29.07.2013 Views

Reproduction performances and conditions of group-housed non ...

Reproduction performances and conditions of group-housed non ...

Reproduction performances and conditions of group-housed non ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

<strong>Reproduction</strong> <strong>performances</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>conditions</strong><br />

<strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>group</strong>-<strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows<br />

Ph.D. thesis by<br />

Anne Grete Kongsted<br />

Department <strong>of</strong> Large Animal Sciences<br />

The Royal Veterinary <strong>and</strong> Agricultural University<br />

Ridebanevej 12, DK-1870 Frederiksberg C<br />

&<br />

Department <strong>of</strong> Agroecology<br />

Danish Institute <strong>of</strong> Agricultural Sciences, Research centre Foulum<br />

P.O. Box 50, DK-8830 Tjele<br />

2004


PREFACE AND AKNOWLEDGEMENTS<br />

The present thesis intends to meet the requirements for obtaining the Ph.D. degree from the<br />

Royal Veterinary <strong>and</strong> Agricultural University, Copenhagen (RVAU). Throughout the<br />

Ph.D. study, the undersigned has been attached to the research <strong>group</strong> Farming Systems,<br />

Dept. <strong>of</strong> Agroecology, Danish Institute <strong>of</strong> Agricultural Sciences (DIAS), Research Centre<br />

Foulum. The study has been financially supported by the Danish Research Agency.<br />

I would like to express my gratitude to my supervisors, senior scientist Troels Kristensen,<br />

Dept. <strong>of</strong> Agroecology (DIAS) <strong>and</strong> Associate Pr<strong>of</strong>essor Sven Bresson, Dept. <strong>of</strong> Large Animal<br />

Sciences (RVAU) for their guidance <strong>and</strong> advice much appreciated. Very special<br />

thanks for co-supervision go to the head <strong>of</strong> the Farming System Group, John E. Hermansen,<br />

whose always constructive criticism <strong>and</strong> unfailing high spirits have been very valuable,<br />

especially during the last couple <strong>of</strong> months. I also wish to thank the rest <strong>of</strong> the Farming<br />

System Group for providing a good collegiate environment. Special thanks to Jytte<br />

Christensen for her excellent assistance with data processing <strong>and</strong> to Lene Kirkegaard for<br />

her help with Reference Manager.<br />

The Ph.D. study included a farm study involving 14 sow herds. I wish to thank the 14<br />

farmers <strong>and</strong> their co-workers for their cooperativeness, kindness <strong>and</strong> great inspiration. I<br />

would also like to express my gratitude to the Research Technicians Henrik K. Andersen,<br />

Kristine R. Hansen, Michael Hansen, Orla Nielsen, Niels H. Thomsen <strong>and</strong> Helge Yde for<br />

their much appreciated assistance in carrying out the data collection.<br />

For discussions <strong>of</strong> statistical analyses, I am very grateful to Senior Scientist Jens Henrik<br />

Badsberg <strong>and</strong> especially to Senior Scientist Erik Jørgensen for his great contributions to<br />

the statistical analyses <strong>and</strong> well-timed ‘pep-talks’. I am also grateful to Dr. Nicoline Soede<br />

<strong>and</strong> Dr. Wouter Hazeleger, Dept. <strong>of</strong> Animal Sciences, Wageningen University, for allowing<br />

me to stay in their <strong>group</strong> for two months <strong>and</strong> for sharing some <strong>of</strong> their knowledge <strong>of</strong><br />

reproduction physiology with me.<br />

I would also like to thank the following people for valuable discussions/assistance: Ph.D.<br />

Marianne Bonde, Ph.D. Tine Rousing, Senior Scientist Lene J. Pedersen, Senior Scientist<br />

Viggo Dannielsen (all from DIAS), Lisbeth U. Hansen, Lisbeth B. Petersen, Flemming<br />

Thorup <strong>and</strong> Brian Fisker (all from The National Committee for Pig Production).<br />

Finally, a very grateful thought to all the two- <strong>and</strong> four-legged creatures at ‘Skovbakken’<br />

whose calm attitudes have been an essential fountain <strong>of</strong> relaxation throughout the study,<br />

<strong>and</strong> especially THANK YOU, Jens, for always being there 110% - also on rainy days.<br />

Foulum, September 2004 , Anne Grete Kongsted


Contents<br />

Summary……………………………………………………………………..1<br />

Sammendrag (Danish)……………………………………………………….5<br />

Background <strong>and</strong> aim…………………………………………………………9<br />

Outline <strong>of</strong> this thesis………………………………………………………..13<br />

How does <strong>group</strong> housing vary in practice?…………………………………15<br />

Paper I. Effect <strong>of</strong> energy intake on pregnancy rate <strong>and</strong> litter size with<br />

particular reference to <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows – a review………21<br />

Paper II. Stress <strong>and</strong> fear as possible mediators <strong>of</strong> reproduction problems<br />

In <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> sows: A review…………………………………………..49<br />

Paper III. Indicators <strong>of</strong> feed intake, fear <strong>and</strong> social stress in commercial herds with<br />

<strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows………………………………………….59<br />

Paper IV. Relation between reproduction performance <strong>and</strong> indicators <strong>of</strong><br />

feed intake, fear <strong>and</strong> social stress in commercial herds with <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong><br />

<strong>non</strong>-lactating sows…………………………………………………………87<br />

General Discussion………………………………………………………..103<br />

Conclusions………………………………………………………………..109<br />

Appendix 1. Litter size <strong>and</strong> farrowing rate, which physiological<br />

processes may go wrong <strong>and</strong> why? ………………………..…………………. 111


- Summary -<br />

SUMMARY<br />

In the last decade the number <strong>of</strong> <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows has been increasing in<br />

Europe. This is mainly caused by elevated public concern <strong>of</strong> animal welfare with changed<br />

legislation as a consequence. However, among herds with <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows<br />

there is a huge variation in the reproduction results. The apparently large variation in farrowing<br />

rate <strong>and</strong> litter size indicates that it will be possible to improve these parameters in<br />

some <strong>of</strong> these herds. On this background the overall aim <strong>of</strong> this thesis was to produce<br />

knowledge that managers can implement in their decision-making to improve the reproduction<br />

performance <strong>of</strong> <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows. The specific aims were to identify<br />

important causes for impaired reproduction performance in <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating<br />

sows <strong>and</strong> to develop <strong>and</strong> evaluate indicators suitable for use in decision-making in commercial<br />

sow herds.<br />

First, a review <strong>of</strong> the literature <strong>of</strong> reproduction physiology was carried out (Appendix 1). It<br />

was concluded that the endocrine regulation <strong>of</strong> reproduction might be influenced by energy<br />

intake <strong>and</strong> stress. Since experimental studies had indicated that <strong>group</strong> housing might lead<br />

to unequal feed intake <strong>and</strong> social stress, the hypothesis was put forward that individual<br />

variation in feed intake <strong>and</strong> social stress might cause impaired reproduction performance in<br />

some <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows. To illuminate this further, two review papers were<br />

written a) to consider the effect <strong>of</strong> energy intake (Paper I) as well as social stress <strong>and</strong> fear<br />

(Paper II) <strong>and</strong> b) to define indicators <strong>of</strong> feed intake, stress <strong>and</strong> fear suitable for use in practice.<br />

A study in 14 herds with different layouts <strong>and</strong> management routines was carried out to<br />

evaluate the defined indicators (Paper III) <strong>and</strong> to investigate whether the indicators were<br />

suitable to express variation in the reproduction performance <strong>of</strong> sows under practical <strong>conditions</strong><br />

(Paper IV).<br />

Paper I<br />

The aim <strong>of</strong> this review was to consider whether the variation in energy intake in a <strong>group</strong> <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>non</strong>-lactating sows can influence variation in litter size <strong>and</strong> pregnancy rate in practice.<br />

Through a review <strong>of</strong> existing literature with main emphasis on publications after 1980, the<br />

effect <strong>of</strong> energy supply before mating <strong>and</strong> in pregnancy on pregnancy rate <strong>and</strong> litter size<br />

was discussed. Based upon experimental studies, indicating that low ranking sows may<br />

consume considerably less than high ranking sows (e.g. 50-80%) in <strong>group</strong> housing, it was<br />

suggested that the variation in feed intake in a <strong>group</strong> <strong>of</strong> restrictedly fed pregnant female<br />

pigs may be sufficiently severe to influence pregnancy rate <strong>and</strong> litter size.<br />

1


Paper II<br />

- Summary -<br />

A review <strong>of</strong> 15 experiments with <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> sows showed that there are indications that<br />

stress <strong>and</strong> fear might be contributing reasons for the impaired reproduction seen in some<br />

<strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> sows. Possible initiators <strong>of</strong> stress <strong>and</strong> fear might be mixing <strong>of</strong> unfamiliar<br />

sows <strong>and</strong> high stocking rates. Traditional methods for assessing stress <strong>and</strong> fear are expen-<br />

sive <strong>and</strong>/or time consuming <strong>and</strong> therefore difficult to use in large-scale on-farm studies or<br />

as components in a management tool to analyse <strong>and</strong> improve the reproduction performance<br />

in <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> sows. Therefore, based on existing knowledge, possible indicators <strong>of</strong><br />

stress <strong>and</strong> fear related reproduction problems suitable for use in practice were put forward<br />

in the paper. However, whether these indicators were suitable to express variation in sows<br />

susceptibility for a good reproduction performance under practical <strong>conditions</strong> was not<br />

known.<br />

Paper III<br />

Experimental studies have indicated that <strong>group</strong> housing may lead to individual variation in<br />

feed intake, fear <strong>and</strong> social stress. However, systematic information <strong>of</strong> between-herd <strong>and</strong><br />

within-herd variation in feed intake, fear <strong>and</strong> social stress in sows <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> under<br />

various <strong>conditions</strong> is lacking. Most likely, this is to some extent because <strong>of</strong> a lack <strong>of</strong> suitable<br />

assessment methods. With the aim to evaluate indicators <strong>of</strong> feed intake, fear <strong>and</strong> social<br />

stress <strong>and</strong> to get insight in the level <strong>and</strong> variation in these indicators in <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> sows<br />

under various on-farm <strong>conditions</strong>, a farm study took place including 14 commercial herds<br />

followed in 11 month. The results showed that back fat, skin lesions <strong>and</strong> behavioural<br />

measurements might be relevant indicators <strong>of</strong> the condition <strong>of</strong> the sows regarding feed<br />

intake, stress <strong>and</strong> fear at herd, batch, <strong>and</strong> individual sow level in commercial herds with<br />

<strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows. For almost all indicators the variation between herds was<br />

larger than the variation between batches within herds. However, the largest contribution to<br />

the variation came from the variation between the individual sows. The between-sow<br />

variation in back fat at farrowing was significantly higher in herds with <strong>group</strong> feeding than<br />

in herds with individual feeding. The study indicated that <strong>group</strong> feeding may lead to overfeeding<br />

<strong>of</strong> high ranking sows <strong>and</strong> severe underfeeding <strong>of</strong> a few low ranking individuals.<br />

The presence <strong>of</strong> feeding stalls reduced the level <strong>of</strong> aggressions the first hour after weaning.<br />

Irrespective <strong>of</strong> layout, sows older than third parity were involved in most aggressions the<br />

first hour after mixing on the day <strong>of</strong> weaning. However, three weeks after mating <strong>and</strong> at<br />

farrowing, these sows had the lowest level <strong>of</strong> skin lesions indicating that any stress experienced<br />

by these old sows was short-lasting. In herds with no escape possibilities, first parity<br />

sows had the highest level <strong>of</strong> skin lesions three weeks after mating <strong>and</strong> at farrowing,<br />

whereas in herds with escape possibilities, second <strong>and</strong> third parity sows had the highest<br />

level. Herds with electronic sow feeding <strong>and</strong> large dynamic <strong>group</strong>s had the highest average<br />

level <strong>of</strong> skin injuries.<br />

2


Paper IV<br />

- Summary -<br />

Results from experimental studies suggest that <strong>group</strong> housing may lead to individual varia-<br />

tion in feed intake, stress <strong>and</strong> fear, which may impair the reproduction performance. How-<br />

ever, whether the individual variation in feed intake, stress <strong>and</strong> fear in sows <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong><br />

under commercial <strong>conditions</strong> is severe enough to be responsible for an impairment <strong>of</strong> the<br />

reproduction performance was not known. Therefore, a detailed farm study including 14<br />

herds with different layouts <strong>and</strong> management routines was carried out <strong>and</strong> the relations<br />

between various indicators <strong>of</strong> feed intake, stress <strong>and</strong> fear <strong>and</strong> reproduction performance<br />

were studied. Positive correlations between back fat gain from weaning to three weeks<br />

after mating <strong>and</strong> chance <strong>of</strong> pregnancy (P


- Sammendrag -<br />

SAMMENDRAG<br />

Antallet af grupppeopstaldede ikke-lakterende søer i Europa er stigende. Årsagen er øget<br />

fokus på dyrevelfærd, som har resulteret i ændrede lovgivninger. Bl<strong>and</strong>t besætninger med<br />

gruppeopstaldede søer i hele ikke-laktationsperioden er der en stor variation i reproduktionsresultater.<br />

Dette indikerer, at det vil være muligt at forbedre disse parametre i mange<br />

besætninger. På den baggrund var det overordnede mål med denne afh<strong>and</strong>ling at producere<br />

viden, som kan forbedre beslutningsgrundlaget for driftsledere i besætninger med gruppeopstaldede<br />

søer. De specifikke mål var at identificere væsentlige årsager til ringe reproduktion<br />

hos gruppeopstaldede søer samt at definere og vurdere indikatorer velegnede til<br />

brug som beslutningsstøtte i kommercielle besætninger.<br />

Først blev et litteraturstudie af soens reproduktionsfysiologi gennemført (Appendiks 1). Det<br />

blev konkluderet, at energi indtag og stress kan påvirke den fysiologiske regulering af soens<br />

reproduktion. Da eksperimentelle studier har indikeret, at gruppeopstaldning kan føre til<br />

ulige energi indtag og social stress, blev hypotesen fremsat, at individuel variation i foderindtag<br />

og social stress kan være medvirkende årsag til ringe reproduktionsresultater hos<br />

gruppeopstaldede søer. For at undersøge dette nærmere blev der skrevet to review-artikler<br />

for at a) undersøge effekten af energiindtag (artikel I) og effekten af social stress og frygtsomhed<br />

(artikel II) samt b) definere indikatorer for stress og frygtsomhed velegnede til brug<br />

i praksis. Et studie, der inkluderede 14 besætninger med forskellige indretninger blev gennemført<br />

for at evaluere de definerede indikatorer (artikel III) og for at undersøge om indikatorerne<br />

var velegnede til at udtrykke variation i søers reproduktion under praktiske forhold<br />

(artikel IV).<br />

Artikel I<br />

Formålet med dette litteraturstudie var at vurdere om variationen i energiindtag i en gruppe<br />

af søer kan påvirke variationen i kuldstørrelse og drægtighedsprocent i praksis. På baggrund<br />

af en gennemgang af eksisterende litteratur med fokus på publikationer efter 1980,<br />

blev effekten af energiindtag i den ikke-lakterende periode på kuldstørrelse og drægtighed<br />

diskuteret. Baseret på litteraturstudiet og eksperimentelle studier, som havde vist, at en lavt<br />

rangerende sos foderindtag kan være betydeligt mindre end en højtrangerende sos (fx 50-<br />

80%) i gruppeopstaldning, blev det foreslået, at foderindtag hos gruppeopstaldede ikkelakterende<br />

søer kan variere i tilstrækkelig grad til, at både drægtighed og kuldstørrelse kan<br />

påvirkes negativt.<br />

Artikel II<br />

Et litteraturstudie af bl.a. 15 eksperimenter med gruppeopstaldede søer bekræftede, at stress<br />

og frygtsomhed kan være medvirkende årsager til ringe reproduktionsresultater hos nogle<br />

5


- Sammendrag -<br />

gruppeopstaldede søer. Mulige årsager til stress og frygtsomhed er sammenbl<strong>and</strong>ing af søer<br />

og høje belægningsgrader. Traditionelle metoder til at vurdere stress og frygtsomhed er<br />

dyre og/eller tidskrævende og derfor uegnede som komponenter i et driftsledelsesværktøj til<br />

at analysere og forbedre reproduktionen hos gruppeopstaldede søer. Derfor, på baggrund af<br />

en litteraturgennemgang, blev indikatorer, der forventedes at være velegnede til at udtrykke<br />

variation i søers reproduktionsresultater i praksis, defineret.<br />

Artikel III<br />

Resultater fra eksperimentelle studier indikerede, at gruppeopstaldning kan føre til individuel<br />

variation i foderindtag, stress og frygtsomhed. Systematisk information om variation<br />

mellem besætninger og indenfor besætning i forskellige gruppeopstaldnings systemer eksisterer<br />

imidlertid ikke. Dette skyldes formentlig, delvist en mangel på velegnede målemetoder.<br />

Bl<strong>and</strong>t <strong>and</strong>et med det formål at evaluere indikatorer for foderindtag, stress og frygtsomhed<br />

samt at få indsigt i niveau og variation i disse indikatorer i besætninger med forskellige<br />

indretninger og driftsledelse, blev et studie gennemført, som inkluderede 14 private<br />

besætninger fulgt i 11 måneder. Resultaterne indikerede, at målinger af rygspæk og<br />

sår/rifter kombineret med adfærdsmæssige observationer var relevante indikatorer for soens<br />

tilst<strong>and</strong> i forhold til foderindtag, stress og frygtsomhed. For størsteparten af indikatorerne<br />

var variationen mellem besætninger større end variationen mellem ugehold indenfor besætning.<br />

Størstedelen af variationen kom dog fra variationen mellem søer. Den individuelle<br />

variationen i rygspæk ved faring var signifikant større i systemer med gruppefodring i fht.<br />

systemer med individuel fodring. Undersøgelsen indikerede, at gruppefodring kan føre til<br />

overfodring af de højtrangerende søer og kraftig underfodring af nogle få lavt rangerende<br />

søer. Tilstedeværelsen af ædebokse reducerede antallet af aggressioner den første time efter<br />

sammenbl<strong>and</strong>ing. Søer ældre end 3. læg var involveret i flest aggressioner på fravænningsdagen,<br />

men tre uger efter løbning havde disse søer det laveste antal sår/rifter, hvilket indikerer,<br />

at eventuel stress oplevet af disse søer kun har været kortvarig. I besætninger uden<br />

flugtmuligheder havde 1. lægssøerne flest sår/rifter tre uger efter løbning og ved faring,<br />

hvorimod i besætninger med flugtmuligheder havde 2. og 3. lægssøerne det højeste niveau.<br />

Besætninger med elektronisk s<strong>of</strong>odring og store dynamiske grupper havde det højeste niveau<br />

af sår/rifter.<br />

Artikel IV<br />

Eksperimentelle studier har indikeret, at gruppeopstaldning kan føre til individuel variation<br />

i foderindtag, social stress og frygtsomhed. Hvorvidt denne variation i praksis er af et tilstrækkeligt<br />

omfang til at være ansvarlig for en reduktion i reproduktionsresultater vides<br />

imidlertid ikke. Bl<strong>and</strong>t <strong>and</strong>et derfor blev et detaljeret studie gennemført som inkluderede 14<br />

private besætninger med forskellige indretninger og driftsledelse, og relationen mellem<br />

reproduktionen og en række indikatorer for foderindtag, stress og frygtsomhed blev under-<br />

6


- Sammendrag -<br />

søgt. Positive korrelationer mellem rygspæktilvækst fra fravænning til tre uger efter løbning<br />

og s<strong>and</strong>synligheden for drægtighed (P>0.05) og kuldstørrelse (P=0.07) blev fundet. Søer<br />

som åd mindre end 20% af alle observationer i forbindelse med fodring havde en signifikant<br />

større risiko for at løbe om i fht. søer som åd mere. Derudover, søer med mindre end<br />

10 mm rygspæk ved fravænning var i højere risiko for at blive udsat i fht. federe søer. Antallet<br />

af sår/rifter korrelerede positivt med interval fra fravænning til første løbning<br />

(P=0.07). Der blev ikke fundet sammenhænge mellem reproduktion og liggeadfærd, antal<br />

aggressioner involveret i efter sammenbl<strong>and</strong>ing eller frygtsomhed estimeret vha. to frygttests.<br />

Samlet set tyder resultaterne på, at gruppeopstaldning kan føre til tilstrækkelig individuel<br />

variation i foderindtag til, at reproduktionen kan påvirkes negativt. Som en følge heraf<br />

kan rygspækmålinger og observationer af søernes ædeadfærd muligvis være brugbare indikatorer<br />

af fodringsrelaterede reproduktionsproblemer hos gruppeopstaldede søer. Der blev<br />

ikke fundet overbevisende sammenhænge mellem indikatorerne for social<br />

stress/frygtsomhed og reproduktionen. Dette demonstrerer imidlertid ikke nødvendigvis, at<br />

der ikke er nogen sammenhæng mellem disse karakteristika og reproduktionen, men det<br />

viser, at de pågældende indikatorer ikke er velegnede til at afspejle gruppeopstaldede søers<br />

reproduktionsevne i praksis.<br />

Sammenfattende tyder det på, at:<br />

1) individuel variation i foderindtag kan være medvirkende årsag til ringe reproduktionsresultater<br />

hos nogle gruppeopstaldede ikke-lakterende søer<br />

2) rygspækmålinger og observationer af søernes ædeadfærd kan være brugbare indikatorer<br />

for fodringsrelaterede reproduktionsproblemer hos gruppeopstaldede søer.<br />

Som følge heraf, kan disse indikatorer muligvis udgøre en vigtig komponent i et<br />

driftsledelsesværktøj til at analysere og forbedre reproduktionen hos gruppeopstaldede<br />

ikke-lakterende søer.<br />

7


- Background <strong>and</strong> aim -<br />

BACKGROUND AND AIM<br />

For several decades, individual housing <strong>of</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows was preferred, probably because<br />

individual housing made it possible to control the individual sows access to important<br />

resources like feed <strong>and</strong> water. However, in the last decade the number <strong>of</strong> <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>lactating<br />

sows has begun to increase in Europe. This is mainly caused by elevated public<br />

concern <strong>of</strong> animal welfare with changed legislations as a consequence. For instance, according<br />

to EU legislation all sows have, from January 2013, to be loose-<strong>housed</strong> in smaller<br />

or larger <strong>group</strong>s from four weeks after mating until seven days before expected farrowing<br />

(Council Directive 2001/88/EC amending Directive 91/630/EEC Laying Down Minimum<br />

St<strong>and</strong>ards for the Protection <strong>of</strong> Pigs). In addition, national extraordinary laws have been<br />

introduced in several countries. In Engl<strong>and</strong>, for instance, all sows have to be <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong><br />

in the entire period from weaning to seven days before expected farrowing according to the<br />

national legislation (The welfare <strong>of</strong> Farmed Animals (Engl<strong>and</strong>) (Amendment) Regulations<br />

2003). In Norway, the sows may only be fixed from three days before until one week after<br />

farrowing, <strong>and</strong> in Sweden it is only allowed to keep sows in crates for maximum one week<br />

if necessary during the production cyclus (Baustad & Lium, 2002). Although no laws or<br />

regulations so far, similar tendencies are also seen in other parts <strong>of</strong> the world (McGlone,<br />

2001; Trezona, 2003).<br />

In Denmark it is still legal to keep the sows in crates from weaning until four weeks after<br />

mating. Nevertheless, the Danish Bacon <strong>and</strong> Meat Council, motivated by export interests,<br />

has introduced an extra pay for slaughter pigs produced by sows that are <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> in<br />

the entire <strong>non</strong>-lactating period. This additional price has caused an increase in the number<br />

<strong>of</strong> sows that are <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> from weaning to shortly before farrowing in Denmark.<br />

However, impaired reproduction in form <strong>of</strong> reduced litter size <strong>and</strong> pregnancy rate in <strong>group</strong><br />

<strong>housed</strong> compared to individual <strong>housed</strong> sows in parts <strong>of</strong> or in the entire <strong>non</strong>-lactating period<br />

has been observed in several Danish on-farm experiments. Sows <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> from weaning<br />

until two days after mating had significant fewer total born piglets compared to sows<br />

individually <strong>housed</strong> in the same period (Hansen, 2000). Sows <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> from weaning<br />

to farrowing had significant fewer total born piglets per litter than sows kept individually in<br />

crates the first four weeks after weaning <strong>and</strong> thereafter loose <strong>housed</strong> until farrowing<br />

(Fisker, 1995). Equally, in other countries a reduced farrowing rate has been seen in <strong>group</strong><br />

<strong>housed</strong> compared to individual <strong>housed</strong> sows (USA: Hurtgen et al., 1980; Finl<strong>and</strong>:<br />

Peltoniemi et al., 1999). Conversely, in other studies, no difference (conception rate <strong>and</strong><br />

litter size: Engl<strong>and</strong> & Spurr, 1969) between <strong>group</strong>ed <strong>and</strong> individually <strong>housed</strong> sows or even<br />

opposite effects (farrowing rate: Bates et al., 2003; Hansen, 2003) have been found. The<br />

divergent results are probably a result <strong>of</strong> differences in the function <strong>of</strong> the <strong>group</strong> housing<br />

9


- Background <strong>and</strong> aim -<br />

systems <strong>and</strong> shows that <strong>group</strong> housing do not ‘automatically’ lead to poor reproduction<br />

performance.<br />

This is further supported by indications <strong>of</strong> a large variation in reproduction performance<br />

between <strong>group</strong> housing systems. In a pilot study the litter size varied from 10.6 to 13.1 born<br />

piglets per litter in five herds with <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows (Nielsen & Calmann-<br />

Hinke, 1998). Data from 78 Danish herds with sows <strong>housed</strong> outdoor in the lactation period<br />

<strong>and</strong> in <strong>group</strong>s either inside or outside in paddocks during the <strong>non</strong>-lactating period showed<br />

an average farrowing rate <strong>of</strong> 78 (range 53-93) % <strong>and</strong> an average litter size <strong>of</strong> 12.1 (range<br />

10.6-14.1) born piglets per litter (Kirk, 2001 pers. comm.).<br />

The apparently large variation in farrowing rate <strong>and</strong> litter size between herds with <strong>group</strong><br />

<strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows indicates that it will be possible to improve the reproduction<br />

performance in some <strong>of</strong> these herds. However, to do that it is first <strong>of</strong> all necessary to identify<br />

important causes for impaired reproduction performance in <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating<br />

sows.<br />

It is likely that numerous management-related factors might influence the function <strong>of</strong> <strong>group</strong><br />

housing systems. Therefore, it will probably be difficult to forecast which factors may influence<br />

reproduction in <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> sows, merely on the background <strong>of</strong> general defined<br />

housing factors like stocking rate, <strong>group</strong> size, floor type etc. Furthermore, even though<br />

main effects <strong>of</strong> different factors can be demonstrated in experimental studies, interactions<br />

might lead to unexpected results in real-life <strong>conditions</strong> (Capdeville & Veissier, 2001). As a<br />

consequence, it seems important that managers are able to make an in situ analysis <strong>of</strong> the<br />

reproduction in <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> sows. To be able to perform an analysis like that, indicators<br />

which provide information <strong>of</strong> the function <strong>of</strong> <strong>group</strong> housing systems <strong>and</strong> suitable for use in<br />

practice are needed.<br />

On this background the overall aim <strong>of</strong> this study is to produce knowledge that managers<br />

can implement in their decision-making to improve the reproduction performance <strong>of</strong> <strong>group</strong><br />

<strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows. The specific aims are:<br />

• To identify important causes for impaired reproduction performance in <strong>group</strong><br />

<strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows.<br />

• To develop <strong>and</strong> evaluate indicators suitable for use in decision-making in commercial<br />

sow herds<br />

10


References<br />

- Background <strong>and</strong> aim -<br />

Bates, R.O., Edwards, D.B., Korthals, R.L., 2003. Sow performance when <strong>housed</strong> either in <strong>group</strong>s with elec-<br />

tronic sow feeder or stalls. Livestock Production Science 79, 29-35.<br />

Baustad, B., Lium, B., 2002. Helse og dyrevelferd i norsk svineproduksjon sett i et internasjonalt perspektiv.<br />

Norsk Veterinærtidsskrift 114, 87-91.<br />

Capdeville, J., Veissier, I., 2001. A metod <strong>of</strong> assessing welfare in loose <strong>housed</strong> dairy cows at farm level, fo-<br />

cusing on animal observations. Acta Agric. Sc<strong>and</strong>. , Sect. A, Animal Science Suppl. 30, 62-68.<br />

Engl<strong>and</strong>, D.C., Spurr, D.T., 1969. Litter size <strong>of</strong> swine confined during gestation. Journal <strong>of</strong> Animal Science<br />

