zayas, S<strong>and</strong>ra<br />

Runes, Anna Abenis-Cintr<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> Esther<br />

Calzada .............................. 3<br />

I can identify Saturn but I can’t brush my teeth:<br />

What happens when the curricular focus for<br />

students with severe disabilities shifts. Kevin<br />

Ayres, K. Alisa Lowrey, Karen H. Douglas,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Courtney Sievers .................. 11<br />

Improving literacy skills in students with complex<br />

communicati<strong>on</strong> needs who use augmentative/alternative<br />

communicati<strong>on</strong> systems.<br />

Rita L. Bailey, Maureen E. Angell, <strong>and</strong><br />

Julia B. St<strong>on</strong>er ......................... 352<br />

Mediati<strong>on</strong> between staff-elderly pers<strong>on</strong>s with<br />

intellectual disability with Alzheimer disease<br />

as a means of enhancing their daily functi<strong>on</strong>ing.<br />

Hefziba Lifshitz <strong>and</strong> Pnina S. Klein . . . 106<br />

Percepti<strong>on</strong>s of supported employment providers:<br />

What students with developmental disabilities,<br />

families, <strong>and</strong> educators need to<br />

know for transiti<strong>on</strong> planning. Sherril Mo<strong>on</strong>,<br />

M<strong>on</strong>ica L. Sim<strong>on</strong>sen, <strong>and</strong> Debra A. Neubert 94<br />

Placebo medicati<strong>on</strong> use for behavior management<br />

in an adult with autism. K. A. Kroeger<br />

<strong>and</strong> Jennifer Brown .................... 470<br />

Postsec<strong>on</strong>dary educati<strong>on</strong> for transiti<strong>on</strong>-age<br />

students with intellectual <strong>and</strong> other developmental<br />

disabilities: A nati<strong>on</strong>al survey. Clare<br />

Papay <strong>and</strong> Linda M. Bambara .......... 78<br />

Providing choice making in employment programs:<br />

The beginning or end of self-determinati<strong>on</strong>?<br />

Martin Agran <strong>and</strong> Michael Krupp . 565<br />

Research-based strategies for teaching c<strong>on</strong>tent<br />

to students with intellectual disabilities:<br />

Adapted videos. Anna S. Evmenova <strong>and</strong> Michael<br />

M. Behrmann .................... 315<br />

Review of the choice <strong>and</strong> preference assessment<br />

literature for individuals with severe to<br />

profound disabilities. Christopher A. Tullis,<br />

Helen Cannella-Mal<strong>on</strong>e, Abby R. Basbigill,<br />

Am<strong>and</strong>a Yeager, Courtney V. Fleming, Daniel<br />

Payne, <strong>and</strong> Pei-Fang Wu ............. 576<br />

Review of Twenty-first century portable electr<strong>on</strong>ic<br />

devices for pers<strong>on</strong>s with moderate intellectual<br />

disabilities <strong>and</strong> autism spectrum<br />

disorders. Linda Mechling .............. 479<br />

Index, Volume <str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g> / 645


Review of video prompting studies with pers<strong>on</strong>s<br />

with developmental disabilities. Devender<br />

B<strong>and</strong>a, Maud S. Dogoe <strong>and</strong> Rose<br />

Marie Matuszny ....................... 514<br />

Self-management procedures: A comparis<strong>on</strong><br />

across the <strong>Autism</strong> spectrum. C<strong>and</strong>ice<br />

Southall <strong>and</strong> David L. Gast ............. 155<br />

Simultaneous prompting: An instructi<strong>on</strong>al strategy<br />

for skill acquisiti<strong>on</strong>. Rebecca E. Waugh,<br />

Paul A. Alberto, <strong>and</strong> Laura D. Fredrick . . . 528<br />

Teaching generalized reading of product warning<br />

labels to young adults with <strong>Autism</strong> using<br />

the c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay procedure. Maud<br />

Dogoe, Devender R. B<strong>and</strong>a, Robin H. Lock,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Rita Feinstein ...................... 204<br />

Teaching M<strong>on</strong>ey Computati<strong>on</strong> Skills to High<br />

School Students with Mild Intellectual <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

via the TouchMath© Program: A<br />

Multi-Sensory Approach . Hugh E. Waters<br />

<strong>and</strong> Richard Bo<strong>on</strong> ..................... 544<br />