28, 220-223.<br />

Fisker, B.N., 1995. Indsættelsesstrategi for gruppefodrede drægtige søer. Meddelelse 311, L<strong>and</strong>sudvalget for<br />

Svin, Den rullende Afprøvning, 7pp.<br />

Hansen, L.U., 2000. Løbeafdeling med enkeltdyrsstier eller flokopstaldning. Meddelelse 6, L<strong>and</strong>sudvalget for<br />

Svin, Den rullende Afprøvning, 6pp.<br />

Hansen, L.U., 2003. Løbeafdeling med enkeltdyrsstier eller flokopstaldning med permanent adgang til æde-<br />

/insemineringsbokse. Meddelelse 602, L<strong>and</strong>sudvalget for Svin, Danske Slagterier, 6pp.<br />

Hurtgen, J.P., Leman, A.D., Crabo, B. 1980. Effect <strong>of</strong> season, parity <strong>and</strong> housing factors on estrus <strong>and</strong> fertility<br />

in swine. Proc. Int. Pig. Vet. Soc., Copenhagen, Denmark. Pp. 20<br />

Kirk, A. 2001. Personal communication. Konsulent, Svinerådgivning Midt-Vest, Holstebro.<br />

McGlone, J.J. 2001. Alternative sow housing systems: Driven by legislation, regulation, free trade <strong>and</strong> free<br />

market systems (but not science). Paper presented at jan 2001 Annual meeting <strong>of</strong> the Manitoba pork<br />

producers, Winnipeg, Manitoba, Canada. 12pp.<br />

Nielsen, N.-P., Calmann-Hinke, D., 1998. Løbeafdelinger med flokopstaldede søer fodret efter ædelyst. Erfaring<br />

9807, L<strong>and</strong>sudvalget for Svin, Den rullende Afprøvning, 10pp.<br />

Peltoniemi, O.A.T., Love, R.J., Heionen, M., Tuovinen, V., Saloiemi, H., 1999. Seasonal <strong>and</strong> management<br />

effects on fertility <strong>of</strong> the sow: a descriptive study. Animal <strong>Reproduction</strong> Science 55, 47-61.<br />

Trezona, M. 2003. Welfare update: Dry sow stalls.<br />

http://www.agric.wa.gov.au/progserv/animal/cntnorth/porkserv/pigtales/2000/Oct2000/article08.htm.<br />

Department <strong>of</strong> Agriculture - Western Australia. 2pp.<br />

11


- Outline <strong>of</strong> this thesis -<br />

OUTLINE OF THIS THESIS<br />

A basic knowledge <strong>of</strong> how <strong>group</strong> housing varies in practice is a prerequisite for identifying<br />

important causes for impaired reproduction performance in <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating<br />

sows. A short introduction to <strong>group</strong> housing in practice is therefore provided in How does<br />

<strong>group</strong> housing vary in practice.<br />

With the aim to identify, which processes that may go wrong <strong>and</strong> why, a brief review <strong>of</strong> the<br />

literature <strong>of</strong> reproduction physiology was carried out. This review is presented in Appendix<br />

1.<br />

As discussed in Appendix 1, several studies have indicated that <strong>group</strong> housing may lead to<br />

individual variation in energy intake <strong>and</strong> increased stress. At the same time there are several<br />

indications that energy intake <strong>and</strong> stress may influence the reproduction physiology <strong>of</strong> the<br />

sow. Therefore, two review papers were written to consider whether individual variation in<br />

energy intake (paper I) as well as stress <strong>and</strong> fear (paper II) could be contributing reasons for<br />

the lower reproduction performance in <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> compared to individually <strong>housed</strong> sows<br />

found in some on-farm studies.<br />

Based on these two review papers it was concluded that there are indications that <strong>group</strong><br />

housing practice may lead to individual variation in energy intake, stress <strong>and</strong> fear which<br />

may influence reproduction performance negatively. However, traditional methods for assessing<br />

feed intake, stress <strong>and</strong> fear in sows are not suitable in large-scale on-farm studies or<br />

for the individual farmer, who wants to find out whether level <strong>of</strong> stress, fear <strong>and</strong> feed intake<br />

could be contributing reasons for reproduction problems in <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> sows. Therefore,<br />

a need for indicators <strong>of</strong> stress, fear <strong>and</strong> energy status suitable for use under practical <strong>conditions</strong><br />

was identified. It was possible to define such indicators based upon existing knowledge<br />

<strong>and</strong> these indicators are presented <strong>and</strong> discussed in paper II <strong>and</strong> paper III. However,<br />

whether these indicators are suitable to express variation in sows susceptibility for a good<br />

reproduction performance under practical <strong>conditions</strong> was not known. Therefore a study in<br />

14 herds with different layouts <strong>and</strong> management routines was carried out. The level, the<br />

between-farm <strong>and</strong> within-farm variation in indicators <strong>of</strong> feed intake, fear <strong>and</strong> stress from<br />

the 14 herds are presented <strong>and</strong> discussed in paper III. In paper IV the relation between the<br />

reproduction performance (e.g. litter size <strong>and</strong> pregnancy rate) <strong>and</strong> the indicators <strong>of</strong> feed<br />

intake, stress <strong>and</strong> fear are presented <strong>and</strong> discussed.<br />

In the General discussion, the results from the four papers are discussed <strong>and</strong> put into perspective.<br />

Finally, a general conclusion is put forward in Conclusions.<br />

13


- How does <strong>group</strong> housing vary in practice? -<br />

HOW DOES GROUP HOUSING VARY IN PRACTICE?<br />

A basic knowledge <strong>of</strong> how <strong>group</strong> housing varies in practice is a prerequisite for identifying<br />

important causes for impaired reproduction performance in <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating<br />

sows. In this chapter some <strong>of</strong> the applied housing <strong>conditions</strong> are shortly described. The description<br />

is based on a combination <strong>of</strong> own observations, personal communication with<br />

people who work with pig production in practice (e.g. advisors), literature presented in pig<br />

magazines <strong>and</strong> scientific journals. The description is to a large degree founded on Danish<br />

<strong>conditions</strong>. However, it is aimed also to include experiences from outside Denmark.<br />

Housing <strong>conditions</strong> may e.g. vary with<br />

respect to feeding procedure, timing <strong>of</strong><br />

mixing <strong>of</strong> <strong>group</strong>s, <strong>group</strong> dynamics,<br />

<strong>group</strong> size, stocking rate <strong>and</strong> floor type.<br />

Feeding procedure<br />

In general, feeding procedure can be<br />

divided into two different principles:<br />

Group feeding <strong>and</strong> individual feeding.<br />

Common <strong>group</strong> feeding procedures are<br />

1. Floor feeding, 2. Providing liquid<br />

feed in long feeding troughs with or<br />

without individual troughs dividers <strong>and</strong><br />

3. Bi<strong>of</strong>ix. (Svendsen et al., 1990; Brouns<br />

& Edwards, 1992; Olsson et al., 1993;<br />

Fisker, 1994). In bi<strong>of</strong>ix the feed is provided<br />

in a trough with individual<br />

troughs dividers in a speed that match<br />

the eating rate <strong>of</strong> the sows in order to<br />

ensure that each sow are ‘fixed’ at one<br />

place throughout the feeding (‘biological<br />

fixed’). In bi<strong>of</strong>ix, as in the other<br />

<strong>group</strong> feeding systems differential feeding<br />

within a <strong>group</strong> is not possible. The<br />

<strong>group</strong> feeding procedures mentioned are<br />

illustrated in Figure 1.<br />

Figure 1. Floor feeding, liquid feed in long<br />

feeding troughs <strong>and</strong> bi<strong>of</strong>ix (photos: L.U. Hansen<br />

<strong>and</strong> A.G. Kongsted).<br />

15


- How does <strong>group</strong> housing vary in practice? -<br />

Individual feeding systems employed in practice are Electronic Sow Feeding (ESF), fitmix,<br />

free access feeding stalls (FAFS) <strong>and</strong> individual feeding stalls (IFS) (Brouns & Edwards,<br />

1992; Nielsen et al., 2000; Hansen & Petersen, 2003). In ESF, the sows are provided with<br />

an electronic identification mark. When the sow enters a feeding box the sow are identified<br />

<strong>and</strong> the ration determent to this particular sow is provided automatically. Fitmix is based on<br />

the same principle as ESF but the sow does not stay in a box while eating. In FAFS all sows<br />

are fed at the same time in individual stalls that closes when the sow enters the stall. In<br />

FAFS the sows have permanently access to the stalls in opposite to IFS where the sows<br />

only have access to the feeding stalls during feeding. In IFS, one <strong>group</strong> is feed at a time <strong>and</strong><br />

when the sows from one <strong>group</strong> are finished eating the sows return to their pen <strong>and</strong> the next<br />

<strong>group</strong> is allowed access to the stalls. These individual feeding procedures are shown in Fig-<br />

ure 2.<br />

Figure 2. Electronic Sow Feeding (ESF), fitmix, free access feeding stalls (FAFS) <strong>and</strong> individual feeding<br />

stalls (IFS) (photos: L.U. Hansen).<br />

16


- How does <strong>group</strong> housing vary in practice? -<br />

From weaning until four weeks after mating, individual feeding procedures are preferred,<br />

however, systems with <strong>group</strong> feeding procedures in the entire <strong>non</strong>-lactating period do also<br />

exist (Sørensen & Thorup, 2003; pers. comm. Hansen, 2004, own observations).<br />

Group dynamic/<strong>group</strong> size<br />

A <strong>group</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows may either be stable or dynamic. In stable <strong>group</strong>s, once the<br />

<strong>group</strong> is established no new sows are moved into the <strong>group</strong>. In dynamics <strong>group</strong>s, new sows<br />

are constantly moved into <strong>and</strong> out <strong>of</strong> the <strong>group</strong> e.g. up till once a week (Svendsen et al.,<br />

1990). Group sizes may vary from 2 (Deininger, 1998) to 150 (Nielsen et al., 2000) sows<br />

per <strong>group</strong>. In the service unit the <strong>group</strong>s are <strong>of</strong>ten stable with <strong>group</strong> sizes equal to or less (if<br />

division into smaller <strong>group</strong>s take place at weaning) than the size <strong>of</strong> the farrowing batch.<br />

Mixing <strong>of</strong> unfamiliar sows<br />

Frequently, sows are individually <strong>housed</strong> in the lactating unit why sows usually are mixed<br />

with unfamiliar sows when moved from the lactation unit to the service unit (Edwards,<br />

1992; Deininger, 1998). In some herds the service <strong>and</strong> pregnancy units are integrated. In<br />

these systems sows stay in the same pen from weaning to shortly before farrowing. In many<br />

herds, however, sows are relocated at least once more when moved from the mating to the<br />

pregnancy section. This may happen in from a few hours to three-four weeks (own observations)<br />

after insemination.<br />

Stocking rate<br />

Stocking rates from less than 2 m 2 (Mortensen & Ruby, 1990; Deininger, 1998) to more<br />

than 4 m 2 per sow (Svendsen et al., 1990) or even 5 m 2 per sow (Deininger, 1998) are practised.<br />

Often, the stocking rate in the service unit is less compared to the stocking rate in the<br />

pregnancy unit (pers comm., Hansen 2004) because it is believed that a high stocking rate<br />

in this period may influence reproduction negatively.<br />

Floor type<br />

Systems exists with large ‘open’ floor plan with deep straw bedding, however, the floor<br />

may also be divided into a lying/activity <strong>and</strong> a dunging area (Olsson et al., 1991). The<br />

dunging area may consist <strong>of</strong> slatted or concrete floor. The lying/activity area may consist <strong>of</strong><br />

concrete with or without small amounts <strong>of</strong> straw provided, straw bedding or deep straw<br />

bedding (Deininger, 1998). It is common to use some amount <strong>of</strong> straw in the service unit<br />

because it is believed to minimize the risk <strong>of</strong> leg injuries caused by fighting <strong>and</strong> mounting<br />

during oestrus (Hansen & Kongsted, 2002). Depending upon the system in use, the amount<br />

<strong>of</strong> straw provided may vary from zero (own observations) to 1.000 kg per sow per year<br />

(Svendsen et al., 1990). The lying/activity area may be divided into smaller or larger ‘nests’<br />

by means <strong>of</strong> ‘pen dividers’ as shown in picture 3.<br />

17


- How does <strong>group</strong> housing vary in practice? -<br />

Figure 3. Electronic Sow Feeding with small (left) or large ‘nests’ (photos: L.U. Hansen).<br />

How are the different layout related factors combined?<br />

Group feeding is usually combined with small <strong>group</strong> sizes e.g. six to 20 sows per <strong>group</strong><br />

(own observations) <strong>and</strong> stable <strong>group</strong>s. In opposite, ESF <strong>and</strong> fitmix are <strong>of</strong>ten equal to dynamic<br />

<strong>group</strong>s <strong>and</strong> large <strong>group</strong> sizes e.g. from 50 to 150 sows per <strong>group</strong> (Nielsen et al.,<br />

2000). The feeding procedures FAFS <strong>and</strong> IFS are frequently combined with stable <strong>group</strong>s<br />

with <strong>group</strong> sizes that vary from 6 to 50 sows per <strong>group</strong>; however, it may also be combined<br />

with larger dynamic <strong>group</strong>s (Nielsen et al., 2000). Each <strong>of</strong> the above mentioned feeding<br />

procedures may be combined with each <strong>of</strong> the above mentioned floor types.<br />

Concluding remarks<br />

Group housing <strong>of</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows in practice is not a well-defined system but varies in a<br />

number <strong>of</strong> ways in respect to housing <strong>conditions</strong>. This variation may influence the possibilities<br />

<strong>of</strong> the sows to cope in the system <strong>and</strong> further affect which management options that<br />

can be taken in order to obtain a good reproduction performance.<br />

References<br />

Brouns, F., Edwards, S.A., 1992. Future prospects for housing <strong>of</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows. Pig News <strong>and</strong> Information<br />

13, 47-50.<br />

Deininger, E. 1998. Beeinflussung der aggressiven Ausein<strong>and</strong>ersetzungen beim gruppieren von abgesetzten<br />

sauen durch das haltungssystem und durch <strong>and</strong>ere massnahmen. Veterinär-medizinischen Fakultet<br />

der Universität Zürich. 120pp.<br />

Edwards, S.A., 1992. Scientific perspectives on loose housing systems for dry sows. Pig Veterinary Journal<br />

28, 40-51.<br />

Fisker, B.N., 1994. Løsgående gruppefodrede søer. Meddelelse 278, L<strong>and</strong>sudvalget for Svin, Den rullende<br />

Afprøvning., 10pp.<br />

18


- How does <strong>group</strong> housing vary in practice? -<br />

Hansen, L.U. 2004. Personal communication. Konsulent, Afd. for stalde og produktionssystemer, L<strong>and</strong>sud-<br />

valget for Svin, Danske Slagterier.<br />

Hansen, L.U., Kongsted, A.G., 2002. Gulvudformning i løbeafdeling med æde-/insemineringsbokse til løsgå-<br />

ende søer. Meddelelse 559, Den rullende Afprøvning, L<strong>and</strong>sudvalget for Svin, Danske Slagterier,<br />

8pp.<br />

Hansen, L.U., Petersen, L.B., Løsgående søer. 96-97. 2003. L<strong>and</strong>sudvalget for Svin, Danske Slagterier.<br />

Kongres for Svineproducenter. 28-10-2003.<br />

Mortensen, B., Ruby, V., 1990. Transpondersystemer til drægtige søer. Meddelelse 181, L<strong>and</strong>sudvalget for<br />

Svin, Den rullende Afprøvning, 8pp.<br />

Nielsen, N.-P., Hansen, L.U., Calmann-Hinke, D., 2000. Stalde til løsgående søer. Rapport 17, L<strong>and</strong>sudvalget<br />

for Svin, Danske Slagterier, 33pp.<br />

Olsson, A.-C., Svendsen, J., Reese, D., Andersson, M., Rantzer, D., 1993. Inhysning av dräktiga suggor i<br />

långsmala boxar med blötutfodring. Rapport 87, Sveriges lantbruksuniversitet, Institutionen för lantbrukets<br />

byggnadsteknik, Lund, 39pp.<br />

Svendsen, J., Andersson, M., Olsson, A.-C., Rantzer, D., Lundqvist, P., 1990. Grupphållning av drägtiga<br />

suggor i isolerade och oisolerade stallar. En beskrivning av resultaten från enkätunder - sökningar,<br />

gårdsbesök och grupperingsförsök. Rapport 66, Institutionen för lantbrukets byggnadsteknik, Sveriges<br />

Lantbruksuniversitet, 202pp.<br />

Sørensen, G., Thorup, F., 2003. Energitildeling i implantationsperioden. Meddelelse 618, L<strong>and</strong>sudvalget for<br />

Svin, Danske Slagterier, 7pp.<br />

19


- Paper I -<br />

Effect <strong>of</strong> energy intake on pregnancy rate <strong>and</strong> litter size with particular<br />

reference to <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows - a review<br />

A.G. Kongsted<br />

Department <strong>of</strong> Agroecology, Danish Institute <strong>of</strong> Agricultural Sciences, P.O. Box 50, DK-<br />

8830 Tjele<br />

Submitted to Livestock Production Science<br />

21<br />

I


- Paper I -<br />

Abstract<br />

The number <strong>of</strong> <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows is increasing rapidly in Europe as a consequence<br />

<strong>of</strong> changed legislation initiated by elevated public concern <strong>of</strong> animal welfare. Lower<br />

litter size <strong>and</strong> pregnancy rate in <strong>group</strong> compared to individual <strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows<br />

have been observed in several on-farm experiments. The aim <strong>of</strong> this review is to consider<br />

whether the variation in energy intake in a <strong>group</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows can influence variation<br />

in litter size <strong>and</strong> pregnancy rate in practice. Through a review <strong>of</strong> existing literature with<br />

main emphasis on publications after 1980, the effect <strong>of</strong> energy supply before mating <strong>and</strong> in<br />

pregnancy on pregnancy rate <strong>and</strong> litter size is discussed. The results indicate that low compared<br />

to high energy intake before mating may impair litter size in gilts <strong>and</strong> in sows that<br />

experienced severe weight loss during lactation. Furthermore, it seems that moderate compared<br />

to low energy intake the first three days after mating may reduce litter size in the gilt<br />

but not in the sows. However, very low energy intake the first four weeks in pregnancy may<br />

impair litter size in gilts <strong>and</strong> sows <strong>and</strong> also pregnancy rate in gilts. Whether the last mentioned<br />

is also the case for sows is not possible to conclude. However, it seems that low energy<br />

intake for several successive parities can increase the risk <strong>of</strong> being culled as a consequence<br />

<strong>of</strong> not being pregnant. Based upon studies indicating that low ranking sows may<br />

consume considerably less than high ranking sows (e.g. 50-80%) in <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> systems,<br />

it is suggested that variation in feed intake in a <strong>group</strong> <strong>of</strong> restricted fed pregnant female pigs<br />

may be large enough to influence pregnancy rate <strong>and</strong> litter size.<br />

Keywords: Sow, Gilt, Group housing, Energy intake, <strong>Reproduction</strong>, Litter size, Pregnancy<br />

rate<br />

22


- Paper I -<br />

1. Introduction<br />

The number <strong>of</strong> <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows is increasing rapidly in Europe as a consequence<br />

<strong>of</strong> changed legislation initiated by elevated public concern <strong>of</strong> animal welfare. According<br />

to EU legislation from January 2013, all sows have to be loose-<strong>housed</strong> in smaller or<br />

larger <strong>group</strong>s from four weeks after mating until seven days before expected farrowing<br />

(Council Directive 2001/88/EC amending Directive 91/630/EEC Laying Down Minimum<br />

St<strong>and</strong>ards for the Protection <strong>of</strong> Pigs). In United Kingdom, all sows have to be <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong><br />

in the entire period from weaning to seven days before expected farrowing according to the<br />

national legislation (Mortensen, 1997). In Norway, the sows may only be fixed from three<br />

days before until one week after farrowing <strong>and</strong> in Sweden, it is only allowed to keep sows<br />

in crates for maximal one week if necessary during the production cyclus (Baustad & Lium,<br />

2002). Similar tendencies are seen in other parts <strong>of</strong> the world (Trezona, 2003).<br />

Impaired reproduction in form <strong>of</strong> reduced litter size <strong>and</strong> pregnancy rate in <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong><br />

sows compared to individual <strong>housed</strong> sows in parts <strong>of</strong> or in the entire <strong>non</strong>-lactating period<br />

has been observed in several on-farm studies. The impairment has been in the range <strong>of</strong> 0.3<br />

(Hansen, 2000) to 0.6 (Fisker, 1995) less born piglets per litter, 0.9 %-point lower farrowing<br />

rate (Hurtgen et al., 1980) <strong>and</strong> 3.4 %-point higher repeat breeding rate in autumn<br />

(Peltoniemi et al., 1999). However, in other studies, no difference (Gjein & Larssen, 1995;<br />

Hansen, 2003) or even opposite effects have been found (Bates et al., 2003; Hansen, 2003).<br />

The divergent results are probably a result <strong>of</strong> differences in the function <strong>of</strong> the <strong>group</strong> housing<br />

systems.<br />

Systems with <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows can vary in a number <strong>of</strong> ways, e.g. in <strong>group</strong><br />

size, <strong>group</strong> dynamics, floor type <strong>and</strong> feeding system. A commonly used feeding procedure<br />

is <strong>group</strong> feeding, providing the feed on the floor or in long feeding trough. These feeding<br />

procedures are economical attractive because <strong>of</strong> the low investments costs, however, the<br />

disadvantage is that individual rationing is not possible. A more expensive alternative to<br />

<strong>group</strong> feeding is individual feeding. Examples <strong>of</strong> popular individual feeding procedures are<br />

the Electronic Sow Feeding (ESF) system, where sows are fed automatically in a crate, one<br />

at a time (Brouns & Edwards, 1992), <strong>and</strong> one feeding stall per sow, where all sows are fed<br />

at the same time in individual crates that closes during eating (Nielsen et al., 1997).<br />

In systems with <strong>group</strong> feeding, the individual sow is not protected during feeding from displacements<br />

by other sows <strong>and</strong> studies have indicated lower feed intake in low ranking sows<br />

compared to high ranking sows in form <strong>of</strong> lower weight gain (Brouns & Edwards, 1994;<br />

Ruis et al., 2002), less increase in chest girth (Olsson & Svendsen, 1997), less time spent at<br />

the central area <strong>of</strong> a pile <strong>of</strong> feed provided on the floor (Csermely & Wood-Gush, 1990) <strong>and</strong><br />

23


- Paper I -<br />

less time spent at the trough (Andersen et al., 1999). This indicate that <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> sys-<br />

tems without individual feeding can lead to individual variation in feed intake between<br />

sows. However, even in <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> systems with individual feeding, such as ESF, lower<br />

feed intake in the low <strong>and</strong> middle ranking sows has been indicated (Mendl et al., 1992).<br />

The reason for this might be that the low <strong>and</strong> middle ranking sows do not always eat the<br />

ration allocated because <strong>of</strong> intimidation from sows waiting outside the feeder (Mendl et al.,<br />

1992). Furthermore, experiences from practice show that high ranking sows visit the feed<br />

station several times to lick feed left over, this may make it more difficult for the low ranking<br />

sows to gain access to the feed station (Olsson & Svendsen, 1997).<br />

Sows are <strong>of</strong>ten fed ad libitum or close to ad libitum before mating (flushing) in commercial<br />

practice. In Denmark, for instance, recommendation for energy supply, in the period from<br />

weaning to mating, is 51-77 MJ ME day -1 (The National Committee for Pig Production,<br />

2003a). When feed is provided ad libitum during pregnancy, the low ranking sows have<br />

been found to have comparable feed intake with higher ranking individuals (Brouns & Edwards,<br />

1994). However, in herds with <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows, the sows are usual<br />

mixed with unfamiliar sows after weaning <strong>and</strong> when unfamiliar sows are mixed, fighting<br />

occur until a hierarchy is established (Arey & Edwards, 1998). Mixing with unfamiliar<br />

sows seems to be a stressful condition with increased level <strong>of</strong> plasma cortisol as a consequence<br />

(Barnett et al., 1981; Dalin et al., 1993; Tsuma et al., 1996) especially for the sows<br />

receiving most aggressions (Mendl et al., 1992). The effect <strong>of</strong> stress upon appetite in pigs<br />

has not yet been studied as far as we know. However, there are indications that stress may<br />

reduce appetite in rats (Rodríguez Echanía et al., 1988; Ottenweller et al., 1989). It is therefore<br />

likely that sows involved in many fights will have reduced appetite <strong>and</strong> therefore reduced<br />

feed intake even though the feed is provided ad libitum.<br />

In commercial practice, pregnant sows are usual fed amounts far below their capacity for<br />

feed intake (Brouns et al., 1991). Therefore, pregnant sows are motivated to eat throughout<br />

the day (Jensen et al., 2000) <strong>and</strong> competition for food is a major cause <strong>of</strong> aggressions in<br />

<strong>group</strong> feeding systems (Olsson et al., 1993). As mentioned above, there are indications that<br />

this may lead to an uneven energy intake in low <strong>and</strong> high ranking sows during pregnancy.<br />

For the above-mentioned reasons, it is believed that individual variation in energy supply is<br />

likely to occur in <strong>group</strong> housing systems both before mating <strong>and</strong> in pregnancy. It is generally<br />

agreed that energy intake can influence reproduction related processes in the female pig<br />

(Einarsson & Rojkittikhun, 1993; Cosgrove & Foxcr<strong>of</strong>t, 1996; Foxcr<strong>of</strong>t, 1997; Prunier &<br />

Quesnel, 2000ab). Therefore, one reason for the lower litter size <strong>and</strong> pregnancy rate in<br />

<strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> sows compared to individual <strong>housed</strong> sows seen in some on-farm studies<br />

could be individual differences in energy intake between sows.<br />

24


- Paper I -<br />

Therefore, the aim <strong>of</strong> this review is to consider whether the variation in feed intake in a<br />

<strong>group</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating female pigs can influence the variation in litter size <strong>and</strong> pregnancy<br />

rate in practice.<br />

Emphasis will be upon studies made in the eighties <strong>and</strong> forth. Several studies in the sixties<br />

<strong>and</strong> seventies have examined the effect <strong>of</strong> energy intake on pregnancy rate <strong>and</strong> litter size.<br />

However, intense genetic selection has changed the genotype <strong>of</strong> breeding sows why some<br />

<strong>of</strong> the early work may no longer be relevant to commercial sow herds (Ashworth & Antipatis,<br />

1999).<br />

Pope (1994) <strong>and</strong> Foxcr<strong>of</strong>t (1997) point out that it is important to consider the effect <strong>of</strong> nutrition<br />

in the different reproductive stages <strong>of</strong> the sows separately because the effect in the<br />

different phases can vary. Furthermore, from a practical point <strong>of</strong> view, it is important to<br />

know when the sow’s reproduction is most susceptible to a low or a high energy intake because<br />

only then it is possible to adjust the feeding system in the different stages. This could<br />

be <strong>of</strong> outmost economical importance for the individual farmer because individual feeding<br />

procedures, like for instance one feeding stall per sow, are much more expensive than<br />

<strong>group</strong> feeding procedures, like floor feeding. Therefore, in this review, the effect <strong>of</strong> energy<br />

intake will, whenever possible, be divided into 1) Effect <strong>of</strong> energy intake before mating (in<br />

the oestrus cycle or after weaning) 2) Effect <strong>of</strong> energy intake in the early pregnancy (also<br />

called the embryonic phase i.e. the first 35 days after fertilization) <strong>and</strong> 3) Effect <strong>of</strong> energy<br />

intake in the mid <strong>and</strong> late pregnancy.<br />

2. Effect <strong>of</strong> energy intake before mating<br />

In Table 1, the results from the 12 experiments presented in this chapter are summarized. In<br />

these experiments, the effect <strong>of</strong> energy intake before mating on ovulation rate; number <strong>of</strong><br />

embryos <strong>and</strong>/or pregnancy rate has been studied.<br />

25


Refer<br />

ence<br />

Table 1. Effect <strong>of</strong> energy intake before mating upon ovulation rate, number <strong>of</strong> embryos <strong>and</strong> pregnancy rate<br />

Feed supply, MJ ME day -1 N Parity Daily gain,<br />

1 H: 2.6 kg 1)<br />

L: 0<br />

2 HHHH: 2.3, 2.3, 2.3, 2.3 kg 1)<br />

LLHH: 0, 0, 2.3, 2.3 kg<br />

LLLH: 0, 0, 0, 2.3 kg<br />

LLLL: 0, 0, 0, 0<br />

3 H: 34.3 MJ NE4) L: 21.4 MJ NE<br />

4 H: 44.8<br />

L: 0<br />

5 H: 3.60 kg 1)<br />

L: 1.8 kg<br />

5 L: 1.8 kg 1)<br />

H: 0, 2.7, 1.8 kg<br />

6 H: ad lib<br />

L: 19.3<br />

7 H: 41.7<br />

L: 24.2<br />

36 2)<br />

36 2)<br />

107<br />

in all<br />

37<br />

32<br />

63<br />

65<br />

74<br />

75<br />

98<br />

98<br />

40<br />

40<br />

18<br />

18<br />

g<br />

0 416<br />

1<br />

-636<br />

(After 42 days)<br />

1 450a<br />

-280b<br />

-<br />

Ovulation rate Embryo survival,<br />

11.6a<br />

9.5b<br />

14.8a<br />

13.0<br />

13.3<br />

12.6b<br />

(30 days La.)<br />

15.2<br />

14.8<br />

%<br />

89.7<br />

88.7<br />

80.5<br />

80.7<br />

77.7<br />

77.4<br />

(30 days La.)<br />

Number <strong>of</strong> embryos Pregnancy rate Treatment period<br />

10.4<br />

8.4<br />

11.9/11.8a<br />

10.3/8.0b<br />

10.7/10.5<br />

9.7/11.3<br />

(Embryos<br />

day 25/litter size) 3)<br />

Different - - - 12.0x<br />

11.1y<br />

(Litter size)<br />

Different - - - 10.6<br />

10.4<br />

(Litter size)<br />

Different - - - 10.5<br />

10.4<br />

(Litter size)<br />

1 716<br />

294<br />

14.8a<br />

13.0b<br />

0 - 17.6a<br />

14.0b<br />

100<br />

61<br />

(Remained pregnant)<br />

10 days before ma. until 14, 18, 22, 26, 30 or 34<br />

days a. ma.<br />

- Day 1, 2, 3 <strong>and</strong> 4 a.w.<br />

- - - w.-ma. (9.1,8.2 days)<br />

0.68<br />

0,74<br />

74<br />

80<br />

(60 days after ma.)<br />

10.0<br />

9.5<br />

(Litter size)<br />

12.9x<br />

10.8y<br />

86<br />

90<br />

77<br />

85<br />

(Farrowing rate)<br />

83<br />

80<br />

(Farrowing rate)<br />

On the day <strong>of</strong> w.<br />

w.-6 days a.w.<br />

(Mated in average on day 4.5 a.w.)<br />

w.-f.<br />

w.-2 days a.w., 2 days a.w.-ma., ma.-f.<br />

- w.-ma. (17.1,16.7 days)<br />

73<br />

73<br />

6 days before oestrus until 24 hour after oestrus<br />

To be continued on next page


Refer<br />

ence<br />

Table 1. Effect <strong>of</strong> energy intake before mating upon ovulation rate, number <strong>of</strong> embryos <strong>and</strong> pregnancy rate (continued)<br />

8 H: 46.1<br />

Feed supply, MJ ME day -1 N Parity Daily gain,<br />

L: 22.6<br />

9 HH: ad lib, ad lib<br />

LH: 25, 35<br />

LL: 25, 25<br />

10 HH: 74.4,74.4<br />

HL: 74.4,37.2<br />

LH: 37.2,74.4<br />

LL: 37.2,37.2<br />

8<br />

8<br />

14<br />

13<br />

14<br />

23<br />

22<br />

22<br />

22<br />

g<br />

0 173a<br />

650b<br />

Ovulation<br />

rate<br />

16.0a<br />

9.4b<br />

0 - 12.7a<br />

12.1a<br />

10.7b<br />

2 - 16.6<br />

16.2<br />

17.7<br />

16.7<br />

5)<br />

Embryo survival,<br />

%<br />

Number <strong>of</strong> embryos Pregnancy rate Treatment period<br />

- - - Day 8 <strong>of</strong> second oestrus cycle until the end <strong>of</strong><br />

oestrus cycle<br />

- - - From 45.3 kg BW to puberty <strong>and</strong> from puberty to<br />

second oestrus<br />

77.7ab<br />

85.0a<br />

70.4bc<br />

64.0c<br />

- 86.9a<br />

82.0a<br />

69.0b<br />

62.0b<br />

(Pregnancy rate)<br />

La., w.-ma. (6.6,6.7 days)<br />

11 HH: 44.0, 45.3<br />

22 0 714<br />

17.1<br />

83-6a<br />

14.3<br />

- Day 1-7 <strong>and</strong> day 8-15 in oestrus cycle<br />

HL: 44.0, 34.0<br />

19<br />

642<br />

18.5<br />

68.3b<br />

12.8<br />

LH: 32.7, 45.3<br />

21<br />

595<br />

17.7<br />

81.7a<br />

14.7<br />

(One cyclus)<br />

Note: In some references the level <strong>of</strong> energy is only reported in DE, in those cases ME is calculated as 0.96*DE (Theil et al. (2002))<br />

Ma.: Mating, F.: Farrowing, W.: Weaning, La.: Lactation, A.: After<br />

A, b, c: Values with different superscripts are significant different (P≤0.05). x, y, z: Values with different superscripts tended to be different (P


- Paper I -<br />

Gilts<br />

When it comes to gilts, the effect <strong>of</strong> flushing is confirmed in several studies (Cox et al.,<br />

1987; Flowers et al., 1989; Beltranena et al., 1991). The experiments mentioned, have all<br />

been investigating the effect <strong>of</strong> high (41.7 MJ ME day -1 -ad libitum) contra low (22.6-25 MJ<br />

ME day -1 ) energy intake. A moderate restriction in energy intake (34.0 MJ ME day -1 ) during<br />

day 8-15, but not during day 1-7, in oestrus cycle, seems to impair embryo survival,<br />

however, the number <strong>of</strong> embryos at day 28 in pregnancy were not affected significantly<br />

(Almeida et al., 2000).<br />

The effect <strong>of</strong> more restricted feeding <strong>of</strong> gilts before mating has, as far as we know, not been<br />

studied in the recent years but Anderson (1975), who investigated the effect <strong>of</strong> total starvation<br />

<strong>of</strong> gilts in ten days before mating, found that the ovulation rate was reduced compared<br />

to control animals given a full diet (2.7 kg). The question is, however, whether this lower<br />

ovulation rate will result in lower litter size because a positive correlation between the ovulation<br />

rate <strong>and</strong> the embryo mortality seems to exist (Toplis et al., 1983). In the study by Cox<br />

et al. (1987), the number <strong>of</strong> fetuses 60 days after mating tended to be decreased in the sows<br />

fed the low energy intake (P


- Paper I -<br />

Therefore, low energy supply compared to flushing does not seem to have any effect when<br />

looking isolated at the period from weaning to mating. Whether it has an effect in sows that<br />

experience severe weight loss during lactation is difficult to say because the results are inconsistent.<br />

King & Williams (1984) did not find any significant interactions between feed<br />

intake in lactation (25 MJ ME day -1 or ad libitum) <strong>and</strong> feed intake from weaning to mating<br />

(19.3 or 50.1 MJ ME day -1 ) on litter size in first litter sows even though the restricted fed<br />

sows had a weight loss <strong>of</strong> 37 kg during lactation. However, Baidoo et al. (1992) found a<br />

significant interaction between feed intake during lactation <strong>and</strong> feed intake after weaning in<br />

second parity sows because low feed supply after weaning reduced embryo survival only in<br />

sows fed restricted during lactation.<br />

Nielsen et al. (1981), reported that fasting compared to 44.8 MJ ME on the day <strong>of</strong> weaning<br />

resulted in a <strong>non</strong>-significant (P=0.08) smaller litter size. In contrast, Tribble & Orr (1982)<br />

were not able to find any effect <strong>of</strong> fasting two days after weaning. Perhaps the lacking effect<br />

in Tribble & Orr (1982) is due to a shorter lactation length (18-38 days compared to<br />

28-56 days in Nielsen et al. (1981) since Allrich et al. (1979) only found an effect <strong>of</strong> starvation<br />

after mating on litter size in sows lactating for 30 days <strong>and</strong> not in sows lactating for 21<br />

days. Weight loss during lactation is not reported in the mentioned studies but it is however<br />

likely that weight loss is higher, the longer lactation length, why these results confirm that<br />

the effect <strong>of</strong> low feed intake after mating depends upon the condition <strong>of</strong> the sow at weaning.<br />

All in all, there seems to be no evidence <strong>of</strong> an effect <strong>of</strong> low energy intake (19.3-37.2 MJ<br />

ME day -1 ) compared to flushing (ad lib or close to ad lib) from weaning to mating on ovulation<br />

rate in sows. However, it is possible that low feed intake (37.2 MJ ME day -1 ) has a<br />

negative effect on litter size in sows that experienced severe weight loss (e.g. 39 kg) during<br />

lactation. Pregnancy rate does not seem to be influenced by energy intake before mating.<br />

3. Effect <strong>of</strong> energy intake in early pregnancy<br />

In Table 2, the results from the 13 experiments presented in this chapter are summarized. In<br />

these experiments, the effect <strong>of</strong> energy intake in early pregnancy on number <strong>of</strong> embryos<br />

<strong>and</strong>/or pregnancy rate has been studied.<br />

29


Table 2. Effect <strong>of</strong> energy intake in early pregnancy upon number <strong>of</strong> embryos <strong>and</strong> pregnancy rate<br />

Refe-<br />

rence<br />

1 H: 2.7 kg 1)<br />

L: 0<br />

2 HH: 29.4, 29.4<br />

HL: 29.4, 17.6<br />

LH: 17.6, 29.4<br />

LL: 17.6, 17.6<br />

3 L: 25,0<br />

H: 50,1<br />

4 H: 23.09<br />

M: 16.84<br />

L: 10.58<br />

5 HH: 72.2, 43.3<br />

HL: 72.2, 21.6<br />

LH: 36.1, 43.3<br />

LL: 36.1, 21.6<br />

Level in treatments,<br />

MJ ME day -1<br />

6 L: 15.7 (Slaughtered a. 30 days)<br />

H: 31.4 (Slaughtered a. 30 days)<br />

L: 15.7 (Slaughtered a. 60 days)<br />

H: 31.4 (Slaughtered a. 60 days)<br />

L: 15.7 (Slaughtered a. Fa.)<br />

H: 31.4 (Slaughtered a. Fa.)<br />

N Parity Daily gain, g Ovulation rate Embryo survival,<br />

36<br />

48<br />

31<br />

29<br />

31<br />

28<br />

16<br />

18<br />

24<br />

23<br />

22<br />

28<br />

29<br />

25<br />

22<br />

24<br />

25<br />

25<br />

27<br />

84<br />

79<br />

0 416<br />

-636<br />

(After 44 days)<br />

0 0,42a<br />

0,15b<br />

0,36a<br />

0,12b<br />

6-7 313a<br />

757b<br />

4.9 2) -70<br />

-34<br />

-86<br />

2 280<br />

164<br />

120<br />

72<br />

0 -30<br />

710<br />

30<br />

750<br />

10<br />

720<br />

11.6a<br />

9.5b<br />

12,5<br />

12,5<br />

13,2<br />

12,0<br />

23.38<br />

23.89<br />

%<br />

89.7<br />

88.7<br />

75,8<br />

76,9<br />

85,4<br />

86,7<br />

LL+LH: 86.2a<br />

HH+HL: 75.2b<br />

17.58<br />

16.76<br />

Number <strong>of</strong> embryos Pregnancy rate Treatment period<br />

10.4<br />

8.4<br />

9,5<br />

9,6<br />

11,2<br />

10,5<br />

LL+LH: 11.0a<br />

HH+HL: 9.5b<br />

0.75<br />

0.72<br />

- - 11.33<br />

10.57<br />

10.73<br />

(Litter size)<br />

- 80.6a<br />

84.6ac<br />

75.8ad<br />

67.4b<br />

13,6<br />

13,4<br />

13,4<br />

12,7<br />

82,5<br />

85,1<br />

82,4<br />

81,8<br />

13.8a<br />

14.5ac<br />

13.1ad<br />

11.5b<br />

11,2<br />

11,6<br />

11,2<br />

10,3<br />

10,5<br />

10,4<br />

100<br />

61<br />

(Remained pregnant)<br />

87.1a<br />

86.2a<br />

87.1a<br />

64.3b<br />

10 days before ma. Until 14,<br />

18, 22, 26, 30 or 34 days<br />

a.ma.<br />

1-10 <strong>and</strong> 11 – 30/35 days<br />

a.ma.<br />

- 3-30 days a.ma.<br />

75<br />

72<br />

69<br />

(Farrowing rate)<br />

82<br />

97<br />

76<br />

77<br />

1-10 days a.ma.<br />

La., 0-25 days a.ma.<br />

- 0-10 days a.ma.<br />

To be continued on next page


Table 2. Effect <strong>of</strong> energy intake in early pregnancy upon number <strong>of</strong> embryos <strong>and</strong> pregnancy rate (continued)<br />

Refe-<br />

rence<br />

Level in treatments,<br />

MJ ME day -1<br />

7 Ll: 23.6 (P: 7.8 g /BW kg 0.75 )<br />

Lh: 23.6 (P: 13.0 g/BW kg 0.75 )<br />

Hl: 38.4 (P: 7.8 g/BW kg 0.75 )<br />

Hh: 38.4 (P: 13.0 g/BW kg 0.75 ) 3)<br />

8 L: 21.4<br />

H: 34.6<br />

9 L: 22.6<br />

H: 33.9<br />

9 L: 20.6<br />

H: 33.9<br />

10 L: 23.0<br />

M: 29.4<br />

H: 38.4<br />

11 L1: 22.8<br />

L2: 22.8<br />

H: 31.3<br />

N Parity Daily gain, g Ovulation rate Embryo survival,<br />

Number <strong>of</strong> embryos Pregnancy rate Treatment period<br />

12 0 216a<br />

13.7<br />

%<br />

- 12.2<br />

- 3-15 days a.ma.<br />

12<br />

200a<br />

15.1<br />

12.8<br />

12<br />

758b<br />

14.2<br />

12.4<br />

12<br />

750b<br />

14.8<br />

11.6<br />

91 0 - - 76.4<br />

78.6<br />

- - 0 - 9/10/11 days a.ma.<br />

36<br />

36<br />

34<br />

34<br />

614<br />

634<br />

631<br />

(Litter)<br />

24<br />

22<br />

21<br />

0 400a<br />

700b<br />

0 310a<br />

560b<br />

Gilts &<br />

sows<br />

0 657<br />

760<br />

830<br />

13.6<br />

13.5<br />

13.9 4)<br />

13.9<br />

71.,3a<br />

83.7b<br />

79.1<br />

78.4<br />

9.6a<br />

11.2b<br />

11.0<br />

10.9<br />

- - - 11.6<br />

11.7<br />

11.9<br />

(Litter size)<br />

14.50<br />

14.95<br />

14.95<br />

85.93a<br />

77.35ab<br />

66.96b<br />

12 L: 13<br />

369 Sows -321<br />

- - 14.9a<br />

M: 31<br />

378<br />

285<br />

15.1a<br />

H: 49<br />

364<br />

1000<br />

15.4b<br />

(Litter size)<br />

Note: In some references the level <strong>of</strong> energy is only reported as DE, in those cases ME is calculated as 0.96*DE (Theil et al. (2002))<br />

Ma: Mating, F: Farrowing, W: Weaning, La: Lactation, A: After<br />

12.25<br />

11.04<br />

9.80<br />

- 3-30 days a.ma.<br />

- 3-24 days a.ma.<br />

85.9<br />

88.4<br />

88.4<br />

(Farrowing rate)<br />

1-28 days a.ma.<br />

- L1: 1-15 days a.ma.<br />

L2: 3-15 days a.ma.<br />

H: 1-15 days a.ma.<br />

86.4<br />

88.5<br />

86.9<br />

(Farrowing rate)<br />

1-28 days a.ma.<br />

a, b, c: Values with different superscripts are significant different (P≤0.05). x,y, z: Values with different superscripts tended to be different (P


- Paper I -<br />

Gilts<br />

Moderate energy supply (29.4 MJ ME day -1 ) compared to low energy supply (17.6 MJ ME<br />

day -1 ) both from mating until 30/35 days after mating <strong>and</strong> from mating until ten days after<br />

mating increased the embryonic mortality <strong>and</strong> reduced the number <strong>of</strong> embryos in gilts<br />

(Dyck & Strain, 1983). Some authors argue that a lower embryo survival in gilts fed moderate<br />

energy intake could be due to the ovulation rate being increased <strong>and</strong> hence, the embryo<br />

mortality as a result <strong>of</strong> the moderate energy intake immediately after mating (Toplis et<br />

al., 1983). However, in the present study, the ovulation rate did not differ between the treatments.<br />

Therefore, this could probably not be the explanation. Jindal et al. (1996) also found<br />

that moderate energy supply in gilts the first 15 days after mating resulted in a significant<br />

lower embryo survival <strong>and</strong> a <strong>non</strong>-significant lower number <strong>of</strong> viable embryos at day 25-30<br />

compared to low energy supply. These authors also found an inverse relationship between<br />

plane <strong>of</strong> nutrition <strong>and</strong> circulating progesterone concentrations. Furthermore they observed<br />

that the <strong>group</strong> <strong>of</strong> gilts with the greatest average plasma progesterone concentration had the<br />

greatest embryonal survival. Similarly, Pharazyn et al. (1991) observed that overall plasma<br />

progesterone concentrations on day three after oestrus were positively related to embryo<br />

survival. Perhaps increases in energy intake leading to rapid gains will result in reduced<br />

embryo survival because <strong>of</strong> an increase in metabolic clearance <strong>of</strong> progesterone as a<br />

consequence <strong>of</strong> increased hepatic blood flow (Einarsson & Rojkittikhun, 1993)<br />

Reduced levels <strong>of</strong> energy from day three to day 15 <strong>of</strong> gestation did not have an effect on the<br />

number <strong>of</strong> embryos in the uterus at day 28 (Pharazyn et al., 1991). Similarly, Jindal et al.<br />

(1996) found that low energy supply from day three after mating until day 15 does not effect<br />

embryo survival. Therefore, these authors suggest that a reduction in feed intake has a<br />

positive effect but only if it occurs in the first days after onset <strong>of</strong> oestrus. This is in agreement<br />

with other studies beginning treatment three days after mating <strong>and</strong> finding no effect<br />

(Liao & Veum, 1994) or even a negative effect (Liao & Veum, 1994) <strong>of</strong> low compared to<br />

moderate energy supply.<br />

In the light <strong>of</strong> the above-mentioned results, it seems that a low feed intake (17.6/22.8 MJ<br />

ME day -1 ) the first days after mating has a positive effect on reproduction performance<br />

compared to moderate feed intake (29.4/31.3 MJ ME day -1 ). However, in other studies with<br />

treatment beginning immediately after mating, no positive effect <strong>of</strong> low energy supply<br />

(15.7/21.4 MJ ME day -1 ) was found compared to moderate energy supply (31.4/34.6 MJ<br />

ME day -1 ) on embryo survival (Dyck, 1991; Cassar & King, 1992) or number <strong>of</strong> foetuses<br />

(Dyck, 1991). So, the results are inconsistent. Foxcr<strong>of</strong>t (1997) argues that the inconsistency<br />

in much <strong>of</strong> the literature regarding effect <strong>of</strong> energy intake on embryo survival could be due<br />

to differences in number <strong>of</strong> animals used <strong>and</strong> level <strong>of</strong> embryo survival in the control <strong>group</strong>.<br />

32


- Paper I -<br />

However, this does not seem to be a possible explanation for the conflicting results in the<br />

mentioned studies given in Table 2.<br />

Anderson (1975) studied the effect <strong>of</strong> total starvation from ten days before mating to respectively<br />

14, 18, 22, 26, 30 <strong>and</strong> 34 days after mating <strong>and</strong> found no differences in embryo<br />

survival rate in the sows that remained pregnant compared to control animals given a full<br />

diet (2.7 kg). The number <strong>of</strong> embryos was lower in the starved sows but this was because <strong>of</strong><br />

a lower ovulation rate <strong>and</strong> therefore probably founded before mating.<br />

The effect <strong>of</strong> energy intake in early pregnancy on pregnancy rate has not been subject for<br />

much research. Dyck & Strain (1983) found that the <strong>group</strong> with low energy supply (17.6<br />

MJ ME day -1 ) in the entire period from mating to 30/35 days after mating had lower pregnancy<br />

rate (64.3%) than the other <strong>group</strong>s (86.2-87.1%). This was not the case for sows with<br />

low energy supply only in the first ten days <strong>of</strong> pregnancy. The conception failure occurred<br />

after day ten <strong>and</strong> before day 30 <strong>and</strong> the authors therefore hypothesized that the conception<br />

failure was due to failure <strong>of</strong> either early embryonic growth or implantation.<br />

It has not been possible to find more recent references about the effect <strong>of</strong> energy intake in<br />

early pregnancy on conception rate. However, Anderson (1975) investigated the effect <strong>of</strong><br />

total inanition in gilts from ten days before mating until 14, 18, 22, 26, 30 <strong>and</strong> 34 days respectively<br />

after mating. They found that 22 <strong>of</strong> 36 gilts (61 %) remained pregnant. Of these<br />

gilts, six out <strong>of</strong> six (100 %) remained pregnant after day 14 after mating whereas 13 out <strong>of</strong><br />

18 (72 %) remained pregnant when inanition continued to day 18, 22 <strong>and</strong> 26 after mating<br />

<strong>and</strong> inanition longer than that resulted in a pregnancy rate <strong>of</strong> 25 % (three <strong>of</strong> 12 gilts).<br />

In summary, there are indications that moderate (31.3 MJ ME day -1 ) compared to low<br />

(17.6-22.8 MJ ME day -1 ) energy supply in the first three days after mating can influence<br />

embryo survival negatively in gilts. However, the results are conflicting. The effect <strong>of</strong> very<br />

low feed intake in early pregnancy in gilts has almost not been studied, but one study did<br />

not find a negative effect <strong>of</strong> total starvation compared to moderate feed intake in early<br />

pregnancy (34 days) on embryo survival rate in the sows that remained pregnant. Regarding<br />

pregnancy rate, it seems that low energy intake (17.6 MJ ME day -1 or less) compared to<br />

moderate energy supply (29.4 MJ ME day -1 , 2.7 kg) the first 30/35 days in pregnancy may<br />

have a negative effect.<br />

Sows<br />

High energy supply (50.1, 38.4, 48.6 MJ ME day -1 ) when treatment beginning three days<br />

after mating (Toplis et al., 1983) or immediately after mating (Sørensen, 1994) does not<br />

33


- Paper I -<br />

seem to impair the number <strong>of</strong> embryos (Toplis et al., 1983), litter size or farrowing rate<br />

(Sørensen, 1994; Sørensen & Thorup, 2003) in sows.<br />

When looking isolated at energy intake in pregnancy, there is apparently no effect <strong>of</strong> low<br />

energy intake (21.6, 23.0 MJ ME day -1 ) the first four weeks on the number <strong>of</strong> embryos<br />

(Kirkwood et al., 1990), litter size or farrowing rate (Sørensen, 1994). However, this is perhaps<br />

only the case when sows are in a good condition at weaning because Kirkwood et al.<br />

(1990) found a lower number <strong>of</strong> embryos in sows restricted fed during lactation (36.1 MJ<br />

ME day -1 ) <strong>and</strong> the first 25 days <strong>of</strong> pregnancy (21.6 MJ ME day -1 ) compared to sows fed<br />

close to ad libitum in lactation <strong>and</strong> restricted fed the first 25 days <strong>of</strong> pregnancy.<br />

Very low feed intake (10.6 MJ ME day -1 ) day one to day ten in pregnancy did not affect the<br />

number <strong>of</strong> piglets born (total or alive) or the farrowing rate (Dyck & Cole, 1986). However,<br />

as found in the study by Dyck & Strain (1983), it seems that low feed intake only has an<br />

effect after day ten <strong>of</strong> pregnancy, so perhaps a longer period <strong>of</strong> very low energy intake<br />

would have influenced the farrowing rate in sows. This is confirmed in a study by Sørensen<br />

& Thorup (2003) who found that very low energy intake (13 compared to 49 MJ ME day -1 )<br />

the first 28 days in pregnancy, reduced the litter size significantly.<br />

All in all, there seems to be no evidence that high (≥38.4 MJ ME day -1 ) energy intake in the<br />

first 28 days <strong>of</strong> pregnancy impair the litter size in sows. It seems that low energy intake<br />

(21.6 MJ ME day -1 ) the first four weeks <strong>of</strong> pregnancy may impair the litter size only in<br />

sows fed very restricted during lactation. Very low energy intake (10.6 MJ ME day -1 ) the<br />

first ten days in pregnancy does apparently not impair the litter size or the pregnancy rate<br />

but very low energy intake (13 MJ ME day -1 ) for a longer period (28 days) may impair the<br />

litter size.<br />

4. Effect <strong>of</strong> energy intake in mid-/late pregnancy <strong>and</strong> in several successive pregnancies<br />

In Table 3, the results from the seven studies presented in this chapter are summarized. In<br />

these experiments, the effect <strong>of</strong> energy intake in mid- <strong>and</strong> late pregnancy <strong>and</strong> in several<br />

successive pregnancies on number <strong>of</strong> embryos; litter size <strong>and</strong>/or culling rate has been studied.<br />

34


Table 3. Effect <strong>of</strong> energy intake in mid- <strong>and</strong> late pregnancy <strong>and</strong> in several successive pregnancies upon number <strong>of</strong> embryos <strong>and</strong> pregnancy rate<br />

Refe-<br />

rence<br />

1 H: 30.5<br />

Level in treatments, MJ ME day -1 N Daily gain, g Number <strong>of</strong> embryos Pregnancy rate Treatment period<br />

L1: 24.4<br />

ML2: 24.4→ 18.3 1)<br />

L2: 18.3<br />

2 H: 37.2<br />

M: 26.7<br />

L: 16.2<br />

3 M1: 2.3 kg 3)<br />

M2: 2.0 kg<br />

L: 1.7 kg<br />

4 M: 28.8<br />

L: 23.8<br />

4 H: 35.2<br />

M: 31.9<br />

L: 28.8<br />

23<br />

23<br />

22<br />

23<br />

9<br />

9<br />

8<br />

20<br />

20<br />

20<br />

160<br />

164<br />

163<br />

153<br />

150<br />

- 8.3 2)<br />

9.0<br />

8.2<br />

8.8<br />

(Litter size)<br />

41.4a<br />

27.6b<br />

2.3c<br />

11.5<br />

11.1<br />

10.7<br />

(Litter size)<br />

- H: 10.8<br />

M: 10.4<br />

L: 11.1<br />

(Litter size)<br />

11.9-47.9<br />

2.4-39.8<br />

(Pregnancy)<br />

59.3-40.7<br />

53.7-32.5<br />

47.6-16.3<br />

(Pregnancy)<br />

9.5a<br />

10.0b<br />

(Litter size)<br />

10.3<br />

10.7<br />

10.6<br />

(Litter size)<br />

- 2 days a.w.-f<br />

Four successive gestations<br />

- Day 43-114 in pregnancy<br />

- Five successive gestations<br />

- Five successive gestations<br />

- Six successive gestations<br />

To be continued on next page


Table 3. Effect <strong>of</strong> energy intake in mid- <strong>and</strong> late pregnancy <strong>and</strong> in several successive pregnancies upon number <strong>of</strong> embryos <strong>and</strong> pregnancy rate (continued)<br />

Refe-<br />

rence<br />

5 H: 34.6<br />

Level in treatments, MJ ME day -1 N Daily gain, g Number <strong>of</strong> embryos Pregnancy rate Treatment period<br />

M: 28.1<br />

L: 21.4<br />

61<br />

63<br />

63<br />

- 11.2 4)<br />

10.8<br />

11.6<br />

More culled sows in L (main reasons: not<br />

pregnant <strong>and</strong> aborted)<br />

1 day a.w.-109 days a.ma.<br />

Four successive gestations<br />

6 H: 38.5<br />

48<br />

58-65a<br />

13.6<br />

- Two successive gestations<br />

L: 22.2<br />

48<br />

21-25b<br />

12.3<br />

(Pregnancy)<br />

(First parity)<br />

Note: In some references the level <strong>of</strong> energy is only reported as DE, in those cases ME is calculated as 0.96*DE (Theil et al. (2002))<br />

Ma.: Mating, F: Farrowing, W: Weaning, La: Lactation, A: After<br />

a, b, c: Values with different superscripts are significant different (P≤0.05). x,y, z: Values with different superscripts tended to be different (P