Teaching number identificati<strong>on</strong> to students<br />

with severe disabilities using resp<strong>on</strong>se<br />

cards. Holly Skibo, Pamela Mims, <strong>and</strong> Fred<br />

Spo<strong>on</strong>er ............................... 124<br />

Teaching social skills to enhance work performance<br />

in a child care setting. Sabra Bostian<br />

Gear, J<strong>on</strong>na Bobzien, Shar<strong>on</strong> Judge, <strong>and</strong><br />

Shar<strong>on</strong> A. Raver ........................ 40<br />

Ten-year Cumulative Author Index of the Journals<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in Mental Retardati<strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>,<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, <strong>and</strong> Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in<br />

<strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

2001, 36(1) through 2010, 45(4). Stanley H.<br />

Zucker <strong>and</strong> Silva Hassert ................ 619<br />

Use of a h<strong>and</strong>-held pers<strong>on</strong>al digital assistant<br />

(PDA) to self-prompt pedestrian travel<br />

by young adults with moderate intellectual<br />

disabilities. Linda Mechling <strong>and</strong> Nicole H.<br />

Seid .................................. 220<br />

Use of multisensory envir<strong>on</strong>ments in schools<br />

for students with severe disabilities: Percepti<strong>on</strong>s<br />

from schools. Jennifer Stephens<strong>on</strong>,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Mark Carter ........................ 276<br />

Using shared stories <strong>and</strong> individual resp<strong>on</strong>ses<br />

modes to promote comprehensi<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> engagement<br />

in literacy for students with multiple,<br />

severe disabilities. Diane Browder, Angela<br />

Lee, <strong>and</strong> Pam Mims ................ 339<br />

Using simultaneous prompting to teach restaurant<br />

words <strong>and</strong> classificati<strong>on</strong>s as n<strong>on</strong>-target<br />

informati<strong>on</strong> to sec<strong>on</strong>dary students with moderate<br />

to severe disabilities. Bethany Smith,<br />

John W. Schuster, Belva Collins, <strong>and</strong> Harold<br />

Kleinert ................................ 251<br />

Using video prompting <strong>and</strong> c<strong>on</strong>stant time delay<br />

to teach an Internet search basic skill to<br />

students with intellectual disabilities. Dimitrios<br />

Zisimopoulos, Jeff Sigafoos, <strong>and</strong><br />

George Koutromanos ................... 238<br />

Video modeling <strong>and</strong> prompting: A comparis<strong>on</strong><br />

of two strategies for teaching cooking skills<br />

to students with mild intellectual disabilities.<br />

Teresa Taber-Doughty, Emily C. Bouck, Kinsey<br />

Tom, Andrea D. Jasper, Sara M. Flanagan,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Laura Bassette ................ 499<br />

6<str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g> / Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>-December 2011


Statement of Ownership, Management, <strong>and</strong> Circulati<strong>on</strong><br />

(Act of August 12, 1970, Secti<strong>on</strong> 3685 Title 39, United States Code)<br />

1. Title of publicati<strong>on</strong>: EDUCATION AND TRAINING IN AUTISM AND DEVELOPMENTAL DIS-<br />

ABILITIES. 2. Publicati<strong>on</strong> number: 0013-1237. 3. Date of Filing: September 30, 2011. 4. Frequency of<br />

issue: Quarterly in March, June, September, <strong>and</strong> December. 5. No. of issues published annually: 4.<br />

6. Annual subscripti<strong>on</strong> price: $60 domestic; $64 foreign; $195 instituti<strong>on</strong>; $199.50 foreign. 7. Locati<strong>on</strong><br />

of known office of publicati<strong>on</strong>: The Council for Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, 2900 Crystal Drive, Suite 1000,<br />

Arlingt<strong>on</strong>, Virginia 22202-3557. 8. Locati<strong>on</strong> of headquarters of general business offices of the publishers:<br />

The Council for Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, 2900 Crystal Drive, Suite 1000, Arlingt<strong>on</strong>, Virginia 22202-<br />

3557. 9. Names <strong>and</strong> addresses of publisher <strong>and</strong> editor: Publisher—The Council for Excepti<strong>on</strong>al<br />

Children, 2900 Crystal Drive, Suite 1000, Arlingt<strong>on</strong>, Virginia 22202-3557; Editor—Dr. Stanley H. Zucker,<br />

Special Educati<strong>on</strong> Program, Farmer 404, Ariz<strong>on</strong>a State University, PO Box 871811, Tempe, AZ 85287-<br />

1811. 10. Owner: The Council for Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children, <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong><br />