- Paper I -<br />

Feeding sows 16, 27 or 37 MJ ME day -1 from day 43 to day 114 in pregnancy did not influ-<br />

ence the litter size (Merk & Kirchgessner, 1984) <strong>and</strong> in accordance with this, Einarsson &<br />

Rojkittikhun (1993) suggest that feed level <strong>of</strong> pregnant sows after the first four weeks has<br />

little effect on litter size.<br />

Litter size seems to be unaffected even by low energy supply (e.g. 18.3 MJ ME) during<br />

pregnancy for several parities (Walker, 1983; Whittemore et al., 1984), though, the results<br />

should be treated with caution due to a relative low number <strong>of</strong> sows per treatment <strong>group</strong> (23<br />

followed in four gestations, 20 followed in five gestations). However, also in larger scale<br />

studies, no convincing effect <strong>of</strong> low energy supply for two (Spoolder et al., 1996), four<br />

(Young et al., 1990) or five (Gatel et al., 1987) parities on litter size has been observed. In<br />

some trials, a higher culling rate was seen among the low fed sows (Walker, 1983;<br />

Whittemore et al., 1984; Young et al., 1990) <strong>and</strong> when the culling reason was reported,<br />

main reasons were reproductive problems like for instance not pregnant <strong>and</strong> abortions<br />

(Young et al., 1990). In contrast, Gatel et al. (1987) found a slightly higher culling rate for<br />

less restricted fed sows (35.2 MJ ME day -1 ) but this was primarily due to anoestrus after<br />

weaning <strong>and</strong> leg problems. Locomotion problems are a well-known consequence <strong>of</strong> high<br />

feed intake during pregnancy (Dourmad et al., 1994). In practice, this could imply consequences<br />

for litter size <strong>and</strong> pregnancy rate because a higher culling rate will lead to a higher<br />

proportion <strong>of</strong> first-litter sows in the herd <strong>and</strong> since younger sows are less prolific than older<br />

sows, this would lead to a reduction in average reproduction performance.<br />

There are several reports <strong>of</strong> a negative relationship between feed intake during pregnancy<br />

<strong>and</strong> feed intake in lactation (e.g.Yang et al., 1989; Young et al., 1990; Xue et al., 1997) <strong>and</strong><br />

a low feed intake in lactation may reduce ovulation rate <strong>and</strong> embryo survival (Zak et al.,<br />

1997; Han et al., 2000) <strong>and</strong> perhaps even conception rate (Hughes et al., 1984) in the next<br />

cyclus. The question is, however, how high feed intake in pregnancy should be before feed<br />

intake in lactation is reduced so much that litter size <strong>and</strong> conception rate is impaired. In the<br />

above-mentioned long-term trials, the level <strong>of</strong> feed intake in pregnancy was in the range<br />

from low level (18.3-22.2 MJ ME per day) to moderate level (30.5-38.5 MJ ME per day)<br />

<strong>and</strong> in these trials, apparently no negative effect <strong>of</strong> the moderate level <strong>of</strong> feed intake on<br />

litter size was observed in the subsequent cyclus. However, whether higher feed intake during<br />

pregnancy can influence litter size negatively by reducing feed intake in lactation, is not<br />

possible to conclude based on these studies.<br />

In summary, when looking isolated upon one cyclus, energy supply after the first four<br />

weeks <strong>of</strong> pregnancy is believed to have little effect on reproduction performance. Regarding<br />

effect <strong>of</strong> energy intake in several successive pregnancies on reproduction, low energy<br />

intake (21.4 MJ ME day -1 ) for up till five parities does not seem to reduce litter size. However,<br />

low energy intake in several gestations may increase the risk <strong>of</strong> being culled due to<br />

37


- Paper I -<br />

pregnancy failure. There is a negative correlation between feed intake during gestation <strong>and</strong><br />

feed intake during lactation, for which reason very high feed intake during gestation may<br />

lead to low energy intake in lactation <strong>and</strong> thereby perhaps a lower litter size <strong>and</strong> pregnancy<br />

rate in the following cyclus. However, how high the feed intake in pregnancy shall be be-<br />

fore reproduction in the next cyclus is impaired, is not possible to say.<br />

5. Discussion<br />

The results reviewed do not provide a clear picture <strong>of</strong> the relation between energy supply<br />

<strong>and</strong> reproduction in female pigs because the results are <strong>of</strong>ten inconsistent. It is therefore<br />

difficult to draw unambiguous conclusions, however, some lines can be drawn.<br />

First <strong>of</strong> all, it seems that the reproduction performance <strong>of</strong> gilts in general is more influenced<br />

by energy supply than the reproduction performance <strong>of</strong> the sow. There are indications that<br />

ovulation rate <strong>and</strong> thereby litter size can be impaired in gilts if they are fed restricted (≤25<br />

MJ ME day -1 ) before mating. When it comes to the sow, the picture is much less clear.<br />

Energy intake from weaning to mating seems to have little influence on litter size although<br />

moderate energy intake (37 compared to 74 MJ ME day -1 ) did impair litter size in sows that<br />

experienced severe weight loss (39 kg) during lactation in one study (Baidoo et al., 1992).<br />

There are indications that litter size in gilts can be reduced if they are fed moderate (≥31 MJ<br />

ME day -1 ) compared to low (18-23 MJ ME day -1 ) level <strong>of</strong> energy the first three days after<br />

mating. Whether this is also the case for the sow has not been confirmed. The number <strong>of</strong><br />

born piglets per litter seems to be highly resistant to even starvation in early pregnancy in<br />

some female pigs, however, there are indications that very low energy intake (≤13 MJ ME<br />

day -1 ) the first four weeks in pregnancy may impair litter size. Less restricted energy intake<br />

(21 MJ ME day -1 ) in early pregnancy may also reduce the litter size in sows fed very restricted<br />

(36 MJ ME day -1 ) during lactation. Pregnancy rate in gilts can apparently be reduced<br />

if they are fed very restricted (≤18 MJ ME day -1 ) the first 35 days <strong>of</strong> pregnancy.<br />

Whether this is the same for sows is not possible to conclude, however, it seems that low<br />

energy intake for several successive parities can increase the risk <strong>of</strong> being culled as a consequence<br />

<strong>of</strong> not being pregnant. Furthermore, it cannot be excluded that high feed intake<br />

during pregnancy reduces voluntary feed intake in lactation <strong>and</strong> thereby reduce the litter<br />

size in the following cyclus.<br />

The existence <strong>of</strong> a relation between energy intake <strong>and</strong> litter size <strong>and</strong> pregnancy rate is further<br />

supported by studies showing an effect <strong>of</strong> energy related hormones, e.g. insulin <strong>and</strong><br />

IGF-1, on the hormonal control <strong>of</strong> litter size <strong>and</strong> pregnancy. The increase in ovulation rate<br />

seen in gilts as a consequence <strong>of</strong> flushing before oestrus is thus believed to be mediated<br />

through an increase in insulin <strong>and</strong> IGF-1 levels followed by an increase in plasma levels <strong>of</strong><br />

38


- Paper I -<br />

gonadotrophins (LH, FSH) (Hughes & Pearce, 1989). Both LH <strong>and</strong> FSH act on the ovaries<br />

to stimulate the development <strong>of</strong> the pre-ovulatory follicle (Foxcr<strong>of</strong>t & Hunter, 1985). LH is<br />

furthermore essential for the maintenance <strong>of</strong> early pregnancy in the pig (Peltoniemi et al.,<br />

1995) why lower pregnancy rate as a consequence <strong>of</strong> restricted feed intake may be due to a<br />

reduction in LH pulse frequency (Peltoniemi et al., 2000). The negative effect <strong>of</strong> high feed<br />

intake the first three days in pregnancy on embryo survival is believed to be caused by an<br />

increase in hepatic blood flow <strong>and</strong> metabolic clearance rate <strong>of</strong> progesterone, as a consequence<br />

<strong>of</strong> rapid weight gain, followed by a decrease in plasma level <strong>of</strong> progesterone<br />

(Hughes & Pearce, 1989; Foxcr<strong>of</strong>t, 1997). Progesterone is the primary director <strong>of</strong> uterine<br />

development <strong>and</strong> secretion (Geisert & Yelich, 1997) <strong>and</strong> therefore a change in plasma level<br />

<strong>of</strong> progesterone could imply detrimental consequences for embryo survival as indicated in a<br />

study by Pharazyn et al. (1991). Progesterone is secreted from Corpus Luteum <strong>and</strong> because<br />

the pig is polyovulatory, it is suggested that it is unlikely that the plasma progesterone concentration<br />

may get below some essential threshold after the first three to four days <strong>of</strong> pregnancy<br />

(Foxcr<strong>of</strong>t, 1997).<br />

The statement, that reproduction may be influenced by nutrition is further supported by<br />

observations <strong>of</strong> wild sows. Matschke (1964 q.f. Graves, 1984) reported that European wild<br />

sows were anoestrous during years <strong>of</strong> small quantity <strong>of</strong> mast <strong>and</strong> similar did Mauget (1981)<br />

report that the timing <strong>of</strong> breeding season was influenced by the amount <strong>of</strong> mast available.<br />

When looking upon the results from the studies reviewed, although it does not provide a<br />

clear picture, it seems that pregnancy rate <strong>and</strong> litter size can be influenced by energy intake<br />

in the <strong>non</strong>-lactative period. However, the question, which remains to be answered, is<br />

whether variation in energy intake between female pigs in commercial <strong>group</strong>-<strong>housed</strong> systems<br />

reaches magnitudes large enough to impair the litter size <strong>and</strong> pregnancy rate?<br />

Andersen et al. (1999) observed that low ranking sows only spent about half as much time<br />

at the trough at feeding compared to high ranking sows (40 vs. 90 % <strong>of</strong> observations) in a<br />

<strong>group</strong> <strong>of</strong> pregnant sows. Although low ranking sows may be able to increase their eating<br />

rate when competition for feed is high, as suggested by Brouns & Edwards (1994), it is<br />

likely that the energy intake <strong>of</strong> the low ranking sows has been considerably lower than the<br />

energy intake <strong>of</strong> the high ranking sows. Mendl et al. (1992) observed that low <strong>and</strong> <strong>non</strong>e<br />

success primaparous sows (the sows were divided into three <strong>group</strong>s according to their ability<br />

to displace other sows in agonistic interactions) had significant lower weight gain than<br />

high success primaparous sows (approximately 7, 9 <strong>and</strong> 19 kg, respectively in one month)<br />

in their 7 th week <strong>of</strong> pregnancy. As Mendl et al. (1992) point out, the lower weight gain in<br />

the low ranking sows is not necessarily a result <strong>of</strong> lower feed intake only but perhaps a<br />

combination <strong>of</strong> lower feed intake <strong>and</strong> an elevated expenditure <strong>of</strong> energy for maintenance as<br />

39


- Paper I -<br />

a consequence <strong>of</strong> stress. However, no matter what may have caused the lower weight gain,<br />

the result would be less energy substrates available for the physiological processes related<br />

to reproduction. In the study by Brouns & Edwards (1994), the weight gain throughout<br />

gestation in the low ranking sows was only 60% (28.3 vs. 46.6 kg) <strong>and</strong> 50% (22.4 vs. 44.9<br />

kg) <strong>of</strong> the weight gain <strong>of</strong> the high ranking sows, respectively in two different experimental<br />

pens. The total gain <strong>of</strong> 44.9 kg <strong>and</strong> 22.4 kg during an entire gestation corresponds with approximately<br />

0.390 kg <strong>and</strong> 0.195 kg daily gain, respectively. The <strong>group</strong>s consisted <strong>of</strong> multiparaous<br />

sows <strong>and</strong> the ranking order was significantly correlated with initial live weight <strong>of</strong><br />

the sows (Brouns & Edwards, 1994). According to Danielsen (personal communication,<br />

2003), one kg <strong>of</strong> gain in gestation requires 14 MJ ME, for which reason 0.390 <strong>and</strong> 0.195 kg<br />

<strong>of</strong> gain would require 6 <strong>and</strong> 3 MJ ME, respectively. Assuming that high ranking sows<br />

weighted about 260 kg <strong>and</strong> low ranking sows 200 kg, the daily energy requirement for<br />

maintenance for high <strong>and</strong> low ranking sows would be approximately 27 MJ ME <strong>and</strong> 23 MJ<br />

ME, respectively (Just et al., 1983; Theil et al., 2002; Danielsen, personal communication<br />

2003). Estimated energy intake for low ranking sows would therefore be approximately 80<br />

% <strong>of</strong> the energy intake <strong>of</strong> the high ranking sows (26 <strong>and</strong> 33 MJ ME, respectively).<br />

In commercial practice, the difference between high <strong>and</strong> low ranking sows’ feed intake<br />

during pregnancy may, under some circumstances, be higher than indicated in the studies<br />

by Andersen et al. (1999) <strong>and</strong> Brouns & Edwards (1994). In these studies, the <strong>group</strong> size<br />

was six <strong>and</strong> 12 sows, respectively, whereas in commercial sow herds, floor feeding is also<br />

practiced in larger <strong>group</strong>s. If the feed is provided on a small area, it is likely that the more<br />

sows in the <strong>group</strong>, the higher the risk that the low ranked sows are kept away from the feeding<br />

place. Experiences from practical husb<strong>and</strong>ry indicate that a huge variation in weight<br />

between sows causes more aggressions <strong>and</strong> displacements at feeding compared to more<br />

uniform <strong>group</strong>s (Olsson & Svendsen, 1997). Therefore, the magnitude <strong>of</strong> the variation in<br />

energy intake between <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> sows will probably not only depend upon feeding procedure<br />

but also on <strong>group</strong> size <strong>and</strong> <strong>group</strong> composition <strong>and</strong> then vary considerably between<br />

herds but also within herds between farrowing batches.<br />

It is generally recommended to feed pregnant sows in the range <strong>of</strong> 25-35 MJ ME day -1<br />

(NRC, 1998; The National Committee for Pig Production, 2003b). If sows for instance are<br />

provided with 30 MJ ME day -1 , <strong>and</strong> assuming that the low ranking sows only eat approximately<br />

50-80% <strong>of</strong> the portion that high ranking sows as indicated in the studies by<br />

Andersen et al. (1999) <strong>and</strong> Brouns & Edwards (1994), this means that high ranking sows<br />

would consume 33-40 MJ ME day -1 <strong>and</strong> low ranking sows 20-26 MJ ME day -1 in average.<br />

In gilts, an energy intake <strong>of</strong> 33-40 MJ ME day -1 could have a negative influence on litter<br />

size if it occurs the first three days after mating. An energy intake <strong>of</strong> 20-26 MJ ME day -1<br />

40


- Paper I -<br />

the first four weeks after mating could perhaps impair pregnancy rate in gilts <strong>and</strong> litter size<br />

in sows fed restricted during lactation.<br />

As regards before mating, it is not possible to say how huge impact the involvement in aggressive<br />

interactions has on a sow’s appetite <strong>and</strong> therefore whether variation in feed intake<br />

can influence the variation in litter size <strong>and</strong> pregnancy rate. Further studies are needed to<br />

clarify this.<br />

6. Conclusion<br />

Based upon a review <strong>of</strong> existing literature, it is suggested that pregnancy rate <strong>and</strong> litter size<br />

can be influenced by energy intake although the results reviewed do not provide a clear<br />

picture. There is little doubt, however, that the reproduction performance <strong>of</strong> the gilt, in general,<br />

is more influenced by energy supply than the reproduction performance <strong>of</strong> the sow. It<br />

is further suggested that variation in feed intake in a <strong>group</strong> <strong>of</strong> restricted fed pregnant female<br />

pigs in commercial pig husb<strong>and</strong>ry, may be large enough to influence pregnancy rate <strong>and</strong><br />

litter size. However, there is no empiricism to support this, why there is a need for studies<br />

analyzing whether a relation between individual feed intake <strong>and</strong> reproduction performance<br />

exists in a <strong>group</strong> <strong>of</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating female pigs.<br />

41


7. References<br />

- Paper I -<br />

Allrich, R.D., Tilton, J.E., Johnson, J.N., Slanger, W.D., Marchello, M.J., 1979. Effect <strong>of</strong> lactation length <strong>and</strong><br />

fasting on various reproductive phenomena <strong>of</strong> sows. J. Anim. Sci. 48, 359-362.<br />

Almeida, F.R.C.L., Kirkwood, R.N., Aherne, F.X., Foxcr<strong>of</strong>t, G.R., 2000. Consquences <strong>of</strong> different patterns <strong>of</strong><br />

feed intake during the estrous cycle in gilts on subsequent fertility. J. Anim. Sci. 78, 1556-1563.<br />

Andersen, I.L., Bøe, K., Kristiansen, A.L., 1999. The influence <strong>of</strong> different feeding arrangements <strong>and</strong> food<br />

type on competition at feeding in pregnant sows. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 65, 91-104.<br />

Anderson, L.L., 1975. Embryonic <strong>and</strong> placental development during prolonged inanition in the pig. Am. J.<br />

Physiol. 229, 1687-1694.<br />

Arey, D., Edwards, S.A., 1998. Factors influencing aggression between sows after mixing <strong>and</strong> the concequences<br />

for welfare <strong>and</strong> production. Livest. Prod. Sci. 56, 61-70.<br />

Ashworth, C.J., Antipatis, C., 1999. Effect <strong>of</strong> pre- <strong>and</strong> post-mating nutrtion on embryo survival in gilts. Reproduc.<br />

Dom. Anim. 34, 103-108.<br />

Baidoo, S.K., Aherne, F.X., Kirkwood, R.N., Foxcr<strong>of</strong>t, G.R., 1992. Effect <strong>of</strong> feed intake during lactation <strong>and</strong><br />

after weaning on sow reproductive performance. Can. J. Anim. Sci. 72, 91-917.<br />

Barnett, J.L., Cronin, G.M., Winfield, C.G., 1981. The effects <strong>of</strong> individual <strong>and</strong> <strong>group</strong> penning <strong>of</strong> pigs on total<br />

<strong>and</strong> free plasma corticosteroids <strong>and</strong> the maximum corticosteroid binding capacity. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol.<br />

44, 219-225.<br />

Bates, R.O., Edwards, D. B., Korthals, R.L., 2003. Sow performance when <strong>housed</strong> either in <strong>group</strong>s with electronic<br />

sow feeder or stalls. Livest. Prod. Sci. 79, 29-35.<br />

Baustad, B., Lium, B., 2002. Helse og dyrevelferd i norsk svineproduksjon sett i et internasjonalt perspektiv.<br />

Norsk Vet. tidsskrift 114, 87-91.<br />

Beltranena, E., Foxcr<strong>of</strong>t, G.R., Aherne, F.X., Kirkwood, R.N., 1991. Endocrinology <strong>of</strong> nutritional flushing in<br />

gilts. Can. J. Anim. Sci. 71, 1063-1071.<br />

Brouns, F., Edwards, S.A., 1992. Future prospects for housing <strong>of</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows. Pig News <strong>and</strong> Inform.<br />

13, 47-50.<br />

Brouns, F., Edwards, S.A., 1994. Social rank <strong>and</strong> feeding behaviour <strong>of</strong> <strong>group</strong>-<strong>housed</strong> sows fed competitively<br />

or ad libitum. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 39, 225-235.<br />

Brouns, F., Edwards, S.A., English, P.R., 1991. Fibrous raw materials in sow diets: Effects on voluntary food<br />

intake, digestibility <strong>and</strong> diurnal activity patterns. Anim. Prod. 52, 598.<br />

Cassar, G., King, G.J., 1992. Effect <strong>of</strong> high energy feeding after mating on developmental uniformity <strong>and</strong><br />

survival <strong>of</strong> porcine conceptuses. J. Anim. Sci. 70 (suppl. 1), 266.<br />

42


- Paper I -<br />

Cosgrove, J.R., Foxcr<strong>of</strong>t, G.R., 1996. Nutrition <strong>and</strong> reproduction in the pig: ovarian aetiology. Anim. Reprod.<br />

Sci. 42, 131-141.<br />

Cox, N.M., Stuart, M.J., Althen, T.G., Bennett, W.A., Miller, W., 1987. Enhancement <strong>of</strong> ovulation rate in<br />

gilts by increasing dietary energy <strong>and</strong> administering insulin during follicular growth. J. Anim. Sci. 64,<br />

507-516.<br />

Csermely, D., Wood-Gush, D.G.M., 1990. Agonistic behaviour in <strong>group</strong>ed sows. II. How social rank affects<br />

feeding <strong>and</strong> drinking behaviour. Boll. Zool. 57, 55-58.<br />

Dalin, A.M., Magnusson, U., Häggendal, J., Nyberg, L., 1993. The effect <strong>of</strong> thiopentone-sodium anesthesia<br />

<strong>and</strong> surgery, relocation, <strong>group</strong>ing, <strong>and</strong> hydrocortisone treatment on the blood levels <strong>of</strong> cortisol, corticosteroid-binding<br />

globulin <strong>and</strong> chatecholamines in pigs. J. Anim. Sci. 71, 1902-1909.<br />

den Hartog, L.A., van der Steen, H.A.M., 1981. Reproductive traits in primaparous sows in relation to feeding<br />

level. Neth. J. <strong>of</strong> Agric. Sci. 29, 285-286.<br />

Dourmad, J.Y., Étienne, M., Prunier, A., Noblet, J., 1994. The effect <strong>of</strong> energy <strong>and</strong> protein intake <strong>of</strong> sows on<br />

their longevity: a review. Livest. Prod. Sci. 40, 87-97.<br />

Dyck, G.W., 1991. The effect <strong>of</strong> postmating diet intake on embryonic <strong>and</strong> fetal survival <strong>and</strong> litter size in gilts.<br />

Can. J. Anim. Sci. 71, 675-681.<br />

Dyck, G.W., Strain, J. H., 1983. Postmating feeding level effects on conception rate <strong>and</strong> embryo survival in<br />

gilts. Can. J. Anim. Sci. 63, 579.<br />

Dyck, G.W., Cole, D. J. A., 1986. The effect <strong>of</strong> restricted energy <strong>and</strong> nutrient intake after mating on reproductive<br />

performance <strong>of</strong> multiparous sows. Anim. Prod. 42, 127.<br />

Einarsson, S., Rojkittikhun, T., 1993. Effects <strong>of</strong> nutrition on pregnant <strong>and</strong> lactating sows. J. Reprod. Fertil.,<br />

Suppl. 48, 229-239.<br />

Fisker, B.N., 1995. Indsættelsesstrategi for gruppefodrede drægtige søer. Meddelelse 311, Den rullende Afprøvning,<br />

L<strong>and</strong>sudvalget for Svin, Danske Slagterier, 7pp.<br />

Flowers, B., Martin, M.J., Cantley, T.C., Day, B.N., 1989. Endocrine changes associated with a dietaryinduced<br />

increase in ovulation rate (flushing) in gilts. J. Anim. Sci. 67, 771-778.<br />

Foxcr<strong>of</strong>t, G.R., 1997. Mechanisms mediating nutritional effects on embryonic survival in pigs. J. Reprod.<br />

Fertil., Suppl. 52, 47-61.<br />

Foxcr<strong>of</strong>t, G.R., Hunter, W.G., 1985. Basic physiology <strong>of</strong> follicular maturation in the pig. J. Reprod. Fertil.,<br />

Suppl. 33, 1-19.<br />

Gatel, F., Castaing, J., Lucbert, J., 1987. Changes in productivity <strong>and</strong> culling rate according to pregnancy feed<br />

intake <strong>and</strong> litter parity. Livest. Prod. Sci. 17, 247-261.<br />

43


- Paper I -<br />

Geisert, R.D., Yelich, J.V., 1997. Regulation <strong>of</strong> conceptus development <strong>and</strong> attachment in pigs. J. Reprod.<br />

Fertil., Suppl. 52, 133-149.<br />

Gjein, H., Larssen, R.B., 1995. Housing <strong>of</strong> pregnant sows in loose <strong>and</strong> confined systems - a field study. 1.<br />

Vulva <strong>and</strong> body lesions, culling reasons <strong>and</strong> production results. Acta vet. Sc<strong>and</strong>. 36, 185-200.<br />

Graves, H.B., 1984. Behavior <strong>and</strong> ecology <strong>of</strong> wild <strong>and</strong> feral swine (Sus Scr<strong>of</strong>a). J. Anim. Sci. 58, 482-492.<br />

Han, I.K., Bosi, P., Hyan, Y., Kim, J.D., Sohn, K.S., Kim, S.W., 2000. Recent advances in sow nutrition to<br />

improve reproductive performance. Asian-Austr. J. Anim. Sci. 13, 335-355.<br />

Hansen, L.U., 2000. Løbeafdeling med enkeltdyrsstier eller flokopstaldning. Meddelelse 6, Den rullende<br />

Afprøvning, L<strong>and</strong>sudvalget for Svin, Danske Slagterier, 6pp.<br />

Hansen, L.U., 2003. Løbeafdeling med enkeltdyrsstier eller flokopstaldning med permanent adgang til æde-<br />

/insemineringsbokse. Meddelelse 602, Den rullende Afprøvning, L<strong>and</strong>sudvalget for Svin, Danske Slagterier,<br />

6pp.<br />

Hughes, P.E., Pearce, G.P., 1989. The endocrine basis <strong>of</strong> nutrition-reproduction interactions. In: Barnett, J.L.,<br />

Hennessy, D.P. (Eds.), Manipulating Pig Production II. Australasian Pig Science Association, Werribee,<br />

pp. 290-295.<br />

Hughes, P.E., Henry, R.W., Pichard, D.W., 1984. The effect <strong>of</strong> lactation food level on subsequent ovulation<br />

rate <strong>and</strong> early embryonic survival in the sow. Anim. Prod. 38, 527.<br />

Hurtgen, J.P., Leman, A.D., Crabo, B., 1980. Effect <strong>of</strong> season, parity <strong>and</strong> housing factors on estrus <strong>and</strong> fertility<br />

in swine. Proc. Int. Pig. Vet. Soc., Copenhagen, Denmark, pp. 20.<br />

Jensen, K.H., Sørensen, L.S., Bertelsen, D., Pedersen, A.R., Jørgensen, E., Nielsen, N.-P., Vestergaard, K.S.,<br />

2000. Management factors affecting activity <strong>and</strong> aggression in dynamic <strong>group</strong> housing systems with<br />

electronic sow feeding: a field trial. Anim. sci. 71, 535-545.<br />

Jindal, R., Cosgrove, J.R., Aherne, F.X., Foxcr<strong>of</strong>t, G.R., 1996. Effect <strong>of</strong> nutrition on embryonal mortality in<br />

gilts: association with progesterone. J. Anim. Sci. 74, 620-624.<br />

Just, A., Jørgensen, H.H., Fern<strong>and</strong>ez, J.A., 1983. Svinenes behov for FEs til vedligehold samt foderstyrkens<br />

betydning for foderforbruget og kroppens indhold af kød. Meddelelse 488, Statens Husdyrbrugsforsøg,<br />

Foulum, Danmark, 5pp.<br />

King, R.H., Williams, I.H., 1984. The effect <strong>of</strong> nutrition on the reproductive performance <strong>of</strong> first-litter sows.<br />

1. Feeding level during lactation, <strong>and</strong> between weaning <strong>and</strong> mating. Anim. Prod. 38, 241-247.<br />

Kirkwood, R.N., Baidoo, S.K., Aherne, F.X., 1990. The influence <strong>of</strong> feeding level during lactation <strong>and</strong> gestation<br />

on the endocrine status <strong>and</strong> reproductive performance <strong>of</strong> second parity sows. Can. J. Anim. Sci.<br />

70, 1119-1126.<br />

Liao, C.W., Veum, T.L., 1994. Effects <strong>of</strong> dietary energy intake by gilts <strong>and</strong> heat stress from days 3 to 24 or 30<br />

after mating on embryo survival <strong>and</strong> nitrogen <strong>and</strong> energy balance. J. Anim. Sci. 72, 2369-2377.<br />

44


- Paper I -<br />

Matschke, G.H., 1964. The influence <strong>of</strong> oak mast on European wild hog reproduction. Proc. <strong>of</strong> the 18th<br />

Annu. Conf. Southeastern Assoc. <strong>of</strong> Game <strong>and</strong> Fish Commissions, October 18-21. Clearwater, FL. Pp.<br />

35-39.<br />

Mauget, R., 1981. Behavioural <strong>and</strong> reproductive strategies in wild forms <strong>of</strong> Sus scr<strong>of</strong>a (European wild boar<br />

<strong>and</strong> feral pigs). In: Sybesma, W. (Ed.), The welfare <strong>of</strong> Pigs. Martinus Nijh<strong>of</strong>f, The Hague, pp. 3-13.<br />