<strong>Disabilities</strong>, 2900 Crystal Drive, Suite 1000, Arlingt<strong>on</strong>, Virginia 22202-3557, no stockholders. [11. Not<br />

applicable.] 12. For completi<strong>on</strong> by n<strong>on</strong>profit organizati<strong>on</strong>s authorized to mail at special rates: The<br />

purpose, functi<strong>on</strong>, <strong>and</strong> n<strong>on</strong>profit status of this organizati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> the exempt status for Federal Income<br />

Tax purposes have not changed during the preceding 12 m<strong>on</strong>ths. 13. Publicati<strong>on</strong> name: EDUCATION<br />

AND TRAINING IN AUTISM AND DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES. 14. Issue date for circulati<strong>on</strong><br />

data below: September 2011. 15. Extent <strong>and</strong> nature of circulati<strong>on</strong>:<br />

Average no.<br />

copies ea. issue<br />

for past 12 mos.<br />

No. of copies<br />

of issue nearest<br />

filing date<br />

A. Total no. copies printed<br />

B. Paid circulati<strong>on</strong><br />

4,909 5,1405<br />

1. Sales through dealers <strong>and</strong> carriers, street vendors, <strong>and</strong><br />

counter sales<br />

125 130<br />

2. Mail subscripti<strong>on</strong> 4,568 4,864<br />

C. Total paid circulati<strong>on</strong> 4,693 4,994<br />

D. Free distributi<strong>on</strong> by mail; samples, complimentary, <strong>and</strong><br />

other free copies<br />

75 60<br />

E. Free distributi<strong>on</strong> outside the mail; carriers or other means 0 0<br />

F. Total free distributi<strong>on</strong> 75 60<br />

G. Total distributi<strong>on</strong><br />

H. Copies not distributed<br />

4,768 5,054<br />

1. Office use, left-over, unaccounted, spoiled after printing 141 86<br />

2. Returns from news agents 0 0<br />

I. Total 4,909 5,140<br />

J. Percent paid <strong>and</strong>/or requested circulati<strong>on</strong> 98.42% 98.81%<br />

16. This statement of ownership will be printed in the Vol. <str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g> No. 4 DEC 11 issue of this<br />

publicati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

17. Signature <strong>and</strong> title:<br />

Bruce A. Ramirez Executive Director September 30, 2011


Look! I'm in College! DVD<br />

Look, I’m in College! Is a half-hour documentary that follows four students through an<br />

extraordinary time in their lives. Terence, Benny, Rayquan, <strong>and</strong> D<strong>on</strong>ald are New York<br />

City public school students from high-need communities. They all have autism <strong>and</strong><br />

intellectual disabilities, <strong>and</strong> they are the charter class in a college-based inclusi<strong>on</strong><br />

program. Through collaborative efforts of the New York City District 75 <strong>and</strong> Pace<br />

University, these four young men from challenging socio-ec<strong>on</strong>omic backgrounds met with<br />

success as they participated in a college community am<strong>on</strong>g their age-appropriate peers.<br />

By the <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong> (DADD). 2008. 31 minutes.<br />

Member Price: $ 34.95<br />

N<strong>on</strong>-Member Price: $ 39.95<br />

http://www.cec.sped.org/ScriptC<strong>on</strong>tent/orders/ProductDetail.cfm?pc=D5890


Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Editorial Policy<br />

Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong> focuses <strong>on</strong> the<br />

educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> welfare of pers<strong>on</strong>s with autism <strong>and</strong> developmental disabilities.<br />

ETADD invites research <strong>and</strong> expository manuscripts <strong>and</strong> critical review of the<br />

literature. Major emphasis is <strong>on</strong> identificati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> assessment, educati<strong>on</strong>al programming,<br />

characteristics, training of instructi<strong>on</strong>al pers<strong>on</strong>nel, habilitati<strong>on</strong>, preventi<strong>on</strong>,<br />

community underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>and</strong> provisi<strong>on</strong>s, <strong>and</strong> legislati<strong>on</strong>.<br />

Each manuscript is evaluated an<strong>on</strong>ymously by three reviewers. Criteria for acceptance<br />

include the following: relevance, reader interest, quality, applicability,<br />

c<strong>on</strong>tributi<strong>on</strong> to the field, <strong>and</strong> ec<strong>on</strong>omy <strong>and</strong> smoothness of expressi<strong>on</strong>. The review<br />

process requires two to four m<strong>on</strong>ths.<br />

Viewpoints expressed are those of the authors <strong>and</strong> do not necessarily c<strong>on</strong>form to<br />

positi<strong>on</strong>s of the editors or of the officers of the <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g>.<br />

Submissi<strong>on</strong> of Manuscripts<br />

1. Manuscript submissi<strong>on</strong> is a representati<strong>on</strong> that the manuscript is the author’s<br />

own work, has not been published, <strong>and</strong> is not currently under c<strong>on</strong>siderati<strong>on</strong> for<br />

publicati<strong>on</strong> elsewhere.<br />

2. Manuscripts must be prepared according to the recommendati<strong>on</strong>s in the Publicati<strong>on</strong><br />

Manual of the American Psychological Associati<strong>on</strong> (Sixth Editi<strong>on</strong>, 2009).<br />

Laser or high density dot printing are acceptable.<br />

3. Each manuscript must have a cover sheet giving the names <strong>and</strong> affiliati<strong>on</strong>s of all<br />

authors <strong>and</strong> the address of the principal author.<br />

4. Graphs <strong>and</strong> figures should be originals or sharp, high quality photographic<br />

prints suitable, if necessary, for a 50% reducti<strong>on</strong> in size.<br />

5. Five copies of the manuscript al<strong>on</strong>g with a transmittal letter should be sent to the<br />

Editor: Stanley H. Zucker, Mary Lou Fult<strong>on</strong> Teachers College, Box 871811,<br />

Ariz<strong>on</strong>a State University, Tempe, AZ 85287-1811.<br />

6. Up<strong>on</strong> receipt, each manuscript will be screened by the editor. Appropriate<br />

manuscripts will then be sent to c<strong>on</strong>sulting editors. Principal authors will receive<br />

notificati<strong>on</strong> of receipt of manuscript.<br />

7. The Editor reserves the right to make minor editorial changes which do not<br />

materially affect the meaning of the text.<br />

8. Manuscripts are the property of ETADD for a minimum period of six m<strong>on</strong>ths.<br />

All articles accepted for publicati<strong>on</strong> are copyrighted in the name of the <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g><br />

<strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong>.


13th Internati<strong>on</strong>al C<strong>on</strong>ference <strong>on</strong><br />

<strong>Autism</strong>, Intellectual <strong>Disabilities</strong>,<br />

& <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

Research to Practice<br />

Council for Excepti<strong>on</strong>al Children<br />

<str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> & <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

The Board of Directors for CEC‛s <str<strong>on</strong>g>Divisi<strong>on</strong></str<strong>on</strong>g> <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong> (DADD), is pleased to<br />

extend an invitati<strong>on</strong> to join us in Miami Beach, Florida, January 18 - 20, 2012, for a stellar professi<strong>on</strong>al<br />

learning opportunity!<br />

DADD‛s 13 th Internati<strong>on</strong>al C<strong>on</strong>ference <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong>, Intellectual <strong>Disabilities</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong><br />

will integrate research <strong>and</strong> practice, reflecting the need for evidence-based strategies within this<br />

diverse field.<br />

The program features more than 100 lecture <strong>and</strong> poster presentati<strong>on</strong>s; c<strong>on</strong>ference delegates may also<br />

attend an in-depth pre-c<strong>on</strong>ference training institute <strong>on</strong> <strong>Autism</strong> Spectrum Disorders: Practical Soluti<strong>on</strong>s to<br />

Everyday Challenges, ledbyDr.BrendaSmithMyles.<br />

Featured speakers include Robert Pio Hajjar (Self-Advocate), Dr. Robert Stodden, Dr. Tom E.C. Smith,<br />

Dr. Michael Wehmeyer, Dr. Am<strong>and</strong>a Boutot, Taylor Crowe (Self-Advocate) <strong>and</strong> Dr. David Crowe.<br />

Our C<strong>on</strong>ference will be held at the historic Deauville Beach Resort, <strong>on</strong>e of the Gr<strong>and</strong>e-Dame hotels in<br />

Miami Beach.<br />

For further informati<strong>on</strong>, please c<strong>on</strong>tact:<br />

Cindy Perras<br />

C<strong>on</strong>ference Co-ordinator<br />

CEC-DADD<br />

cindy.perras@cogeco.ca<br />

www.daddcec.org


December 2011 Educati<strong>on</strong> <strong>and</strong> Training in <strong>Autism</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Developmental</strong> <strong>Disabilities</strong> Vol. <str<strong>on</strong>g>46</str<strong>on</strong>g>, No. 4, pp. 477–648

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!