Mendl, M., Zanella, A.J., Broom, D.M., 1992. Physiological <strong>and</strong> reproductive correlates <strong>of</strong> behavioural<br />

strategies in female domestic pigs. Anim. Behav. 44, 1107-1121.<br />

Merk, V.L., Kirchgessner, M., 1984. Einfluss des energetischen versorgunsniveaus in der trächtighkeit auf<br />

einige reproduktionskriterien bei sauen. Züchtungskunde 56, 48-62.<br />

Mortensen, B., 1997. Kontraktproduktion af UK-grise. Notat 9712, L<strong>and</strong>sudvalget for Svin, Danske Slagterier,<br />

2pp.<br />

Nielsen, H.E., Danielsen, V.O., Larsen, A.E., 1981. Fodring af søer på fravænningsdagen. Meddelelse 400,<br />

Statens Husdyrbrugsforsøg, Foulum, Danmark, 2pp.<br />

Nielsen, N.-P., Hansen, L.U., Pedersen, P.N., 1997. Idékatalog til løbeafdelinger til løsgående søer. Notat<br />

9716, L<strong>and</strong>sudvalget for Svin, Danske Slagterier, 9pp.<br />

NRC (National Research Council). 1998. Nutrient Requirements <strong>of</strong> Swine. National Academy Press, Washington,<br />

D.C. 189pp.<br />

Olsson, A.-C., Svendsen, J., 1997. Effekter av olika konkurrensförhåll<strong>and</strong>en vid utfodring på suggors hälsa<br />

och produktion. Rapport 113, Sveriges Lantbruksuniversitet, Instituationen för jordbrukets biosystem<br />

och teknologi, 37pp.<br />

Olsson, A.-C., Svendsen, J., Reese, D., Andersson, M., Rantzer, D., 1993. Inhysning av dräktiga suggor i<br />

långsmala boxar med blötutfodring. Rapport 87, Sveriges lantbruksuniversitet, Institutionen för lantbrukets<br />

byggnadsteknik, 39pp.<br />

Ottenweller, J.E., Natelson, B.H., Pitman, D.L., Drastal, S.D., 1989. Adrenocortical <strong>and</strong> behavioral responses<br />

to repeated stressors: Toward an animal model <strong>of</strong> chronic stress <strong>and</strong> stress-related mental illness. Biol.<br />

Psychiatry 26, 829-841.<br />

Peltoniemi, O.A.T., Easton, B.G., Love, R.J., Klupiec, C., Evans, G., 1995. Effect <strong>of</strong> chronic treatment with a<br />

GnRH agonist (Goserelin) on LH secretion <strong>and</strong> early pregnancy in gilts. Anim. Reprod. Sci. 40, 121-<br />

133.<br />

Peltoniemi, O.A.T., Love, R.J., Heionen, M., Tuovinen, V., Saloiemi, H., 1999. Seasonal <strong>and</strong> management<br />

effects on fertility <strong>of</strong> the sow: a descriptive study. Anim. Reprod. Sci. 55, 47-61.<br />

Peltoniemi, O.A.T., Tast, A., Love, R.J., 2000. Factors affecting reproduction in the pig: seasonal effects <strong>and</strong><br />

restricted feeding <strong>of</strong> the pregnant ilt <strong>and</strong> sow. Anim. Reprod. Sci. 60, 173-184.<br />

45


- Paper I -<br />

Pharazyn, A., den Hartog, L.A., Foxcr<strong>of</strong>t, G.R., Aherne, F.X., 1991. Dietary energy <strong>and</strong> protein intake,<br />

plasma progesterone <strong>and</strong> embryo survival in early pregnancy in the gilt. Can. J. Anim. Sci. 71, 949-<br />

952.<br />

Pope, W.F., 1994. Embryonic mortality in swine. In: Zavy, M.T., Geisert, R.D. (Eds.), Embryonic mortality in<br />

domestic species. Boca Raton, Florida, pp. 53-77.<br />

Pope, W.F., First, N.L., 1985. Factors affecting the survival <strong>of</strong> pig embryos. Theriogenology 23, 91-105.<br />

Prunier, A., Quesnel, H., 2000a. Influence <strong>of</strong> the nutritional status on ovarian development in female pigs.<br />

Anim. Reprod. Sci. 60, 185-197.<br />

Prunier, A., Quesnel, H., 2000b. Nutritional influences on the hormonal control <strong>of</strong> reproduction in female<br />

pigs. Livest. Prod. Sci. 63, 1-16.<br />

Rodríguez Echanía, E.L., Gonzalez, A.S., Cabrera, R., Fracchia, L.N., 1988. A further analysis <strong>of</strong> behavioural<br />

<strong>and</strong> endocrine effects <strong>of</strong> unpredictable chronic stress. Physiol. Behav. 43, 789-795.<br />

Ruis, M.A.W., te Brake, J.H.A., Engel, B., Buist, W.G., Blokhuis, H.J., Koolhaas, J.M., 2002. Implications <strong>of</strong><br />

coping characteristics <strong>and</strong> social status for welfare <strong>and</strong> production <strong>of</strong> paired growing gilts. Appl. Anim.<br />

Behav. Sci. 75, 207-231.<br />

Spoolder, H.A.M., Burbidge, J.A., Edwards, S.A., Simmins, P.H., Lawrence, A.B., 1996. Effects <strong>of</strong> food level<br />

<strong>and</strong> straw bedding during pregnancy on sow performance <strong>and</strong> responses to an ACTH challenge. Livest.<br />

Prod. Sci. 47, 51-57.<br />

Sørensen, G., 1994. Søernes foderstyrke de første fire uger efter løbning. Meddelelse 280, Den rullende Afprøvning,<br />

L<strong>and</strong>sudvalget for Svin, Danske Slagterier, 5pp.<br />

Sørensen, G., Thorup, F., 2003. Energitildeling i implantationsperioden. Meddelelse 618, Den rullende Afprøvning,<br />

L<strong>and</strong>sudvalget for Svin, Danske Slagterier, 7pp.<br />

Theil, P.K., Jørgensen, H., Jakobsen, K., 2002. Energy <strong>and</strong> protein metabolism in pregnant sows fed two<br />

levels <strong>of</strong> dietary protein. J. Anim. Physiol. a. Anim. Nutr. 86, 399-413.<br />

The National Committee for Pig Production, 2003a. Fodring af gold-søer, Info Svin Håndbog, L<strong>and</strong>sudvalget<br />

for Svin, Danske Slagterier. 1pp.<br />

The National Committee for Pig Production, 2003b. Fodring af drægtige søer, Info Svin Håndbog, L<strong>and</strong>sudvalget<br />

for Svin, Danske Slagterier. 1pp.<br />

Toplis, P., Ginesi, M.F.J., Wrathall, A.E., 1983. The influence <strong>of</strong> high food levels in early pregnancy on embryo<br />

survival in multiparous sows. Anim. Prod. 37, 45-48.<br />

Trezona, M., 2003. Welfare update: Dry sow stalls.<br />

http://www.agric.wa.gov.au/progserv/animal/cntnorth/porkserv/pigtales/2000/Oct2000/article08.htm ,<br />

1-2. 2003. Department <strong>of</strong> Agriculture - Western Australia.<br />

46


- Paper I -<br />

Tribble, L.F., Orr, D.E., 1982. Effect <strong>of</strong> feeding level after weaning on reproduction in sows. J. Anim. Sci. 55,<br />

608-612.<br />

Tsuma, V.T., Einarsson, S., Madej, A., Kindahl, H., Lundeheim, N., Rojkittikhun, T., 1996. Endocrine<br />

changes during <strong>group</strong> housing <strong>of</strong> primaparous sows in early pregnancy. Acta vet. Sc<strong>and</strong>. 37, 481-490.<br />

Walker, N., 1983. The effects <strong>of</strong> food intake in gestation on sows lactating for 14 days. Anim. Prod. 37, 25-<br />

31.<br />

Whittemore, C.T., Taylor, A.G., Hillyer, G.M., Wilson, D., Stamataris, C., 1984. Influence <strong>of</strong> body fat stores<br />

on reproductive performance <strong>of</strong> sows. Anim. Prod. 38, 527 Abstr.<br />

Xue, J.L., Koketsu, Y., Dial, G.D., Pettigrew, J.E., Sower, A.F., 1997. Glucose tolerance, Luteinizing Hormone<br />

release, <strong>and</strong> reproductive performance <strong>of</strong> first-litter sows fed two levels <strong>of</strong> energy during gestation.<br />

J. Anim. Sci. 75, 1845-1852.<br />

Yang, H., Eastham, P.R., Phillips, P., Whittemore, C.T., 1989. Reproductive performance, body weight <strong>and</strong><br />

body condition <strong>of</strong> breeding sows with differing body fatness at parturition, differing nutrition during<br />

lactation <strong>and</strong> differing litter size. Anim. Prod. 48, 181-201.<br />

Young, L.G., King, G.J., Walton, S., McMillan, I., Klevorick, M., Shaw, J., 1990. Gestation energy <strong>and</strong> reproduction<br />

in sows over four parities. Can. J. Anim. Sci. 70, 493-506.<br />

Zak, L.J., Cosgrove, J.R., Aherne, F.X., Foxcr<strong>of</strong>t, G.R., 1997. Pattern <strong>of</strong> feed intake <strong>and</strong> associated metabolic<br />

<strong>and</strong> endocrine changes differentially affect postweaning fertility on primiparous lactating sows. J.<br />

Anim. Sci. 75, 208-216.<br />

47


- Paper II -<br />

Stress <strong>and</strong> fear as possible mediators <strong>of</strong> reproduction problems in <strong>group</strong><br />

<strong>housed</strong> sows: A review<br />

A.G. Kongsted<br />

Department <strong>of</strong> Agroecology, Danish Institute <strong>of</strong> Agricultural Sciences, P.O. Box 50, DK-<br />

8830 Tjele<br />

Acta Agric. Sc<strong>and</strong>., Sect. A, Animal Science 54: 58-66, 2004<br />

49<br />

II


- Paper III -<br />

Indicators <strong>of</strong> feed intake, fear <strong>and</strong> social stress in commercial herds with<br />

<strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows<br />

A.G. Kongsted, J.E. Hermansen <strong>and</strong> T. Kristensen<br />

Department <strong>of</strong> Agroecology, Danish Institute <strong>of</strong> Agricultural Sciences, P.O. Box 50, DK-<br />

8830 Tjele<br />

Submitted to Acta Agric. Sc<strong>and</strong>., Sect. A, Animal Science<br />

59<br />

Ш


- Paper III -<br />

Abstract<br />

A.G. Kongsted, J.E. Hermansen <strong>and</strong> T. Kristensen (Department <strong>of</strong> Agroecology, Danish<br />

Institute <strong>of</strong> Agricultural Sciences, P.O. Box 50, DK-8830 Tjele). Indicators <strong>of</strong> feed intake,<br />

fear <strong>and</strong> social stress in commercial herds with <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows. Acta Agric.<br />

Sc<strong>and</strong>., Sect. A, Animal Sci.<br />

The number <strong>of</strong> <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows is increasing rapidly in Europe as a consequence<br />

<strong>of</strong> changed legislation. With the aim to evaluate indicators <strong>of</strong> feed intake, fear <strong>and</strong><br />

stress <strong>and</strong> to get insight in the level <strong>and</strong> variation in these indicators in <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> sows<br />

under various on-farm <strong>conditions</strong>, a study took place including 14 herds. The results<br />

showed that back fat-, skin lesions- <strong>and</strong> behavioural measurements might be relevant indicators<br />

<strong>of</strong> sows’ condition at herd, batch <strong>and</strong> individual sow level. For almost all indicators,<br />

the variation between herds was larger than the variation between batches. The betweensow<br />

variation in back fat was significant higher in <strong>group</strong>- compared to individual feeding<br />

systems. For some <strong>of</strong> the indicators, the effect <strong>of</strong> parity differed between different layouts.<br />

For instance, in herds without escape possibilities, first parity sows had the highest level <strong>of</strong><br />

lesions, whereas in herds with escape possibilities, second <strong>and</strong> third parity sows had the<br />

highest level.<br />

Keywords: Farm study, skin lesions, eating behaviour, aggressions, <strong>group</strong> feeding, individual<br />

feeding<br />

60


- Paper III -<br />

1. Introduction<br />

The number <strong>of</strong> <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows is increasing rapidly in Europe as a consequence<br />

<strong>of</strong> changed EU legislation (Council Directive 2001/88/EC amending Directive<br />

91/630/EEC Laying Down Minimum St<strong>and</strong>ards for the Protection <strong>of</strong> Pigs) combined with<br />

extraordinary national laws (Baustad & Lium, 2002; The welfare <strong>of</strong> Farmed Animals (Engl<strong>and</strong>)<br />

(Amendment) Regulations 2003).<br />

However, since individual housing <strong>of</strong> sows has been the far most common system in commercial<br />

sow herds for many decades, most <strong>of</strong> the information available about sows’ production<br />

<strong>and</strong> condition arrives from studies with individual housing. Although the amount <strong>of</strong><br />

scientific work related to <strong>group</strong> housing <strong>of</strong> sows has increased markedly in the last ten<br />

years, there is still a lot <strong>of</strong> unanswered questions concerning the function <strong>of</strong> the sows in<br />

commercial herds with <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows.<br />

Several experimental studies indicated that <strong>group</strong> housing <strong>of</strong> sows might lead to individual<br />

variation in feed intake, social stress, <strong>and</strong> fear (reviews by Kongsted, 2004ab). This may<br />

constitute a welfare problem <strong>and</strong> in addition affect the reproduction results negatively (reviews<br />

by Kongsted, 2004ab). Most <strong>of</strong> the studies referred to in the mentioned reviews were<br />

performed under <strong>conditions</strong> that did not always reflect those circumstances seen in practice<br />

which are large <strong>group</strong> sizes (Nielsen et al., 2000), high stocking rates (Svendsen et al.,<br />

1990), constant introduction <strong>of</strong> new sows into dynamics <strong>group</strong>s (Svendsen et al., 1990),<br />

slippery floors (Hansen & Kongsted, 2002) <strong>and</strong> sows with locomotion problems (Gjein &<br />

Larssen, 1995; Nielsen et al., 2000; Hansen & Kongsted, 2002).<br />

In a few Danish on-farm experiments, level <strong>and</strong> variation <strong>of</strong> indicators <strong>of</strong> feed intake expressed<br />

as growth rate (Fisker, 1994; Nielsen, 1995; Fisker, 1999; Hansen, 2000) <strong>and</strong> stress<br />

expressed as adrenocortical response to additional stressors (Jensen et al., 1995) in <strong>group</strong><br />

<strong>housed</strong> sows has been assessed. However, systematic information <strong>of</strong> variation in feed intake,<br />

fear <strong>and</strong> social stress in sows <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> in the entire <strong>non</strong>-lactating period under<br />

various <strong>conditions</strong> are lacking. This is most likely, to some extent, because traditional<br />

methods for assessing these factors are expensive <strong>and</strong>/or time-consuming <strong>and</strong> therefore<br />

difficult to employ under practical <strong>conditions</strong> in large scale. First <strong>of</strong> all, it is therefore necessary<br />

to identify indicators <strong>of</strong> energy intake, stress <strong>and</strong> fear suitable for use in practice.<br />

Based upon existing knowledge, a suggestion for such indicators has been put forward in<br />

Kongsted (2004a). However, whether these indicators are suitable for use in <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong><br />

sows under various on-farm <strong>conditions</strong> is not known.<br />

On this background the aim <strong>of</strong> this study is 1) to evaluate indicators believed to be suitable<br />

to gain information <strong>of</strong> variation in feed intake, social stress <strong>and</strong> fear in <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows<br />

61


- Paper III -<br />

<strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> under different on-farm <strong>conditions</strong> <strong>and</strong> 2) to get insight in the level <strong>and</strong> be-<br />

tween-farm <strong>and</strong> within-farm variation in indicators <strong>of</strong> energy intake, fear <strong>and</strong> social stress<br />

in <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> under various on-farm <strong>conditions</strong>.<br />

2. Materials <strong>and</strong> methods<br />

2.1 Herds, sows <strong>and</strong> design <strong>of</strong> recordings<br />

The study was conducted during an 11-month period from May 2003 to March 2004 in<br />

fourteen Danish commercial sow herds with <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows. The herds<br />

were chosen to represent different layouts <strong>and</strong> management routines. The herd sizes varied<br />

from 180 to 1000 sows. Farrowing batch interval was 1, 2 <strong>and</strong> 2½ week in six, five <strong>and</strong><br />

three herds, respectively. In all herds, the sows were moved to the service department <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>group</strong>ed on the day <strong>of</strong> weaning or the day after. One herd had integrated service <strong>and</strong> pregnancy<br />

department. In all other herds, the sows were moved to the pregnancy department<br />

between 0 to 28 days after first mating. In Table 1 the layout <strong>of</strong> the herds are detailed. It<br />

appears e.g. that eight <strong>of</strong> the 14 herds practiced <strong>group</strong> feeding (bi<strong>of</strong>ix, on the floor <strong>and</strong> long<br />

feeding troughs) in the pregnancy unit.<br />

Four batches were observed on each farm from weaning to farrowing. In each <strong>of</strong> the four<br />

batches, ten focal sows (F-sows) were r<strong>and</strong>omly chosen in the lactation department just<br />

before the sows were moved to the service department on the day <strong>of</strong> weaning. In those<br />

herds where the sows were divided into different pens according to size (small, normal,<br />

large) in the service department the farmer marked the ‘normal’ sows the day before weaning<br />

<strong>and</strong> the ten F-sows were then chosen r<strong>and</strong>omly from this <strong>group</strong> <strong>of</strong> sows. This procedure<br />

was carried out to ensure that all F-sows were placed in the same pen (or at least in adjacent<br />

pens) to make sure that it was possible to observe all F-sows at the same time. To allow<br />

individual identification during behavioural observations, the ten F-sows were sprayed with<br />

a number on their back <strong>and</strong> sides.<br />

62


Table 1. Applied feeding <strong>and</strong> housing systems in the service <strong>and</strong> the pregnancy unit <strong>of</strong> 14 farms<br />

Feeding system No. herds in total Ad lib. No. <strong>of</strong> daily Feed type Floor type Group dynamics Group size<br />

feeding feedings<br />

1 ≥2 Dry Liquid Concrete Deep lit- Stable Dynamic


- Paper III -<br />

2.2 Registration <strong>of</strong> indicators<br />

2.2.1 Eating behaviour<br />

Although eating speed may differ between sows (Brouns & Edwards, 1994), behavioural<br />

observations <strong>of</strong> time spend eating is likely to give some indications <strong>of</strong> feed intake<br />

(Andersen et al., 1999).<br />

For each F-sow, it was recorded whether the sow was eating or not for each half minute<br />

during feeding. A sow was considered eating if she, while chewing had her snout in the<br />

trough or in contact with the floor (Csermely & Wood-Gush, 1990). However, if the sow<br />

shortly lifted her head from the trough or floor still chewing, she was also considered eating.<br />

The recordings began the moment the feed was supplied <strong>and</strong> stopped 25 minutes (50<br />

recordings) after or until the last sow had stopped eating. Number <strong>of</strong> times the sow was not<br />

eating compared to number <strong>of</strong> recordings (max 50) was calculated for each F-sow (% not<br />

eating).<br />

2.2.2 Back fat depth <strong>and</strong> back fat gain<br />

Measurements <strong>of</strong> back fat depth is a method to assess the body condition <strong>of</strong> sows (Charettte<br />

et al., 1996 fra livestock s. 4) <strong>and</strong> is possible to perform also under practical <strong>conditions</strong><br />

(pers.comm., Maes, 2004).<br />

Back fat depth was measured on the F-sows by means <strong>of</strong> a digital ultrasound back fat indicator<br />

LEAN MEATER ® (Baltic Korn A/S, Naestved, Denmark). All measurements were<br />

performed in the home pen <strong>of</strong> the sow i.e. in a farrowing crate (at weaning <strong>and</strong> at farrowing)<br />

or in a <strong>group</strong> <strong>of</strong> sows (three weeks after mating). The back fat was measured about 65<br />

mm from either side <strong>of</strong> the backbone at the 10 th <strong>and</strong> 12 th (last) rib (conventionally known as<br />

P2-measurements) <strong>and</strong> all three layers <strong>of</strong> fat was measured. Four measurements were performed<br />

in all (two at each rib). The average value <strong>of</strong> the four measurements was used to<br />

characterise the back fat depth <strong>of</strong> the sow. Observations with more than five mm deviations<br />

between the lowest <strong>and</strong> highest measurement were excluded from the material (15 sows).<br />

Back fat gain per day were calculated as the difference between back fat at the beginning <strong>of</strong><br />

the period <strong>and</strong> back fat at the end <strong>of</strong> the period divided with the number <strong>of</strong> days between<br />

the two measurement days.<br />

2.2.3 Fear tests<br />

Three examples <strong>of</strong> fear tests believed to be possible to perform in commercial sow herds<br />

were used in this study. These test are further discussed in Kongsted (2004a).<br />

At weaning, when the F-sows were routinely moved from the service to the pregnancy department,<br />

a human approach test (HA-test) (Rousing et al., 1999; Bonde et al., 2003) was<br />

64


- Paper III -<br />

performed. The sow had to pass a human (a research technician) at a passage <strong>of</strong> 10 metres<br />

long <strong>and</strong> about one meter wide. The relation between the time required to approach the per-<br />

son (AB) compared to the total time required to pass the 10 metres (AC) was calculated<br />

(=AB/AC). If the sow had not passed the 10 metres after two minutes the test was stopped.<br />

If the AB was more than two minutes the value <strong>of</strong> AB/AC was specified as 1. Sows with<br />

AB/AC values <strong>of</strong> > 0.67 were categorized as fearful.<br />

In the home pen <strong>of</strong> the F-sows, a forced human approach (FHA-test) (Andersen et al.,<br />

2003) was performed. A research technician entered the home pen quietly, approached the<br />

sow <strong>and</strong> squatted down immobile 20 cm from the sows for 30 seconds. The sow’s reaction<br />

was categorised into one <strong>of</strong> six possible reactions: 1. Fled at a distance <strong>of</strong> more than 1 m<br />

from the technician, 2. Withdrew some steps away from or turned her head away from the<br />

technician, <strong>and</strong> stayed there ore continued with activity for the rest <strong>of</strong> the test period, 3.<br />

Withdrew some steps away from or turned her head away from the technician, but approached<br />

the technician again <strong>and</strong> initiated physical contact within the test period, 4. Neither<br />

withdrew from nor approached the technician, but remained in the same posture or<br />

continued with activity, 5. Remained in the same posture or continued with activity, but<br />

seeked contact with the technician before the end <strong>of</strong> the test period or 6. Approached <strong>and</strong><br />

initiated physical contact with the technician (pers. comm., Andersen 2003). Sows with<br />

reaction 1 or 2 were categorized as fearful.<br />

After the FHA test a forced human touch test (FHT-test) (Pedersen et al., 2003) was performed.<br />

The technician approached the head <strong>of</strong> the sow <strong>and</strong> touched or tried to touch the<br />

sows neck. The sows reaction was divided into three reactions: 1. Fled before touching her<br />

neck was possible/fled with or without squaling/stood immobile holding the head still while<br />

keeping the eyes fixed, 2. Walked away without squealing or 3. Stood calmly/moved the<br />

head towards the technician/approached the technician (Pedersen et al., 2003). Sows with<br />

reaction 1 were categorised as fearful.<br />

The FHA <strong>and</strong> the FHT tests were performed outside the sows’ expected resting period.<br />

Based upon previous studies (Jensen et al., 1996) <strong>and</strong> statements from the individual farmers,<br />

resting period was chosen as between 11 <strong>and</strong> 13 PM in all 14 herds.<br />

Sows may use feeding stalls as an escape possibility during fighting (Olsson & Samuelsson,<br />

1993 cf. Arey & Edwards, 1998) <strong>and</strong> frequency <strong>of</strong> reside in stalls may reflect level <strong>of</strong> fear<br />

towards the other sows in the <strong>group</strong>. Therefore, for each F-sow, it was recorded whether the<br />

sow was in box or not for each 10 minute the first hour after weaning <strong>and</strong> half an hour at<br />

mating <strong>and</strong> three weeks after mating. Number <strong>of</strong> times the sow was in box compared to<br />

number <strong>of</strong> recordings (7 or 4) was calculated for each F-sow (% in box).<br />

65


- Paper III -<br />

2.2.4 Aggressive interactions, skin lesions <strong>and</strong> lying solitary<br />

Involvements in aggressive interactions (Barnett et al., 1981; Mendl et al., 1992), skin le-<br />

sions (Barnett et al., 1992) <strong>and</strong> frequency <strong>of</strong> lying solitary during resting periods (Barnett et<br />

al., 1984; Bonde, 2004) were used as indicators <strong>of</strong> stress. For a discussion <strong>of</strong> why these<br />

parameters are believed to be suitable indicators for stress, see review by Kongsted (2004a).<br />

The first hour after weaning <strong>and</strong> half an hour at mating <strong>and</strong> three weeks after mating outside<br />

the resting period, the number <strong>of</strong> aggressive interactions the individual sow participated<br />

in was recorded. An interaction was defined to have occurred when one sow initiated<br />

a behaviour, which was clearly directed at another sow in the <strong>group</strong> (Bradshaw et al.,<br />

2000). All interactions were categorised into four different types: 1) a threat, 2) one<br />

bite/knock/push, 3) several bites/knocks/pushes <strong>and</strong> 4) bites from both sows (Jensen et al.,<br />

1996; Bradshaw et al., 2000; Jensen et al., 2002).<br />

The total number <strong>of</strong> skin lesions divided into four length categories (10<br />

cm) on head, ears, neck <strong>and</strong> shoulders (Barnett et al., 1992) were recorded for all F-sows by<br />

a research technician. All lesions (except shoulder wounds because they are not a result <strong>of</strong><br />

aggressive interactions) were recorded (also superficial scratches). The recordings were<br />

performed in the home pen <strong>of</strong> the F-sows, i.e. in a <strong>group</strong> <strong>of</strong> sows. Total length was calculated<br />

as total number in the length category 1-4 multiplied with 0.5, 2.5, 7.5 <strong>and</strong> 10 respectively.<br />

The behaviour <strong>of</strong> the F-sows was recorded during resting period for 25 minutes. Every five<br />

minute the position <strong>of</strong> the individual sow (in a feeding stall, in the dung area, in the resting/activity<br />

area) was recorded <strong>and</strong> the ten F-sows’ behaviour were categorised into the<br />

following: 1) lying/sitting solitary, 2) lying/sitting socially or 3) st<strong>and</strong>ing/walking/running<br />

(if a sows position was a feeding stall her behaviour was not recorded). Lying/sitting solitary<br />

was defined as lying/sitting in a distance <strong>of</strong> 20 cm or more from other sows (Bonde,<br />

2004). If the sow was lying/sitting more frequently than not, the sow was categorised as<br />

lying. If the sow was lying/sitting solitary more frequently than lying/sitting socially, the<br />

sow were categorised as lying solitary.<br />

2.3 Timing <strong>and</strong> frequency <strong>of</strong> recordings<br />

The timing <strong>of</strong> registrations were first <strong>of</strong> all chosen to give information <strong>of</strong> the sows’ <strong>conditions</strong><br />

from weaning to first mating <strong>and</strong> from first mating to three weeks in pregnancy because<br />

these phases seems to be the main periods <strong>of</strong> relevance regarding reproduction performance<br />

(Kongsted, 2004ab). The timing <strong>of</strong> the above mentioned recordings <strong>and</strong> measurements<br />

are presented in Table 2. The recordings took place at weaning, at mating, ap-<br />

66


- Paper III -<br />

proximately three weeks after first mating or just before the following farrowing. Only<br />

sows, pregnant after first mating were followed all the way to the following farrowing.<br />

Table 2. Time schedule <strong>of</strong> measurements/recordings (number <strong>of</strong> herds in parenthesis)<br />

Indicator<br />

Measurements/recordings<br />

Day <strong>of</strong> weaning At mating<br />

(W)<br />

1)<br />

After weaning<br />

(M)<br />

2)<br />

At farrowing<br />

(AM)<br />

(F)<br />

Back fat X (14) - X (14) X (14)<br />

Eating behaviour X (2) X (2) X ( 9) -<br />

Fear test (HA) X (14) - - -<br />

Fear test (FHA <strong>and</strong> FHT) - - X (14) -<br />

Aggressions <strong>and</strong> % in box X (14) X (4) X (13) -<br />

Lesions X (14) X (14) X (14) X (14)<br />

Lying behaviour X (14) X (4) X (13) -<br />

1) 4-7 days after weaning 2) 20-28 days after weaning<br />

In ten herds the sows were fixed in feeding stalls in the days or hours around insemination<br />

why it was not possible or relevant to record eating <strong>and</strong> resting behaviour or to perform fear<br />

tests. For three farrowing batches in two different herds it was impossible to perform the<br />

human approach test when moving sows routinely at weaning because no suitable testing<br />

area was available. The two fear tests (FHA <strong>and</strong> FHT) were not performed at mating because<br />

it turned out to be very difficult to perform on sows in oestrus. Sows in oestrus were<br />

in general very contact seeking, which made the results unreliable <strong>and</strong> sometimes endangered<br />

the test person! In one herd, the large <strong>group</strong> sizes <strong>and</strong> the layout <strong>of</strong> the stables made it<br />

impossible to overview the ten F-sows resting behaviour three weeks after mating. Measurement<br />

<strong>of</strong> eating behaviour was only performed in herds with <strong>group</strong> feeding.<br />

2.4 Statistical analysis<br />

All analyses were based on data from individual sows. Parity was divided into three <strong>group</strong>s:<br />

1. First parity sows 2. Second <strong>and</strong> third parity sows <strong>and</strong> 3. Sows older than third parity.<br />

First parity sows were defined as sows that had weaned one litter when entering the service<br />

unit.<br />

The layout <strong>of</strong> the mating unit was divided into +/- escape possibilities (defined as +/- access<br />

to feeding stalls) whereas the pregnancy unit were categorised as <strong>group</strong> vs. individual feeding<br />

or +/- escape possibilities (defined as <strong>group</strong> sizes above 30 or access to feeding stalls).<br />

When analysing the effect <strong>of</strong> parity <strong>and</strong> layout, the following mixed model was applied:<br />

E(Yijklm )= µ + αi + βj + (αβ)ij + a·xijklm + Ak(j) + Bl(kj)<br />

In which Yijklm is the observed independent variable transformed by the natural logarithm<br />

(skin lesions), square root (% not eating <strong>and</strong> aggressions) or logit (% in box). E() indicates<br />

expected value. In case <strong>of</strong> the dichotomous categorical variables E(Yijklm) corresponds to<br />

logit to the probability <strong>of</strong> the observed outcome, pijklm. µ is the overall mean <strong>of</strong> the observa-<br />

67


- Paper III -<br />

tions, αi is the fixed effect <strong>of</strong> parity <strong>group</strong>, βj is the fixed effect <strong>of</strong> layout, (αβ)ij is the interac-<br />

tion between parity <strong>group</strong> <strong>and</strong> layout, xijklm is the back fat at weaning or three weeks after<br />

mating, a is the regression parameter, Ak(j) <strong>and</strong> Bl(kj) are the normal distributed r<strong>and</strong>om ef-<br />

fects <strong>of</strong> herd (within layout) <strong>and</strong> batch (within herd) respectively. For all continuous indica-<br />

tors, Yijklm ~ N(E(Yijklm), σijklm 2 ) whereas for all categorical indicators, Yijklm ~ B(1, pijklm).<br />

The covariate xijklm was only included in the model <strong>of</strong> the indicator back fat gain. Further,<br />

when analysing the effect <strong>of</strong> herd <strong>and</strong> batch, βj <strong>and</strong> (αβ)ij were excluded. Effects with Pvalues<br />

above 0.10 were eliminated from the model one by one <strong>and</strong> the analysis was repeated.<br />

For all continuous indicators, the statistical analyses were performed with a linear mixed<br />

model using the MIXED procedure (Littell et al., 1996) in SAS ® (SAS Institute Inc. 1990).<br />

For all categorical indicators, the statistical analyses were performed with a generalized<br />

linear mixed model using the glmmPQL function in the MASS package (Venables & Ripley<br />

2002) <strong>of</strong> R (R Development Core Team 2004). Effect <strong>of</strong> herd <strong>and</strong> batch within herd<br />

(r<strong>and</strong>om effects) were analysed by a Wald Z-test that provides an approximate test that the<br />

variance components are zero. For all continuous indicators, this was done by including the<br />

option COVTEST in the MIXED procedure (Littell et al., 1996), <strong>and</strong> a similar test was calculated<br />

in R. The test for variance heterogeneity (test for different between-sow variation in<br />

herds with <strong>group</strong> feeding <strong>and</strong> herds with individual feeding) for all back fat measurements<br />

was performed by comparing a model with homogeneous covariance structure to a model<br />

with heterogeneous covariance structure by means <strong>of</strong> the likelihood ratio test. For all continuous<br />

indicators, the option GROUP in the MIXED procedure was used to specify heterogeneity<br />

in the covariance structure (Littell et al., 1996).<br />

The correlation between continuous indicators was calculated using the CORR procedure<br />

(SAS Institute Inc. 1990). The calculations were based on data transformed to obtain normality<br />

<strong>and</strong> corrected for herd <strong>and</strong> batch mean.<br />

3. Results<br />

For all continuous indicators, the overall averages <strong>and</strong> different measurements <strong>of</strong> the variation<br />

between sows across all 14 herds are presented in Table 3. For all lesions- <strong>and</strong> aggressions<br />

measurements, the 50% quantile was lower than the mean value, which indicates that<br />

these variables were not normally distributed but skewed to the right.<br />

68


- Paper III -<br />

Table 3. Overall mean <strong>and</strong> variation shown as the st<strong>and</strong>ard deviations (Std), the minimum (Min) <strong>and</strong><br />

maximum (Max) values, the median (M50) <strong>and</strong> the 5% (Q5) <strong>and</strong> 95% (Q95) quantiles for all continuous<br />

indicators (sow level)<br />

N Mean Std Min Max M50 Q5 Q95<br />

Back fat W mm 551 15 5 6 35 14 9 23<br />

Back fat AW mm 524 16 4 7 34 15 10 24<br />

Back fat F mm 428 18 6 6 43 17 10 30<br />

Back fat gain W → AM mm/day 481 0.03 0.07 -0.19 0.27 0.03 -0.08 0.16<br />

Back fat gain AM → F mm/day 387 0.03 0.04 -0.11 0.17 0.02 -0.03 0.1<br />

Not eating W % 19 54 - 0 98 52 0 98<br />

Not eating M % 62 46 - 0 100 47 4 92<br />

Not eating AM % 299 27 - 0 100 21 0 74<br />

Lesions W cm 554 3 - 0 162 0 0 14<br />

Lesions M cm 542 66 - 0 330 48 3 181<br />

Lesions AM cm 543 76 - 0 384 63 6 197<br />

Lesions F cm 456 46 - 0 249 31 0 143<br />

Lesions W no. 554 2 - 0 100 0 0 12<br />

Lesions M no. 542 28 - 0 171 23 1 72<br />

Lesions AM no.. 543 37 - 0 208 30 3 86<br />

Lesions F no. 456 26 - 0 188 18 0 78<br />

Aggressions W no. 552 4.0 - 0 66.0 3 0 13<br />

Aggressions M no.. 99 0.7 - 0 7.0 0 0 3.0<br />

Aggressions AM no. 479 1.5 - 0 21 1 0 5<br />

Aggressions int 3+4 1) W no. 552 2.5 - 0 36 2 0 8<br />

Aggressions int 3+4 1) M no. 99 0.3 - 0 3 0 0 2<br />

Aggressions int 3+4 1) AM no. 479 0.3 - 0 6 0 0 2<br />

% in box W % 348 63 - 0 100 71 0 100<br />

% in box AM % 74 95 - 0 100 100 75 100<br />

W: Weaning M: Mating AM: Three weeks after mating F: Farrowing<br />

1) Only serious interactions, 3 (several bites/knocks/pushes) <strong>and</strong> 4 (bites from both sows).<br />

The variation cannot be presented as variation between sows for all lying <strong>and</strong> fear measurements<br />

because these indicators are categorical. Instead the variations <strong>of</strong> these indicators<br />

are presented as the variation between batches (Table 4).<br />

Table 4. Mean <strong>and</strong> variation shown as the minimum (Min) <strong>and</strong> maximum (Max) values, the median<br />

(M50) <strong>and</strong> the 25% (Q25) <strong>and</strong> 75% (Q75) quantiles for all categorical indicators (%sows per batch)<br />

N Mean Min Max M50 Q25 Q75<br />

Lying W 56 26 0 100 20 10 40<br />

Lying AM 52 68 0 100 80 55 90<br />

Lying alone W 52 25 0 30 10 0 11<br />

Lying alone AM 55 8 0 70 20 10 30<br />

Fear test (HA) 53 30 0 90 22 10 50<br />

Fear test (FHA) M 8 63 22 100 72 30 89<br />

Fear test (FHA) AM 56 47 0 100 50 30 60<br />

Fear test (FHT) M 8 49 0 89 50 34 71<br />

Fear test (FHT) AM 56 48 0 100 50 30 66<br />

W: Weaning M: Mating AM: Three weeks after mating F: Farrowing<br />

3.1 Relation between indicators<br />

The correlations between the indicators are shown in Table 5. Back fat measured at different<br />

times were highly correlated. Further, although less pronounced, correlations between<br />

back fat <strong>and</strong> back fat gain existed. High back fat at weaning was negative correlated with<br />

back fat gain from weaning to three weeks after weaning but positive correlated to back fat<br />

69


- Paper III -<br />

gain from three weeks after mating to farrowing. The skin lesion measurements, taken at<br />

different times were also significant interrelated.<br />

Also significant correlations between different indicators existed. Back fat measured at all<br />

three stages were e.g. negatively correlated to lesions (both number <strong>and</strong> length) three weeks<br />

after weaning <strong>and</strong> at the following farrowing. Aggressions at weaning were positive correlated<br />

to back fat gain, but negatively correlated to % not eating <strong>and</strong> lesions. This indicates<br />

that sows, involved in few aggressions at weaning have a low feed intake <strong>and</strong> receive many<br />

aggressions during pregnancy.<br />

70


Table 5. Correlations between all continuous indicators (transformed to obtain approximate normality <strong>and</strong> corrected for herd <strong>and</strong> batch mean). Correlations<br />

in bold are significant<br />

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19<br />

Back fat. mm<br />

1. W *** *** *** ** ns ns ns ns * ** ns * * ns ns ns ns ns<br />

2. AM 0.85 *** *** ** ns ns ** ns *** *** ns ** *** ns ns ns ns ns<br />

3. F 0.70 0.75 ** *** ns ns *** ns *** *** ns *** *** * ns ns ns ns<br />

Back fat gain, mm/day<br />

4. W to AM -0.23 0.23 0.20 ns ns ** ** ns ** ns ns ** ns ** ** ns ns ns<br />

5. AM to F 0.14 0.15 0.68 0.02 ns ns ** ns ** *** ns *** *** * ns ns ns ns<br />

Not eating, %<br />

6. W 0.36 0.25 0.19 -0.08 -0.16 ** ns ns ns ns ns ns ns ns ns * - -<br />

7. M 0.34 0.05 0.01 -0.43 -0.01 0.85 ns ns ns ns ns ns ns ns * ns - -<br />

8. AM -0.09 -0.18 -0.30 -0.20 -0.27 0.24 0.15 ns *** ns ns *** ns ** ns ns ns -<br />

Lesions, cm<br />

9. M -


- Paper III -<br />

3.2 Between-farm <strong>and</strong> within-farm variation <strong>of</strong> indicators<br />

In Figure 1, the variation between herds <strong>and</strong> between batches within herds are shown for<br />

some <strong>of</strong> the indicators <strong>and</strong> in Table 6, the effects <strong>of</strong> herd <strong>and</strong> batches are given.<br />

Table 6. Effect <strong>of</strong> herd <strong>and</strong> farrowing batch within herd on level <strong>of</strong> indicators<br />

Variance component % <strong>of</strong> variation explained Significance<br />

Herd (σk 2 ) Batch (σl(k) 2 ) Sow (ε ) Herd Batch Sow Herd Batch<br />

Back fat W 1.8 2 1.1 2 4.0 2 16 5 79 * *<br />

Back fat AM 1.5 2 1.2 2 3.8 2 12 8 80 * *<br />

Back fat F 2.8 2 0 5.0 2 23 0 77 * -<br />

Back fat gain W → AM 0.01 2 0.02 2 0.06 2 5 8 87 ns *<br />

Back fat gain AM → F 0.02 2 0.008 2 0.03 2 31 5 64 * ns<br />

Lesions, No. M 0.6 2 0.4 2 0.9 2 28 9 63 * **<br />

Lesions, No. AM 0.5 2 0.4 2 0.8 2 24 14 62 * **<br />

Lesions, No. F 1.1 2 0.3 2 1.0 2 52 5 43 ** *<br />

% not eating AM 1.3 2 0.7 2 1.9 2 29 7 64 * *<br />

Aggressions W 0.5 2 0.4 2 0.9 2 22 11 67 * **<br />

Aggressions AM 0.4 2 0.3 2 0.7 2 22 10 67 * **<br />

% in box W 0.6 2 0.8 2 2.1 2 7 12 81 ns *<br />

Lying alone W 0.9 2 1.0 2 - a) 45 55 - a) ** ***<br />

Lying alone AM 0.7 2 0.03 2 - a) 99


Back fat W, mm<br />

Back fat gain, AM-F, mm/day<br />

Skin lesions AM, no.<br />

30<br />

25<br />

20<br />

15<br />

10<br />

5<br />

0<br />

0,12<br />

0,07<br />

0,02<br />

-0,03<br />

-0,08<br />

-0,13<br />

100<br />

90<br />

80<br />

70<br />

60<br />

50<br />

40<br />

30<br />

20<br />

10<br />

0<br />

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1011121314<br />

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1011121314<br />

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1011121314<br />

Figure 1. Variation between the four batches within the 14 herds<br />

Back fat AM, mm<br />

% not eating AM<br />

Skin lesions F, no.<br />

30<br />

25<br />

20<br />

15<br />

10<br />

5<br />

0<br />

70<br />

60<br />

50<br />

40<br />

30<br />

20<br />

10<br />

0<br />

100<br />

90<br />

80<br />

70<br />

60<br />

50<br />

40<br />

30<br />

20<br />

10<br />

0<br />

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1011121314<br />

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1011121314<br />

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1011121314<br />

Back fat F, mm<br />

Aggressions W, no.<br />

Fear test (HA), % sows<br />

30<br />

25<br />

20<br />

15<br />

10<br />

5<br />

0<br />

20<br />

15<br />

10<br />

5<br />

0<br />

100<br />

90<br />

80<br />

70<br />

60<br />

50<br />

40<br />

30<br />

20<br />

10<br />

0<br />

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1011121314<br />

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1011121314<br />

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1011121314<br />

Back fat gain W-AM, mm/day<br />

Skin lesions M, no<br />

Fear test (FHA), % sows<br />

0,12<br />

0,07<br />

0,02<br />

-0,03<br />

-0,08<br />

-0,13<br />

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 1112 1314<br />

100<br />

90<br />

80<br />

70<br />

60<br />

50<br />

40<br />

30<br />

20<br />

10<br />

0<br />

100<br />

90<br />

80<br />

70<br />

60<br />

50<br />

40<br />

30<br />

20<br />

10<br />

0<br />

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1011121314<br />

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1011121314


- Paper III -<br />

Table 7. The between-sow variation in back fat measurements in herds with <strong>group</strong> feeding <strong>and</strong> herds<br />

with individual feeding<br />

Variance component (variation between sows)<br />

Group Individual Significance<br />

Back fat W 4.2 2 3.5 2 *<br />

Back fat AM 3.9 2 3.6 2 NS<br />

Back fat F 5.6 2 3.9 2 ***<br />

Back fat gain W → AM 0.069 2 0.055 2 **<br />

Back fat gain AM → F 0.037 2 0.024 2 ***<br />

W: Weaning AM: Three weeks after mating F: Farrowing<br />

*. **. ***: Significant effect (P=


- Paper III -<br />

from three weeks after weaning to farrowing, no effect <strong>of</strong> back fat three weeks after mating<br />

(P>0.1), feeding system (P>0.1) or interaction between parity <strong>and</strong> feeding system (P>0.1)<br />

was found.<br />

As shown in Table 9, % not eating three weeks after mating differed significantly between<br />

parity <strong>group</strong>s (P=0.01) in that sows older than third parity were spending more time eating<br />

compared to younger sows.<br />

75


Table 8. Effect <strong>of</strong> parity on back fat at weaning, three weeks after mating <strong>and</strong> at farrowing <strong>and</strong> back fat gain from weaning to three weeks after mating in<br />

herds with <strong>group</strong> feeding <strong>and</strong> herds with individual feeding shown as LS-MEANS <strong>and</strong> number <strong>of</strong> sows<br />

Feeding system Parity <strong>group</strong><br />

Back fat W<br />

LS-MEANS n<br />

Back fat AM<br />

LS-MEANS n<br />

Back fat F<br />

LS-MEANS n<br />

Back fat gain W→AM<br />

LS-MEANS n<br />

1 13.1 (84) 13.5 (79) 16.2 (53) 0.012 (72)<br />

Group<br />

Individual<br />

2 14.8 (141) 15.9 (131) 18.44 (115) 0.042 (123)<br />

3 17.3 (87) 18.3 (87) 22.2 (72) 0.062 (80)<br />

1 13.6 (69) 14.4 (68) 17.0 (51) 0.019 (65)<br />

2 14.4 (76) 15.3 (74) 17.0 (60) 0.026 (65)<br />

3 14.8 (93) 16.0 (88) 17.6 (77) 0.029 (75)<br />

P-value (parity <strong>group</strong> x feeding system)


- Paper III -<br />

Number <strong>of</strong> skin lesions. The number <strong>of</strong> skin lesions at mating were significant lower in<br />

herds with escape possibilities compared to herds without escape possibilities in the mating<br />

unit (13.7 vs. 30.9, n=400 vs. 142, P=0.01). There was no effect (P>0.1) <strong>of</strong> parity <strong>group</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

no interaction (P>0.1) between +/- escape possibilities <strong>and</strong> parity <strong>group</strong> for number <strong>of</strong> le-<br />

sions at mating.<br />

There was a significant interaction between parity <strong>group</strong> <strong>and</strong> escape possibility in the pregnancy<br />

unit for number <strong>of</strong> skin lesions three weeks after mating (P0.1) was found on aggressions<br />

three weeks after mating <strong>and</strong> no parity <strong>group</strong> x escape interaction (P>0.1). There<br />

was no effect <strong>of</strong> parity <strong>group</strong> on % in box at weaning (P>0.1).<br />

Lying behaviour. As shown in Table 9, parity <strong>group</strong> affected the probability <strong>of</strong> lying solitary<br />

at weaning (p=0.04) in that, the probability <strong>of</strong> lying solitary was higher for sows older<br />

than third parity compared to younger sows. There was no effect <strong>of</strong> +/- escape possibilities<br />

(P=0.6) on lying solitary at weaning <strong>and</strong> no effect <strong>of</strong> parity <strong>group</strong> (P=0.3), +/- escape possibilities<br />

(P=0.6) or interaction (P=0.4) for lying solitary three weeks after mating.<br />

Fear tests. In the HA-test test, first litter sows were more likely to be tested fearful compared<br />

to the other parity <strong>group</strong>s (P=0.03) as shown in Table 9. No main effect <strong>of</strong> parity<br />

<strong>group</strong> (P= 0.3, 0.4), +/- escape possibilities (P=0.5, 0.4) or interaction between parity <strong>group</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> +/- escape possibilities (P=0.3, 0.8) were found for the FHA-test or the FHT-test, respectively.<br />

4. Discussion<br />

With a few exceptions as mentioned in 2.3, the indicators were possible to perform in<br />

commercial sow herds at the planned stages at weaning, mating, three weeks after mating<br />

<strong>and</strong>/or at the following farrowing.<br />

77


- Paper III -<br />

Almost all indicators were influenced by herd <strong>and</strong> batches within herd. This suggests that<br />

the indicators are sensitive to herd- <strong>and</strong> batch-specific factors. Furthermore, it seems that all<br />

indicators, except % in box, lying solitary three weeks after mating <strong>and</strong> the two fear tests<br />

performed three weeks after first mating were sensitive to the sow-specific factor, parity.<br />

With the above-mentioned <strong>and</strong> the theoretical background (Kongsted, 2004a) in mente,<br />

there are indications that the back fat measurements, the skin lesion assessments <strong>and</strong> the<br />

behavioural observations might be relevant indicators <strong>of</strong> the sows’ condition especially in<br />

relation to feed intake, stress <strong>and</strong> fear at herd, batch <strong>and</strong> individual sow level in commercial<br />

herds with <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows.<br />

For almost all indicators, the variation between herds was larger than the variation within<br />

herd between batches. This suggests that factors that might differ between herds, e.g. layout,<br />

management <strong>and</strong> genetics, influence the indicators more than factors which might differ<br />

between batches like e.g. climatic <strong>conditions</strong>. When the layout factors employed in the<br />

later analyses were taken into account, the percentage <strong>of</strong> variation explained by herd decreased<br />

only with a few percentage points (


- Paper III -<br />

In herds practising <strong>group</strong> feeding in the pregnancy department, back fat at farrowing de-<br />

creased significantly with decreasing parity number. This was not the case in herds with<br />

individual feeding. Since the same pattern was seen already at weaning, this may partly be a<br />

result <strong>of</strong> the sows’ condition at weaning more than a result <strong>of</strong> the sows condition throughout<br />

the <strong>non</strong>-lactating period (qua the high correlations between the back fat measurements<br />

between different measurement day). However, as the interaction between system <strong>and</strong> parity<br />

<strong>group</strong> became more <strong>and</strong> more significant for every day <strong>of</strong> measurement, it indicates that<br />

high-parity sows are favoured in herds with <strong>group</strong> feeding to a much larger extent compared<br />

to herds with individual feeding. This is further supported by the higher increase in<br />

back fat gain from weaning to three weeks in pregnancy with increasing parity <strong>group</strong> seen<br />

in the <strong>group</strong> feeding systems. The effect <strong>of</strong> parity <strong>group</strong> on back fat gain from three weeks<br />

after mating to farrowing did not differ between feeding systems. However when including<br />

the interaction, although the <strong>non</strong>-significance (P=0.17) the same trend was seen (results not<br />

shown). In addition, the behavioural observations showed that sows older than third parity<br />

spend significant more time eating compared to first to third parity sows in the <strong>group</strong> feeding<br />

herds.<br />

As several studies have found a positive correlation between parity <strong>and</strong> rank (Arey & Edwards,<br />

1998), it seems that the low ranked sows had less access to feed compared to the<br />

older high ranked sows in <strong>group</strong> feeding systems. This is supported by results from a previously<br />

field study that indicated lower feed intake in low compared to high ranking sows, as<br />

indicated by less increase in chest girth (Olsson & Svendsen, 1997). Also several experimental<br />

studies have indicated lower feed intake in low compared to high ranking sows in<br />

<strong>group</strong> feeding systems, as indicated by lower weight gain (Brouns & Edwards, 1994; Ruis<br />

et al., 2002), less time spend at the central area <strong>of</strong> the pile <strong>of</strong> feed provided on the floor<br />

(Csermely & Wood-Gush, 1990) <strong>and</strong> less time spent at the trough (Andersen et al., 1999).<br />

The result <strong>of</strong> the back fat measurements implies that <strong>group</strong> fed sows in average are provided<br />

with a larger amount <strong>of</strong> feed compared to individual fed sows probably in an attempt<br />

to achieve that all sows, also the low ranked get adequate amount <strong>of</strong> feed during pregnancy.<br />

The results <strong>of</strong> this study indicate an overfeeding <strong>of</strong> sows older than third parity more than<br />

an underfeeding <strong>of</strong> the young sows when looking upon average data. However, in the herds<br />

with <strong>group</strong> feeding, six % <strong>of</strong> all sows had back fat depth less than 10 mm at farrowing. In<br />

addition, in the <strong>group</strong> feeding systems, 11 out <strong>of</strong> 256 sows ate less than 20 % <strong>of</strong> all observations<br />

during feeding <strong>and</strong> four sows did not eat at all three weeks after mating. In this<br />

study, all the sows in the <strong>group</strong> feeding herds were fed amounts <strong>of</strong> feed below their capacity<br />

for feed intake during pregnancy (Brouns et al., 1991). It is therefore presumable that<br />

the majority <strong>of</strong> sows were motivated to eat (Jensen et al., 2000) <strong>and</strong> that sows, which did<br />

not eat were displaced from the feed or ‘chose’ to stay away to avoid aggressions. Taken<br />

79


- Paper III -<br />

together, the results from this study indicate a serious welfare problem for a few sows that<br />

are not able to cope in this kind <strong>of</strong> system. Also, the apparently overfeeding <strong>of</strong> the older<br />

sows may constitute both a welfare- <strong>and</strong> productivity problem. Locomotion problems<br />

(Dourmad et al., 1994) <strong>and</strong> a low feed intake during lactation, which may reduce ovulation<br />

rate <strong>and</strong> embryo survival (Zak et al., 1997; Han et al., 2000) <strong>and</strong> perhaps even conception<br />

rate (Hughes et al., 1984) are well-known consequences <strong>of</strong> high feed intake during pregnancy.<br />

It is therefore <strong>of</strong> outmost importance that managers <strong>of</strong> <strong>group</strong> feeding systems try to<br />

avoid this unequal feed allocation. Possible management initiatives might be assuring sufficient<br />

‘isolation pens’ for sows, which are not able to cope in the system, providing the feed<br />

on a larger area in systems with floor feeding. In systems with liquid feeding, setting up<br />

body partitions may have a positive effect (Andersen et al., 1999), however, it presupposes<br />

that the feed are equally divided in the trough, which is difficult to achieve in practice<br />

(Olsson et al., 1993).<br />

Aggressions, skin lesions <strong>and</strong> lying behaviour. Since it is well known that mixing <strong>of</strong> unfamiliar<br />

sows causes fighting between sows to settle social ranking (Arey & Edwards, 1998)<br />

it is not surprising that the level <strong>of</strong> aggressions were highest on the day <strong>of</strong> weaning. The<br />

first hour after mixing, the individual sow was in average involved in four aggressions,<br />

varying from 0 to 66. The study indicates that feeding stalls have a reducing effect on aggressions<br />

the first hour after mixing, as also found in a previous experimental study<br />

(Barnett et al., 1992). In view <strong>of</strong> the fact that sows in average spend 63 % <strong>of</strong> all observations<br />

in box the first hour after mixing; this is not surprising. Irrespective <strong>of</strong> layout, sows<br />

older than third parity were involved in more aggressions compared to younger sows the<br />

first hour after mixing. As sows had access to feeding stalls in the majority <strong>of</strong> herds, one<br />

explanation for this could be that the lower ranked sows stayed in the stalls during the most<br />

serious hierarchical fights to avoid aggressions.<br />

In general, as also found in other field studies (Gjein & Larssen, 1995; Andersen & Bøe,<br />

1999; Leeb et al., 2001), the skin lesions were mainly superficial lesions. The total number<br />

<strong>and</strong> length <strong>of</strong> skin lesions approximately five days after mixing was 28 <strong>and</strong> 66 cm respectively.<br />

In an experimental study with comparable scoring method it was found that number<br />

<strong>of</strong> skin lesions varied in average from 11.1 to 17.0 <strong>and</strong> length <strong>of</strong> skin lesions in average<br />

from 19.4 to 28.0 cm ten days after mixing in sows <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> in various pen designs<br />

(Barnett et al., 1992). The level <strong>of</strong> skin lesions has been assessed in several field studies (de<br />

Koning, 1985; Gjein & Larssen, 1995; Olsson & Svendsen, 1997; Andersen & Bøe, 1999;<br />

Leeb et al., 2001), however, due to differences in measurement <strong>and</strong> scoring methods it is<br />

difficult to use the results <strong>of</strong> these studies as comparison.<br />

80


- Paper III -<br />

Previous work have found that mixing <strong>of</strong> sows into small <strong>group</strong>s (12 <strong>and</strong> 3 sows respec-<br />

tively) lead to the highest level <strong>of</strong> skin injuries (Olsson & Svendsen, 1997) <strong>and</strong> the highest<br />

<strong>and</strong> longest lasting elevated level <strong>of</strong> cortisol (Tsuma et al., 1996) in low ranked sows. In<br />

contrast, Mendl et al. (1992) reported that if sows where mixed into a large <strong>group</strong> (37 gilts),<br />

the intermediate sows received the highest level <strong>of</strong> aggressions <strong>and</strong> had the highest level <strong>of</strong><br />

cortisol. The dominating sows received the lowest level <strong>of</strong> aggressions <strong>and</strong> the submissive<br />

sows were in between. These results indicate that when little free space is available, the<br />

submissive sows are the ones receiving most aggressions probably because they receive<br />

aggressions from both the dominating <strong>and</strong> the intermediate sows, whereas the intermediate<br />

mainly receives aggressions from the dominating sows. In contrast, in large <strong>group</strong>s the<br />

most submissive sows have more chances to avoid the more dominant sows due to more<br />

available free space. Accordingly, this study found that in herds with no escape possibilities<br />

(small <strong>group</strong> sizes <strong>and</strong> no feeding stalls) in the pregnancy unit, first parity (<strong>and</strong> probably<br />

low ranked) sows had significant more lesions compared to older sows three weeks after<br />

mating <strong>and</strong> at farrowing, whereas in herds with escape possibilities, the second-third parity<br />

(<strong>and</strong> probably middle ranked) sows had the highest number <strong>of</strong> skin injuries.<br />

Although not significant, more skin lesions were observed in herds with escape possibilities<br />

in the pregnancy unit, especially at farrowing. One explanation for this could be that the<br />

<strong>group</strong> <strong>of</strong> herds with escape possibilities include the two herds with ESF <strong>and</strong> large dynamic<br />

<strong>group</strong>s with constantly introduction <strong>of</strong> new sows into the <strong>group</strong>. As seen in Figure 1, these<br />

two herds (herd 7 <strong>and</strong> 15) had the highest level <strong>of</strong> lesions at farrowing. This is in agreement<br />

with Leeb et al. (2001) who found significant more lesions in herds with dynamic <strong>group</strong>s<br />

compared to herds with stable <strong>group</strong>s in a field trial that involved 55 herds <strong>and</strong> O'Connell et<br />

al. (2003) who found a higher injury level in dynamic compared to stable <strong>group</strong>s in an<br />

experimental study.<br />

In accordance with Olsson & Svendsen (1995), the frequency <strong>of</strong> lying solitary was highest<br />

on the day <strong>of</strong> weaning. Further, the likelihood <strong>of</strong> lying solitary at weaning was highest for<br />

sows older than third parity, which were also the sows involved in most aggressions at<br />

weaning. Accordingly, Bonde (2004) found that solitary lying behaviour was positive correlated<br />

to involvement in aggressive interactions. Since there was no effect <strong>of</strong> parity <strong>group</strong><br />

on the probability <strong>of</strong> lying at weaning (P=0.8, results not shown) the results was not solely<br />

a consequence <strong>of</strong> old sows lying more <strong>of</strong>ten than younger sows.<br />

Fear tests. According to the results from the human approach test performed at weaning,<br />

first parity sows were more likely to be categorised as fearful compared to older sows. This<br />

trend could however not be rediscovered in the two fear tests three weeks after mating in<br />

the home arena <strong>of</strong> the sows. The test arena for the human approach tests differed inevitable<br />

81


- Paper III -<br />

between the 14 herds. As system differences previously have shown to influence the results<br />

<strong>of</strong> human approach tests (Marchant-Forde et al., 2003), the between-herd variation should<br />

be interpreted with caution.<br />

For managers <strong>of</strong> herds with <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> sows, information <strong>of</strong> the sows' condition would<br />

be very useful in relation to analysing <strong>and</strong> improving animal welfare <strong>and</strong> perhaps also productivity.<br />

Since it is likely that the sows’ general condition, to a large extent is influenced<br />

by management related factors, it may be difficult to predict the presence <strong>of</strong> e.g. stress, fear<br />

<strong>and</strong> unequal feed intake merely on the knowledge <strong>of</strong> layout related factors like feeding system<br />

<strong>and</strong> escape possibilities. Therefore, it seems important that managers are able to make<br />

an on site judgement <strong>of</strong> the sows’ condition. The indicators presented in this paper are possible<br />

to perform in practice, <strong>and</strong> taken together the results indicate that they could be relevant<br />

indicators <strong>of</strong> social stress, fear <strong>and</strong> feed intake. As a consequence, these indicators<br />

might be relevant ingredients <strong>of</strong> a management tool to analyse <strong>and</strong> improve the welfare <strong>and</strong><br />

productivity <strong>of</strong> <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> sows.<br />

Acknowledgements<br />

The authors wish to thank the participating farmers <strong>and</strong> the technicians Henrik K. Andersen,<br />

Kristine R. Hansen, Michael Hansen, Orla Nielsen, Niels H. Thomsen, <strong>and</strong> Helge Yde<br />

for their much appreciated assistance in carrying out the data collection. The authors also<br />

wish to express their gratitude to Erik Jørgensen for valuable contributions to the statistical<br />

analyses.<br />

82


References<br />

- Paper III -<br />

Andersen, I.L. 2003. Ph.D. Department <strong>of</strong> Animal <strong>and</strong> Aquacultural Sciences, Agricultural University <strong>of</strong><br />

Norway, P.P. Box 5065, 1432 Aas, Norway. Personal communication.<br />

Andersen, I.L. & Bøe, K.E. 1999. Straw bedding or concrete floor for loose-<strong>housed</strong> pregnant sows: Conse-<br />

quences for aggression, production <strong>and</strong> physical health. Acta Agric. Sc<strong>and</strong>., Sect. A, Anim. Sci. 49,<br />

190-195.<br />

Andersen, I.L., Bøe, K. & Kristiansen, A.L. 1999. The influence <strong>of</strong> different feeding arrangements <strong>and</strong> food<br />

type on competition at feeding in pregnant sows. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 65, 91-104.<br />

Andersen, I.L., Berg, S., Bøe, K.E. & Edwards, S. 2003. Effect <strong>of</strong> short term h<strong>and</strong>ling on fear <strong>of</strong> humans <strong>and</strong><br />

the consequences for maternal abilities <strong>of</strong> sows. Proceedings <strong>of</strong> the 37th International Congress <strong>of</strong><br />

the ISAE, Abano Terme, Italy, June 24-28, 2003. Abstract, Pp. 79<br />

Arey, D. & Edwards, S.A. 1998. Factors influencing aggression between sows after mixing <strong>and</strong> the concequences<br />

for welfare <strong>and</strong> production. Livest. Prod. Sci. 56, 61-70.<br />

Barnett, J.L., Cronin, G.M. & Winfield, C.G. 1981. The effects <strong>of</strong> individual <strong>and</strong> <strong>group</strong> penning <strong>of</strong> pigs on<br />

total <strong>and</strong> free plasma corticosteroids <strong>and</strong> the maximum corticosteroid binding capacity. Gen. Comp.<br />

Endocrinol., 44, 219-225.<br />

Barnett, J.L., Cronin, G.M., Winfield, C.G. & Dewar, A.M. 1984. The welfare <strong>of</strong> adult pigs: the effects <strong>of</strong> five<br />

housing treatments on behaviour, plasma corticosteroids <strong>and</strong> injuries. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 12,<br />

209-232.<br />

Barnett, J.L., Hemsworth, P.H., Cronin, G.M., Newman, E.A., McCallum, T.H. & Chilton, D. 1992. Effects<br />

<strong>of</strong> pen size, partial stalls <strong>and</strong> method <strong>of</strong> feeding on welfare-related behavioural <strong>and</strong> physiological responses<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>group</strong>-<strong>housed</strong> pigs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci34, 207-220.<br />

Baustad, B. & Lium, B. 2002. Helse og dyrevelferd i norsk svineproduksjon sett i et internasjonalt perspektiv.<br />

Norsk Vet. tidsskrift 114, 87-91.<br />

Bonde, M., 2003. Welfare Assessment in a Commercial Sow herd. Development, evaluation <strong>and</strong> report <strong>of</strong> the<br />

method. Revised reprint <strong>of</strong> PhD thesis by Marianne Kjær Bonde. DIAS Report 46 (Animal Husb<strong>and</strong>ry).<br />

Danish Institute <strong>of</strong> Agricultural Sciences. 98pp.<br />

Bonde, M., Rousing, T. & Sørensen, J.T. 2003. Human approach test for on-farm use in loose-<strong>housed</strong> pregnant<br />

sows: relation between behaviour <strong>and</strong> health. Proceedings <strong>of</strong> the 37th International Congress <strong>of</strong><br />

the ISAE, Abano Terme, Italy, June 24-28, 2003. Abstract, pp. 81.<br />

Bradshaw, R.H., Skyrme, J., Brenninkmiejer, E.E. & Broom, D.M. 2000. Consistency <strong>of</strong> measurement <strong>of</strong><br />

social status in dry-sows <strong>group</strong>-<strong>housed</strong> in indoor <strong>and</strong> outdoor systems. Anim. Welfare 9, 75-79.<br />

Brouns, F. & Edwards, S.A. 1994. Social rank <strong>and</strong> feeding behaviour <strong>of</strong> <strong>group</strong>-<strong>housed</strong> sows fed competitively<br />

or ad libitum. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci39, 225-235.<br />

Brouns, F., Edwards, S.A. & English, P.R. 1991. Fibrous raw materials in sow diets: Effects on voluntary<br />

food intake, digestibility <strong>and</strong> diurnal activity patterns. Anim. Prod. 52, 598.<br />

83


- Paper III -<br />

Charette, R., Bigras-Poulin. M. & Martineau, G.-P. 1996. Body condition evaluation in sows. Livest. Prod.<br />

Sci. 46, 107-115.<br />

Csermely, D. & Wood-Gush, D.G.M. 1990. Agonistic behaviour in <strong>group</strong>ed sows. II. How social rank affects<br />

feeding <strong>and</strong> drinking behaviour. Boll. Zool. 57, 55-58.<br />

de Koning, R. 1985. On the well-being <strong>of</strong> dry sows. Ph.D. Thesis. 170pp.<br />

Dourmad, J.Y., Étienne, M., Prunier, A. & Noblet, J. 1994. The effect <strong>of</strong> energy <strong>and</strong> protein intake <strong>of</strong> sows on<br />

their longevity: a review. Livest. Prod. Sci. 40, 87-97.<br />

Fisker, B.N., 1994. Løsgående gruppefodrede søer. Meddelelse 278, Den rullende Afprøvning, L<strong>and</strong>sudvalget<br />

for Svin, Danske Slagterier, 10pp.<br />

Fisker, B.N., 1999. Foder med 60 pct. pulpetter til drægtige søer opstaldet i stabile grupper. Meddelelse 444,<br />

Den rullende Afprøvning, L<strong>and</strong>sudvalget for Svin, Danske Slagterier, 11pp.<br />

Gjein, H. & Larssen, R.B. 1995. Housing <strong>of</strong> pregnant sows in loose <strong>and</strong> confined systems - a field study. 1.<br />

Vulva <strong>and</strong> body lesions, culling reasons <strong>and</strong> production results. Acta vet. Sc<strong>and</strong>. 36, 185-200.<br />

Han, I.K., Bosi, P., Hyan, Y., Kim, J.D., Sohn, K.S. & Kim, S.W. 2000. Recent advances in sow nutrition to<br />

improve reproductive performance. Asian-Australian J. <strong>of</strong> Anim. Sci. 13, 335-355.<br />

Hansen, L.U. 2000. Sammenligning af bio-fix fodring og gulvfodring til drægtige søer i stabile grupper. Meddelelse<br />

484, Den rullende Afprøvning, L<strong>and</strong>sudvalget for Svin, Danske Slagterier. 8pp.<br />

Hansen, L.U. & Kongsted, A.G. 2002. Gulvudformning i løbeafdeling med æde-/insemineringsbokse til løsgående<br />

søer. Meddelelse 559, Den rullende Afprøvning, L<strong>and</strong>sudvalget for Svin, Danske Slagterier,<br />

8pp.<br />

Hughes, P.E., Henry, R.W. & Pichard, D.W. 1984. The effect <strong>of</strong> lactation food level on subsequent ovulation<br />

rate <strong>and</strong> early embryonic survival in the sow. Anim. Prod. 38, 527.<br />

Jensen, K.H., Pedersen, B.K., Pedersen, L.J. & Jørgensen, E. 1995. Well-being in pregnant sows: Confinement<br />

versus <strong>group</strong> housing with electronic sow feeding. Acta Agric. Sc<strong>and</strong>. , Sect. A, Anim. Sci. 45,<br />

266-275.<br />

Jensen, K.H., Pedersen, L.J., Nielsen, E.K., Heller, K.N., Ladewig, J. & Jørgensen, E. 1996. Intermittent<br />

stress in pigs: Effects on behaviour, pituitary-adrenocortical axis, growth, <strong>and</strong> gastric ulceration.<br />

Phys. <strong>and</strong> behav. 59, 741-748.<br />

Jensen, K.H., Sørensen, L.S., Bertelsen, D., Pedersen, A.R., Jørgensen, E., Nielsen, N.-P. & Vestergaard, K.S.<br />

2000. Management factors affecting activity <strong>and</strong> aggression in dynamic <strong>group</strong> housing systems with<br />

electronic sow feeding: a field trial. Anim. sci. 71, 535-545.<br />

Jensen, K.H., Nielsen, B.L. & Olsen, A.N. 2002. Resultater fra forsøg vedr. udvikling af test til identifikation<br />

af sociale egenskaber hos søer, der indebærer forøget risiko for højt aggressionsniveau i en gruppe.<br />

Intern rapport 155, Danmarks JordbrugsForskning, 32pp.<br />

84


- Paper III -<br />

Kongsted, A.G. 2004a. Stress <strong>and</strong> fear as possible mediators <strong>of</strong> reproduction problems in <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> sows:<br />

A review. Accepted for publication in Acta. Agric. Sc<strong>and</strong>., Sect. A, Anim. Sci.<br />

Kongsted, A.G. 2004b. Effect <strong>of</strong> energy intake on pregnancy rate <strong>and</strong> litter size with particular reference to<br />

<strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows – a review. Submitted to Livest. Prod. Sci.. In revision.<br />

Leeb, B., Leeb, Ch. & Troxler, J.S.M. 2001. Skin lesions <strong>and</strong> callosities in <strong>group</strong>-<strong>housed</strong> pregnant sows: Ani-<br />

mal related welfare indicators. Acta Agric. Sc<strong>and</strong>. , Sect. A, Anim. Sci. Suppl. 30, 82-87.<br />

Littell, R. C., Milliken, G.A., Stroup, W.W. & Wolfinger, R.D. 1996. SAS ® System for mixed models. Cary,<br />

NC: SAS Institute Inc. 633 pp.<br />

Marchant-Forde, J.N., Bradshaw, R.H., Marchant, R.M. & Broom, D.M. 2003. A note on the effect <strong>of</strong> gesta-<br />

tion housing environment on approach test measures in gilts. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 80, 287-296.<br />

Maes, D.G.D. 2004. Ph.D. Department <strong>of</strong> <strong>Reproduction</strong>, Obstetrics <strong>and</strong> herd health, Faculty <strong>of</strong> Veterinary<br />

Medicine, Salisburylaan 133, 9820 Merelbeke, Belgium. Personal Communication.<br />

Mendl, M., Zanella, A.J. & Broom, D.M. 1992. Physiological <strong>and</strong> reproductive correlates <strong>of</strong> behavioural<br />

strategies in female domestic pigs. Anim. Behav. 44, 1107-1121.<br />

Nielsen, N.-P. 1995. Elektronisk s<strong>of</strong>odring. Meddelelse 312, Den rullende Afprøvning, L<strong>and</strong>sudvalget for<br />

Svin, Danske Slagterier. 9pp.<br />

Nielsen, N.-P., Hansen, L.U. &Calmann-Hinke, D. 2000. Stalde til løsgående søer. Rapport 17, L<strong>and</strong>sudval-<br />

get for Svin, Danske Slagterier, 33pp.<br />

O'Connell, N.E., Beattie, V.E. & Moss, B.W. 2003. Influence <strong>of</strong> social status on the welfare <strong>of</strong> sows in static<br />

<strong>and</strong> dynamic <strong>group</strong>s. Anim. Welf. 12, 239-249.<br />

Olsson, A.-C. & Samuelsson, O.V. 1993. Grouping studies <strong>of</strong> lactating <strong>and</strong> newly weaned sows. Collins, E.<br />

<strong>and</strong> Boon, C. 475-482. 1993. Proceedings <strong>of</strong> livestock environment coventry, UK, 6-9 July. American<br />

Society <strong>of</strong> Agricultural Engineers.<br />

Olsson, A.-C. & Svendsen, J. 1995. Problem och rutiner vid gruppering av suggor och gyltor. Rapport 96,<br />

Sveriges Lantbruksuniversitet, Institutionen för jordbrukets biosystem och teknologi (JBT), 52pp.<br />

Olsson, A.-C. & Svendsen, J. 1997. Effekter av olika konkurrensförhåll<strong>and</strong>en vid utfodring på suggors hälsa<br />

och produktion. SLU Rapport 113, Sveriges Lantbruksuniversitet, 37pp.<br />

Olsson, A.-C., Svendsen, J., Reese, D., Andersson, M. & Rantzer, D. 1993. Inhysning av dräktiga suggor i<br />

långsmala boxar med blötutfodring. Rapport 87, Sveriges lantbruksuniversitet, Institutionen för lantbrukets<br />

byggnadsteknik, Lund, 39pp.<br />

Pedersen, L.J., Damm, B.I. & Kongsted, A.G. 2003. The influence <strong>of</strong> adverse or gentle h<strong>and</strong>ling procedures<br />

on sexual behaviour in fearful <strong>and</strong> confident sows. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci83, 277-290.<br />

R Development Core Team, 2004. R: A language <strong>and</strong> environment for statistical computing. R foundation for<br />

statistical computing. http://www.R-Project.org.<br />

85


- Paper III -<br />

Rousing, T., Bonde, M. & Sørensen, J.T. 1999. Fear testing in loose housing systems for pregnant sows.<br />

Poster presentation. 1999. Proceedings <strong>of</strong> the 33rd International Congress <strong>of</strong> the international Society<br />

for Applied Ethology (ISAE), 17-21 august 1999, Lillehammer Norway.<br />

Ruis, M.A.W., te Brake, J.H.A., Engel, B., Buist, W.G., Blokhuis, H.J. & Koolhaas, J.M. 2002. Implications<br />

<strong>of</strong> coping characteristics <strong>and</strong> social status for welfare <strong>and</strong> production <strong>of</strong> paired growing gilts. Appl.<br />

Anim. Behav. Sci. 75, 207-231.<br />

SAS Institute Inc. 1990. SAS/STAT ® User's Guide. Gary, NC.<br />

Svendsen, J., Andersson, M., Olsson, A.-C., Rantzer, D. & Lundqvist, P. 1990. Grupphållning av drägtiga<br />

suggor i isolerade och oisolerade stallar. En beskrivning av resultaten från enkätunder - sökningar,<br />

gårdsbesök och grupperingsförsök. Rapport 66, Institutionen för lantbrukets byggnadsteknik, Sveriges<br />

Lantbruksuniversitet, 202pp.<br />

Thorup, F. 2004. Ph.D. The National Committee for Pig Production, Danish Bacon <strong>and</strong> Meat Council. Personal<br />

Communication. Axeltorv 3, 1609 Copenhagen V.<br />

Tsuma, V.T., Einarsson, S., Madej, A., Kindahl, H., Lundeheim, N. & Rojkittikhun, T. 1996. Endocrine changes<br />

during <strong>group</strong> housing <strong>of</strong> primaparous sows in early pregnancy. Acta vet. Sc<strong>and</strong>. 37, 481-490.<br />

Venables, W.N. & Ripley, B.D. 2002. Modern Applied statistics with S. Fourth Edition. Springer, ISBN 0-<br />

387-95457-0<br />

Zak, L.J., Cosgrove, J.R., Aherne, F.X & Foxcr<strong>of</strong>t, G.R. 1997. Pattern <strong>of</strong> feed intake <strong>and</strong> associated metabolic<br />

<strong>and</strong> endocrine changes differentially affect postweaning fertility on primiparous lactating sows. J.<br />

Anim. Sci. 75, 208-216.<br />

86


- Paper IV -<br />

Relation between reproduction performance <strong>and</strong> indicators <strong>of</strong> feed intake,<br />

fear <strong>and</strong> social stress in commercial herds with <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>lactating<br />

sows<br />

A.G. Kongsted<br />

Department <strong>of</strong> Agroecology, Danish Institute <strong>of</strong> Agricultural Sciences, P.O. Box 50, DK-<br />

8830 Tjele<br />

87<br />

ІV


- Paper IV -<br />

Abstract<br />

Group housing <strong>of</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows is becoming more <strong>and</strong> more widespread in commercial<br />

sow herds in several European countries as a result <strong>of</strong> changed legislations. Results<br />

from experimental studies suggest that <strong>group</strong> housing may lead to individual variation in<br />

feed intake, stress <strong>and</strong> fear, which may impair the reproduction performance. However,<br />

whether the individual variation in feed intake, stress <strong>and</strong> fear in sows <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> under<br />

commercial <strong>conditions</strong> is severe enough to impair the reproduction performance is not<br />

known. Therefore a detailed farm study including 14 herds (in total about 550 sows) with<br />

different layouts <strong>and</strong> management routines was carried out, <strong>and</strong> the relations between various<br />

indicators <strong>of</strong> feed intake, stress <strong>and</strong> fear <strong>and</strong> reproduction performance were studied.<br />

11.4 % <strong>of</strong> all mated sows were re-mated <strong>and</strong> average litter size was 14.8 born piglets per<br />

litter. Positive correlations between back fat gain from weaning to three weeks after mating<br />

<strong>and</strong> chance <strong>of</strong> pregnancy (P


- Paper IV -<br />

1. Introduction<br />

Group housing <strong>of</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows is becoming increasingly widespread in commercial<br />

sow herds in European countries as a result <strong>of</strong> changed EU-legislations (Council Directive<br />

2001/88/EC amending Directive 91/630/EEC Laying Down Minimum St<strong>and</strong>ards for the<br />

Protection <strong>of</strong> Pigs) <strong>and</strong> national extraordinary laws (Baustad & Lium, 2002; The welfare <strong>of</strong><br />

Farmed Animals (Engl<strong>and</strong>) (Amendment) Regulations 2003) initiated by elevated public<br />

concern <strong>of</strong> animal welfare. Although no legislation yet, similar tendencies are also seen in<br />

other parts <strong>of</strong> the world (Trezona, 2003).<br />

Impaired reproduction in the shape <strong>of</strong> reduced litter size or pregnancy rate has been observed<br />

in <strong>group</strong> compared to individually <strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows in several on-farm<br />

studies (Hurtgen et al., 1980; Fisker, 1995; Peltoniemi et al., 1999; Hansen, 2000). Conversely,<br />

in other studies, no difference (Engl<strong>and</strong> & Spurr, 1969) between <strong>group</strong>ed <strong>and</strong> individually<br />

<strong>housed</strong> sows or even opposite effects (Bates et al., 2003; Hansen, 2003) have been<br />

found. The divergent results are probably a result <strong>of</strong> differences in the function <strong>of</strong> the <strong>group</strong><br />

housing systems <strong>and</strong> shows that <strong>group</strong> housing do not ‘automatically’ lead to poor reproduction<br />

performance.<br />

Results from experimental studies, not necessarily reflecting <strong>conditions</strong> seen in practice<br />

have indicated that the impaired reproduction performance seen under some circumstances<br />

in <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows might be a result <strong>of</strong> social relations causing individual<br />

variation in energy intake (review by Kongsted, 2004b), fear <strong>and</strong> stress (review by Kongsted,<br />

2004a).<br />

Further, registrations <strong>of</strong> various indicators <strong>of</strong> feed intake, stress <strong>and</strong> fear in sows <strong>group</strong><br />

<strong>housed</strong> under various on-farm <strong>conditions</strong> suggest a large individual variation in these characteristics<br />

(Kongsted et al., 2004). However, whether these individual variations are severe<br />

enough to impair the reproduction performance in sows <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> in commercial herds<br />

is not known. Therefore, a detailed farm study including 14 commercial sow herds with<br />

different layout <strong>and</strong> management routines was carried out to investigate the relation between<br />

various indicators <strong>of</strong> feed intake, stress <strong>and</strong> fear <strong>and</strong> the reproduction performance in<br />

practice.<br />

2. Materials <strong>and</strong> methods<br />

2.1 Design <strong>and</strong> herds<br />

In 14 herds with <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows, ten focal sows (F-sows) in each <strong>of</strong> four<br />

batches were observed from weaning to farrowing. The ten sows were r<strong>and</strong>omly chosen in<br />

the lactation unit just before the sows were moved to the service unit. The F-sows’ back fat,<br />

89


- Paper IV -<br />

eating <strong>and</strong> lying behaviour, skin lesions <strong>and</strong> reaction in fear test were monitored at wean-<br />

ing, at mating <strong>and</strong>/or three weeks after mating.<br />

The study was conducted during an 11-month period from May 2003 to March 2004. The<br />

timing <strong>of</strong> the registrations differed maximum 1.5 months between the herds. The 14 herds<br />

were chosen to represent different layouts <strong>and</strong> management routines to ensure that any<br />

found correlations could be transferred to apply in a broad spectrum <strong>of</strong> herds. The herd<br />

sizes varied from 180 to 1000 sows. Feeding systems applied in the service unit were freeaccess<br />

feeding <strong>and</strong> insemination stalls (11 herds), providing feed in long trough (1 herd),<br />

floor feeding (1 herd) <strong>and</strong> bi<strong>of</strong>ix (1 herd). Feeding systems applied in the pregnancy unit<br />

were floor feeding (5 herds), providing feed in long trough (2 herds) or feed dispenser (1),<br />

Electronic Sow Feeding (2 herds), free-access feeding stalls (2 herds), individual feeding<br />

stalls (1 herd) <strong>and</strong> bi<strong>of</strong>ix (1 herd). See Kongsted et al. (2004) for a more detailed description<br />

<strong>of</strong> the farms.<br />

2.2 Recordings<br />

2.2.1 <strong>Reproduction</strong> <strong>and</strong> culling data<br />

For each F-sow, weaning date, date for first mating, date for re-mating for the sows that<br />

returned to oestrus after the first mating were recorded, farrowing date <strong>and</strong> the number <strong>of</strong><br />

born piglets (alive <strong>and</strong> stillborn). A sow was defined as pregnant after first mating if no<br />

date for re-mating was noted. For culled sows, the date <strong>of</strong> culling <strong>and</strong> estimated culling<br />

reason was recorded. The sows were only followed until re-mating, culling or farrowing.<br />

Therefore, the number <strong>of</strong> culled sows did not include any re-mated sows.<br />

2.2.2 Indicators<br />

The timing <strong>of</strong> the recordings <strong>of</strong> the indicators was first <strong>of</strong> all chosen to give information<br />

about the sows’ <strong>conditions</strong> from weaning to first mating <strong>and</strong> from first mating to three<br />

weeks in pregnancy because these stages are believed to be the main periods <strong>of</strong> relevance<br />

regarding reproduction performance (Kongsted, 2004ab). The motives for choosing the<br />

respective indicators are discussed in Kongsted (2004a) <strong>and</strong> a more thoroughly description<br />

<strong>of</strong> the recordings is provided in Kongsted et al. (2004).<br />

Back fat gain <strong>and</strong> eating behaviour were used as indicators for feed intake. Back fat depth<br />

was measured by means <strong>of</strong> a digital ultrasound back fat indicator LEAN MEATER ® (Baltic<br />

Korn A/S, Naestved, Denmark) 65 mm from either side <strong>of</strong> the backbone at the 12 th (last)<br />

<strong>and</strong> 10 th rib (conventionally known as P2-measurements) at weaning <strong>and</strong> three weeks after<br />

mating. Eating behaviour was only registered in the eight herds with <strong>group</strong> feeding in the<br />

pregnancy unit. For each sow, it was recorded whether the sow was eating or not for each<br />

half minute during feeding three weeks after mating. Number <strong>of</strong> times the sow was not eat-<br />

90


- Paper IV -<br />

ing compared to number <strong>of</strong> recordings (max 50) was calculated for each sow (% not eating).<br />

Three fear tests were performed as indicators <strong>of</strong> fear. One Human Approach test (HA-test)<br />

were performed when sows were routinely moved at weaning (Rousing et al., 1999; Bonde<br />

et al., 2003). The sow had to pass a human (research technician) at a passage <strong>of</strong> 10 metres<br />

long <strong>and</strong> about one meter wide. The human was placed halfway. If the relation between the<br />

time required to approach the person compared to the total time required to pass the 10 metres<br />

were more than 0.67 then the sow was categorised as fearful. Two Forced Human Approach<br />

tests (FHA-tests) were performed in the home arena <strong>of</strong> the sows (Andersen et al.,<br />

2003, Pedersen et al., 2003) three weeks after mating. The sows’ reaction was categorised<br />

into reaction 1 to 6 <strong>and</strong> 1 to 3 respectively with low score indicating fleeing behaviour.<br />

Only sows, which scored 1 or 2 in the first <strong>and</strong> 1 in the second test, were categorised as<br />

fearful.<br />

Involvements in aggressive interactions, skin lesions <strong>and</strong> lying behaviour during resting<br />

periods were used as indicators <strong>of</strong> stress. The total number <strong>of</strong> aggressive interactions the<br />

individual sow participated in together with the number <strong>of</strong> lost interactions the first hour<br />

after weaning was recorded. The total number <strong>of</strong> skin lesions on head, ears, neck <strong>and</strong><br />

shoulders were recorded for each sow at mating <strong>and</strong> three weeks after. Every five minute<br />

during resting period at weaning <strong>and</strong> three weeks after mating for 25 minutes, it was registered<br />

whether the sow was lying solitary (defined as lying/sitting in a distance <strong>of</strong> 20 cm or<br />

more from other sows) or socially. If the sow was lying alone more frequently than socially,<br />

the sow was categorised as lying solitary.<br />

2.4 Statistical analysis<br />

When analysing the effect <strong>of</strong> the indicators on the interval from weaning to first mating,<br />

pregnancy chance, culling risk <strong>and</strong> litter size, the following model was applied:<br />

E(Yijk)= µ+αI(ijk)+γI(ijk) W +γI(ijk) AM +δI(ijk) W +δI(ijk) AM + Ai + Bi(j)+<br />

91<br />

7<br />

∑<br />

u=1<br />

βu xuijk<br />

Where Yijk is the observation for herd i, batch j <strong>and</strong> sow k. αI(ijk) is the effect <strong>of</strong> parity <strong>group</strong><br />

I(ijk) (1, 2-3 <strong>and</strong> >3 parity). I(ijk) is an indicator function given the parity <strong>of</strong> observation<br />

ijk. The indicator functions are specific to each effect, however, to keep Eq. (1) as simple as<br />

possible this I() notation is used throughout. γI(ijk) W <strong>and</strong> γI(ijk) AM is the effect <strong>of</strong> fear category<br />

(j,k =fearful, confident) at weaning <strong>and</strong> three weeks after mating respectively, δI(ijk) W <strong>and</strong><br />

δI(ijk) AM the effect <strong>of</strong> lying behaviour (l,m=lying solitary, lying socially) at weaning <strong>and</strong><br />

(1)


- Paper IV -<br />

three weeks after mating respectively. Ai <strong>and</strong> Bi(j) are the normal distributed r<strong>and</strong>om effects<br />

<strong>of</strong> herd <strong>and</strong> batch nested within herd.<br />

All covariates are represented in the last expression in Eq. (1). x1-7ijk is 1. the lactation<br />

length transformed by the natural logarithm, 2. the back fat at weaning, 3. the back fat gain<br />

(mm/week), 4. <strong>and</strong> 5. the number <strong>of</strong> skin lesions at mating <strong>and</strong> three weeks after mating<br />

transformed by the natural logarithm, 6. the number <strong>of</strong> aggressions <strong>and</strong> 7. % not eating<br />

transformed by square root respectively. β1-7 are regression parameters.<br />

The interactions between parity <strong>group</strong> <strong>and</strong> the indicators were also included in the model<br />

one by one but for the sake <strong>of</strong> simplicity these are not included in Eq. (1).<br />

For pregnancy chance after first mating <strong>and</strong> culling risk, E(Yijk) corresponds to logit to the<br />

probability <strong>of</strong> the observed outcome, pijk. For the interval from weaning to first mating <strong>and</strong><br />

litter size, Yijk ~ N(E(Yijk), σijk 2 ) whereas for pregnancy chance <strong>and</strong> culling risk, Yijk ~ B(1,<br />

pijk).<br />

Some exceptions were made to the general modelling strategy. The model for effect on litter<br />

size <strong>and</strong> pregnancy chance included the additional covariate, weaning to first mating<br />

interval. Similarly, when analysing interval from weaning to first mating, sows mated later<br />

than eight days after weaning (26 sows) were excluded from the analysis, <strong>and</strong> only covariates<br />

measured at weaning <strong>and</strong> mating were included in the model. In an experimental study,<br />

sows with back fat less than 10 mm were more predisposed to culling compared to fatter<br />

sows (Young et al. 1990). Therefore, when analysing risk <strong>of</strong> culling the analysis was repeated<br />

with the continuous covariate back fat at weaning categorised into a discrete variable<br />

with two levels: back fat category (


- Paper IV -<br />

mixed model using the glmmPQL function in the MASS package (Venables & Ripley,<br />

2002) <strong>of</strong> R (R Development Core Team, 2004).<br />

3. Results<br />

For the reproduction parameters, the overall averages <strong>and</strong> the variation between sows<br />

across all 14 herds are presented in Table 1. In average, the sows involved in this study<br />

showed a very high litter size <strong>of</strong> nearly 15 born piglets per litter. The percent <strong>of</strong> re-mated<br />

sows varied from 0 to 45% <strong>and</strong> the number <strong>of</strong> born piglets per litter varied from 13.6 to<br />

15.7 between the herds.<br />

Table 1. Overall level <strong>and</strong> variation in different production parameters for all focal sows<br />

N Mean Std Min Max<br />

Interval from weaning to first mating, days 541 5.3 - 2 40<br />

Re-mated sows, % 541 11 - - -<br />

Culled in all, % <strong>of</strong> weaned 554 4.4 1) - - -<br />

Total number <strong>of</strong> born piglets/litter 2) 437 14.8 3.3 4.0 24.0<br />

Number <strong>of</strong> dead born/litter 437 1.5 - 0 12<br />

1) Any sows culled after re-mating are not included 2) Alive <strong>and</strong> dead born<br />

Parameter estimates <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard errors from the analysis <strong>of</strong> weaning to first mating interval,<br />

pregnancy chance <strong>and</strong> litter size are shown in Table 2. There were no effects <strong>of</strong> % not<br />

eating, aggressions after mixing, fear or lying behavior for any <strong>of</strong> the dependent variables.<br />

Weaning to first mating interval. The total number <strong>of</strong> skin lesions at mating tended to correlate<br />

positive with weaning to first mating interval (P=0.07) <strong>and</strong> a significant two-factor interaction<br />

between back fat at weaning <strong>and</strong> parity was found, in that the interval decreased<br />

with increased back fat at weaning but only for first parity sows (Figure 1). Further, a significant<br />

negative correlation with lactation length was found. No main effects or interactions<br />

with parity for aggressions, fear category or lying behaviour were found.<br />

93


Days from weaning to first mating<br />

5,2<br />

5<br />

4,8<br />

4,6<br />

4,4<br />

4,2<br />

4<br />

3,8<br />

3,6<br />

3,4<br />

parity 1<br />

parity 2-3<br />

parity >3<br />

10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30<br />

Back fat at weaning, mm<br />

Figure 1. The modelled relation between back fat at<br />

weaning <strong>and</strong> days from weaning to first mating for<br />

each parity <strong>group</strong> (P


Table 2. Estimates <strong>and</strong> st<strong>and</strong>ard error for all independent variables when analysing the effect on interval from weaning to first mating, pregnancy chance<br />

<strong>and</strong> litter size<br />

N Range 1) Weaning to first mating interval Pregnancy chance Litter size<br />

Estimate Std.err. P-value Estimate Std.err. P-value Estimate Std.err. P-value<br />

Intercept 5.43 0.45 10.24 3.29 6.96 3.28<br />

Parity 554 0.0002 0.006<br />

1 0.66 3) 0.23 0 . -0.73 0.42<br />

2-3 -0.02 3) 0.18 0.98 0.32 0.50 0.36<br />

>3 0 3) . 1.15 0.39 0 .<br />

Lactation length, ln(days) 554 -0.30 0.13 0.02 -1.21 0.72 0.24 2.30 0.74 0.003<br />

Weaning to first mating int., days 515 0-8 -1.11 0.31 0.0004 -0.22 0.26 0.39<br />

Back fat at weaning, mm 551 6-35 0.01 3) Indicators<br />

0.01 0.03 0.06 0.03 0.43 0.07 0.04 0.11<br />

Back fat gain, mm/week 481 -1.3-1.9 0.67 0.29 0.02 0.59 0.33 0.08<br />

Not eating AM 2) , square root(%) 299 0-100 - - NS - - NS<br />

Skin lesions M 2) , ln(no.) 542 0-171 0.04 0.02 0.07 - - NS - - NS<br />

Skin lesions AM 2) , ln(no.) 543 0-208 - - NS - - NS<br />

Aggressions, square root(no.) 552 0-66 - - NS - - NS - - NS<br />

Fear (HA-test) W 2) 513 - - NS - - NS - - NS<br />

Fear (FHA-test) AM 2) 529 - - NS - - NS<br />

Lying behaviour W 2) 253 - - NS - - NS - - NS<br />

Lying behaviour AM 2) 1)<br />

Untransformed values<br />

386 - - NS - - NS<br />

2)<br />

W: at weaning, M: At mating, AM: three weeks after mating<br />

3)<br />

Parity <strong>group</strong> interacted with back fat at weaning, estimate for interaction for parity <strong>group</strong> one, two <strong>and</strong> three respectively: -0,041 (0,016), -0,0019 (0,011), 0


- Paper IV -<br />

4. Discussion<br />

The average litter size was high compared to earlier studies, whereas the percentage <strong>of</strong><br />

sows re-mated corresponded to several previous on-farm studies (Gjein & Larssen, 1995;<br />

Olsson & Svendsen, 1997; Hansen & Kongsted, 2002). The high litter sizes <strong>of</strong> the sows<br />

involved in this study, support that <strong>group</strong> housing <strong>of</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows does not necessarily<br />

lead to poor reproduction performance.<br />

The chance <strong>of</strong> pregnancy (P=0.02) <strong>and</strong> number <strong>of</strong> born piglets per litter (P=0.08) decreased<br />

with decreased back fat gain from weaning to three weeks after mating. Furthermore, sows<br />

eating less than 20% <strong>of</strong> the observations at feeding were in markedly higher risk <strong>of</strong> returning<br />

to oestrus compared to sows eating more frequently (P


- Paper IV -<br />

cial effect on high ranked sows (equal to the sows receiving fewest aggressions) more than<br />

a suppressing effect in sows <strong>of</strong> low rank in respect to weaning to oestrus interval.<br />

A negative effect <strong>of</strong> a poor body condition at weaning on weaning to oestrus interval is well<br />

documented in experimental studies (reviews by Dourmad et al., 1994; Whittemore, 1996).<br />

In this study, however, this effect was seen only in first parity sows. This indicates that first<br />

parity sows are more sensitive to a low back fat at weaning compared to older sows as also<br />

proposed by Whittemore & Morgan (1990). These results show that if it is difficult to avoid<br />

poor body condition at weaning, special attention should be given in the post weaning period<br />

to young sows. The negative effect <strong>of</strong> a low back fat depth at weaning might be caused<br />

by reduced LH level (Aherne & Kirkwood, 1985). Since boar stimuli after weaning may<br />

stimulate LH secretion in sows with metabolic constraints (Langendijk, 2001), careful boar<br />

stimulation could perhaps counterbalance the negative effect <strong>of</strong> poor body condition at<br />

weaning.<br />

In agreement with a previously experimental study (Young et al., 1990), a relation between<br />

back fat depth <strong>and</strong> culling was found, in that sows with a very low back fat depth at weaning<br />

(less than 10 mm) were more likely to be culled later in the <strong>non</strong>-lactating period compared<br />

to sows with 10 mm or more back fat (P


- Paper IV -<br />

hour after mixing (Kongsted et al., 2004). However, sows involved in many aggressions at<br />

weaning had the fewest skin lesions three weeks after mating (Kongsted et al., 2004). As a<br />

consequence, any stress experienced by these old, <strong>and</strong> probably high ranked sows (Arey &<br />

Edwards, 1998) might have been too short lasting to influence their reproduction. The<br />

number <strong>of</strong> skin lesions has previously shown to correlate with incidences <strong>of</strong> aggressive acts<br />

(Barnett et al., 1992). However, not only physical contact but also treats <strong>and</strong> visual contact<br />

may cause stress in the submissive sows (Ruis et al., 2002). Skin lesions per se may there-<br />

fore be inadequate to determine levels <strong>of</strong> stress. Finally, the lack <strong>of</strong> relation between repro-<br />

duction <strong>and</strong> fear could partly be due to the timing <strong>of</strong> the fear tests. Fearfulness towards hu-<br />

mans around oestrus might decrease the chance <strong>of</strong> a correct timing <strong>of</strong> ovulation <strong>and</strong> in-<br />

semination (Kongsted, 2004a). However, the fear tests were not performed on the observation<br />

day around mating because the oestrus behaviour <strong>of</strong> the sows made the test results unreliable<br />

(Kongsted et al., 2004). Therefore, the fear tests were only carried out at weaning<br />

<strong>and</strong> three weeks after mating, <strong>and</strong> it cannot be excluded that the fearfulness <strong>of</strong> the sows at<br />

that time were different compared to around mating.<br />

In conclusion, with the heterogeneity <strong>of</strong> the herds involved <strong>and</strong> the <strong>non</strong>-st<strong>and</strong>ardisation <strong>of</strong><br />

possible influential factors related to layout <strong>and</strong> management in mente, the found relations<br />

are noteworthy. Taken together, the results indicate that the individual variation in feed<br />

intake in sows <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> in commercial herds may be large enough to impair pregnancy<br />

rate <strong>and</strong> perhaps also litter size. However, it cannot be excluded that the found associations<br />

between back fat <strong>and</strong> reproduction are linked to social stress. The relations between indicators<br />

<strong>of</strong> feed intake <strong>and</strong> reproduction performance may not only be a consequence <strong>of</strong> feed<br />

intake per se but a combination <strong>of</strong> low feed intake <strong>and</strong> a high level <strong>of</strong> social stress in the<br />

low ranked sows. Additional studies are needed to illuminate the role <strong>of</strong> social stress in<br />

commercial sow herds further. Special attention should be aimed at developing additional<br />

indicators <strong>of</strong> social stress suitable for use in practical pig production.<br />

Acknowledgements<br />

The author wishes to thank the participating farmers <strong>and</strong> the technicians Henrik K. Andersen,<br />

Kristine R. Hansen, Michael Hansen, Orla Nielsen, Niels H. Thomsen, <strong>and</strong> Helge Yde<br />

for their much appreciated assistance in carrying out the data collection. The author also<br />

wishes to express her gratitude to John E. Hermansen <strong>and</strong> Troels Kristensen for valuable<br />

comments on this paper <strong>and</strong> to Erik Jørgensen for valuable contributions to the statistical<br />

analyses.<br />

98


References<br />

- Paper IV -<br />

Aherne, F.X., Kirkwood, R.N., 1985. Nutrition <strong>and</strong> sow prolificacy. J. Reprod. Fert. , Suppl. 33, 169-183.<br />

Andersen, I.L., Bøe, K., Kristiansen, A.L., 1999. The influence <strong>of</strong> different feeding arrangements <strong>and</strong> food<br />

type on competition at feeding in pregnant sows. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 65, 91-104.<br />

Andersen, I.L., Berg, S., Bøe, K.E. & Edwards, S. 2003. Effect <strong>of</strong> short term h<strong>and</strong>ling on fear <strong>of</strong> humans <strong>and</strong><br />

the consequences for maternal abilities <strong>of</strong> sows. Proceedings <strong>of</strong> the 37th International Congress <strong>of</strong><br />

the ISAE, Abano Terme, Italy, June 24-28, 2003. Abstract, Pp. 79<br />

Anderson, L.L., 1975. Embryonic <strong>and</strong> placental development during prolonged inanition in the pig. Am. J.<br />

Physiol. 229, 1687-1694.<br />

Arey, D. & Edwards, S.A. 1998. Factors influencing aggression between sows after mixing <strong>and</strong> the concequences<br />

for welfare <strong>and</strong> production. Livest. Prod. Sci. 56, 61-70.<br />

Barnett, J.L., Hemsworth, P.H., Cronin, G.M., Newman, E.A., McCallum, T.H. & Chilton, D. 1992. Effects<br />

<strong>of</strong> pen size, partial stalls <strong>and</strong> method <strong>of</strong> feeding on welfare-related behavioural <strong>and</strong> physiological responses<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>group</strong>-<strong>housed</strong> pigs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci34, 207-220.<br />

Bates, R.O., Edwards, D.B., Korthals, R.L., 2003. Sow performance when <strong>housed</strong> either in <strong>group</strong>s with electronic<br />

sow feeder or stalls. Livest. Prod. Sci. 79, 29-35.<br />

Baustad, B., Lium, B., 2002. Helse og dyrevelferd i norsk svineproduksjon sett i et internasjonalt perspektiv.<br />

Norsk Vet.tidsskrift 114, 87-91.<br />

Bonde, M., Rousing, T., Sørensen, J.T. 2003. Human approach test for on-farm use in loose-<strong>housed</strong> pregnant<br />

sows: relation between behaviour <strong>and</strong> health. Proceedings <strong>of</strong> the 37th International Congress <strong>of</strong> the<br />

ISAE, Abano Terme, Italy, June 24-28, 2003.<br />

Brouns, F., Edwards, S.A., 1994. Social rank <strong>and</strong> feeding behaviour <strong>of</strong> <strong>group</strong>-<strong>housed</strong> sows fed competitively<br />

or ad libitum. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 39, 225-235.<br />

Csermely, D. & Wood-Gush, D.G.M. 1990. Agonistic behaviour in <strong>group</strong>ed sows. II. How social rank affects<br />

feeding <strong>and</strong> drinking behaviour. Boll. Zool. 57, 55-58.<br />

Dourmad, J.Y., Étienne, M., Prunier, A., Noblet, J., 1994. The effect <strong>of</strong> energy <strong>and</strong> protein intake <strong>of</strong> sows on<br />

their longevity: a review. Livest. Prod. Sci. 40, 87-97.<br />

Dyck, G.W., Strain, J.H., 1983. Postmating feeding level effects on conception rate <strong>and</strong> embryo survival in<br />

gilts. Cana. J. Anim. Sci. 63, 579.<br />

Engl<strong>and</strong>, D.C., Spurr, D.T., 1969. Litter size <strong>of</strong> swine confined during gestation. J. Anim. Sci. 28, 220-223.<br />

Fisker, B.N., 1995. Indsættelsesstrategi for gruppefodrede drægtige søer. Meddelelse 311, L<strong>and</strong>sudvalget for<br />

Svin, Den rullende Afprøvning, 7pp.<br />

Gjein, H., Larssen, R.B., 1995. Housing <strong>of</strong> pregnant sows in loose <strong>and</strong> confined systems - a field study. 1.<br />

Vulva <strong>and</strong> body lesions, culling reasons <strong>and</strong> production results. Acta vet. Sc<strong>and</strong>. 36, 185-200.<br />

99


- Paper IV -<br />

Hansen, L.U. 2000. Løbeafdeling med enkeltdyrsstier eller flokopstaldning. Meddelelse 6, L<strong>and</strong>sudvalget for<br />

Svin, Den rullende Afprøvning, 6pp.<br />

Hansen, L.U. 2003. Løbeafdeling med enkeltdyrsstier eller flokopstaldning med permanent adgang til æde-<br />

/insemineringsbokse. Meddelelse 602, L<strong>and</strong>sudvalget for Svin, Danske Slagterier, 6pp.<br />

Hansen, L.U. & Kongsted, A.G. 2002. Gulvudformning i løbeafdeling med æde-/insemineringsbokse til løs-<br />

gående søer. Meddelelse 559, Den rullende Afprøvning, L<strong>and</strong>sudvalget for Svin, Danske Slagterier,<br />

8pp.<br />

Hennessy, D.P. & Williamson, P. 1983. The effects <strong>of</strong> stress <strong>and</strong> <strong>of</strong> ACTH administration in hormone pr<strong>of</strong>iles,<br />

oestrus <strong>and</strong> ovulation in pigs. Theriogenology 20, 13-26.<br />

Hurtgen, J.P., Leman, A.D. & Crabo, B. 1980. Effect <strong>of</strong> season, parity <strong>and</strong> housing factors on estrus <strong>and</strong><br />

fertility in swine. 20. 1980. Proc. Int. Pig. Vet. Soc., Copenhagen, Denmark.<br />

Jensen, K.H., Pedersen, L.J., Hagelsø, A.M.G., Heller, K.N., Jørgensen, E. & Ladewig, J. 1995a. Intermittent<br />

stress in pigs: Behavioural <strong>and</strong> pituitary-adrenocortical reactivity. Acta Agric. Sc<strong>and</strong>. , Sect. A,<br />

Anim. Sci 45, 276-285.<br />

Jensen, K.H., Pedersen, B.K., Pedersen, L.J. & Jørgensen, E. 1995b. Well-being in pregnant sows: Confinement<br />

versus <strong>group</strong> housing with electronic sow feeding. Acta Agric. Sc<strong>and</strong>. , Sect. A, Anim. Sci. 45,<br />

266-275.<br />

Kongsted, A.G. 2004a. Stress <strong>and</strong> fear as possible mediators <strong>of</strong> reproduction problems in <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> sows:<br />

A review. Accepted for publication in Acta. Agric. Sc<strong>and</strong>., Sect. A, Anim. Sci.<br />

Kongsted, A.G. 2004b. Effect <strong>of</strong> energy intake on pregnancy rate <strong>and</strong> litter size with particular reference to<br />

<strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows – a review. Submitted to Livest. Prod. Sci. In revision.<br />

Kongsted, A.G., Hermansen, J.E., Kristensen, T. 2004. Indicators <strong>of</strong> feed intake, fear <strong>and</strong> social stress in<br />

commercial herds with <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows. Submitted for publication in Acta Agric.<br />

Sc<strong>and</strong>., Sect. A, Anim. Sci.<br />

Langendijk, P. 2001. Is there af future for the boar? The role <strong>of</strong> boar stimuli in reproductive processes around<br />

estrus in the pig. Ph.d. Department <strong>of</strong> Animal Sciences, Wageningen Institute <strong>of</strong> Animal Sciences,<br />

Wageningen University, The Netherl<strong>and</strong>s.<br />

Littell, R. C., G. A. Milliken, W. W. Stroup, <strong>and</strong> R. D. Wolfinger. 1996. SAS ® System for mixed models.<br />

Cary, NC: SAS Institute Inc..<br />

Mendl, M., Zanella, A.J., Broom, D.M., 1992. Physiological <strong>and</strong> reproductive correlates <strong>of</strong> behavioural strategies<br />

in female domestic pigs. Anim. Behav. 44, 1107-1121.<br />

Mormede, P. 1990. Neuroendocrine responses to social stress. In: Zayan, R. & Dantzer, R. (Eds): Social stress<br />

in domestic animals. Dordrecht, Klüwer Academic Publisher. P. 203-211.<br />

Nielsen, N.-P., Hansen, L.U., Calmann-Hinke, D., 2000. Stalde til løsgående søer. Rapport 17, L<strong>and</strong>sudvalget<br />

for Svin, Danske Slagterier, 33pp.<br />

100


- Paper IV -<br />

Olsson, A.-C., Svendsen, J., 1997. Effekter av olika konkurrensförhåll<strong>and</strong>en vid utfodring på suggors hälsa<br />

och produktion. SLU Rapport 113, Sveriges Lantbruksuniversitet, 37pp.<br />

Pedersen, L.J., Rojkittikhun, T., Einarsson, S., Edqvist, L.-E., 1993. Postweaning <strong>group</strong>ed sows: effects <strong>of</strong><br />

aggression on hormonal patterns <strong>and</strong> oestrous behaviour. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 38, 25-39.<br />

Pedersen, L.J., Jensen, K.H., Jørgensen, E., 1996. Pre- <strong>and</strong> postpubertal LH <strong>and</strong> estradiol pattern in gilts sub-<br />

jected to intermittent inescapable electroshock. Acta vet. Sc<strong>and</strong>. 37, 153-161.<br />

Pedersen, L.J., Damm, B.I., Kongsted, A.G., 2003. The influence <strong>of</strong> adverse or gentle h<strong>and</strong>ling procedures on<br />

sexual behaviour in fearful <strong>and</strong> confident sows. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 83, 277-290.<br />

Pedersen, P.N., Thorup, F., 1995. Analyse af besætningens reproduktionsresultater i bedriftsløsningens rap-<br />

portgenerator. Erfaring 9524, L<strong>and</strong>sudvalget for Svin, Danske Slagterier, 17pp.<br />

Peltoniemi, O.A.T., Love, R.J., Heionen, M., Tuovinen, V., Saloiemi, H., 1999. Seasonal <strong>and</strong> management<br />

effects on fertility <strong>of</strong> the sow: a descriptive study. Anim. Repro. Sci. 55, 47-61.<br />

R Development Core Team, 2004. R: A language <strong>and</strong> environment for statistical computing. R foundation for<br />

statistical computing. http://www.R-project.org<br />

Rousing, T., Bonde, M., Sørensen, J.T. 1999. Fear testing in loose housing systems for pregnant sows. Poster<br />

presentation. 1999. Proceedings <strong>of</strong> the 33rd International Congress <strong>of</strong> the international Society for<br />

Applied Ethology (ISAE), 17-21 august 1999, Lillehammer Norway.<br />

Ruis, M.A.W., te Brake, J.H.A., Engel, B., Buist, W.G., Blokhuis, H.J., Koolhaas, J.M., 2002. Implications <strong>of</strong><br />

coping characteristics <strong>and</strong> social status for welfare <strong>and</strong> production <strong>of</strong> paired growing gilts. Appl.<br />

Anim. Behav. Sci. 75, 207-231.<br />

SAS Institute Inc. 1990. SAS/STAT ® User's Guide. Gary, NC.<br />

Sørensen, G., Thorup, F., 2003. Energitildeling i implantationsperioden. Meddelelse 618, L<strong>and</strong>sudvalget for<br />

Svin, Danske Slagterier, 7pp.<br />

Trezona, M. 2003. Welfare update: Dry sow stalls. Department <strong>of</strong> Agriculture - Western Australia.<br />

http://www.agric.wa.gov.au/progserv/animal/cntnorth/porkserv/pigtales/2000/Oct2000/article08.htm<br />

Venables, W. N. <strong>and</strong> B. D. Ripley. 2002. Modern Applied statistics with S. Fourth Edition. Springer. 495pp.<br />

Whittemore, C.T., 1996. Nutrition reproduction interaction in primiparous sows. Livest. Prod. Sci. 46, 65-83.<br />

Whittemore, C.T., Morgan, C.A., 1990. Model components for the determination <strong>of</strong> energy <strong>and</strong> protein requirements<br />

for breeding sows: a review. Livest. Prod. Sci. 26, 1-37.<br />

Young, L.G., King, G.J., Walton, S., McMillan, I., Klevorick, M., Shaw, J., 1990. Gestation energy <strong>and</strong> reproduction<br />

in sows over four parities. Can. J. Anim. Sci. 70, 493-506.<br />

101


102


- General discussion -<br />

GENERAL DISCUSSION<br />

The specific aims <strong>of</strong> this thesis were 1) to identify important causes for impaired reproduction<br />

performance in <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> sows <strong>and</strong> 2) to define <strong>and</strong> evaluate indicators suitable for<br />

use in decision-making to improve the reproduction performance <strong>of</strong> commercial herds with<br />

<strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> sows.<br />

Based upon a review study, the hypotheses put forward were: 1) individual variation in energy<br />

intake as well as fear <strong>and</strong> social stress might be important causes for impaired reproduction<br />

performance in <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows, <strong>and</strong> 2) back fat, skin lesions <strong>and</strong><br />

behavioural measurements might be indicators <strong>of</strong> these characteristics, suitable to express<br />

variation in sows’ reproduction performance under practical <strong>conditions</strong>.<br />

The results <strong>of</strong> the detailed farm study, including 14 commercial herds, supported that <strong>group</strong><br />

housing may lead to individual variation in feed intake severe enough to impair pregnancy<br />

rate <strong>and</strong> perhaps also litter size (Paper IV). The 14 herds were chosen to represent different<br />

layouts <strong>and</strong> management routines to ensure that any relation found could be transferred to a<br />

broad spectrum <strong>of</strong> herds. For the 554 sows followed, a significantly (P=0.02), positive correlation<br />

between back fat gain <strong>and</strong> chance <strong>of</strong> pregnancy was found <strong>and</strong> further a tendency<br />

(P=0.08) to a positive correlation between back fat gain <strong>and</strong> litter size. The modelled relations<br />

indicate that a sow with a negative back fat gain <strong>of</strong> 0.5 mm per week has 5 percentage<br />

points lower chance <strong>of</strong> pregnancy after first mating <strong>and</strong> 0.6 fewer born piglets per litter<br />

compared to a sow with a positive back fat gain <strong>of</strong> 0.5 mm per week. In addition, it was<br />

found that sows eating less than 20% <strong>of</strong> all observations three weeks after mating had a<br />

significantly (P


- General discussion -<br />

No convincing associations were found between the indicators <strong>of</strong> stress <strong>and</strong> fear <strong>and</strong> the<br />

risk <strong>of</strong> returning to oestrus or litter size. As some <strong>of</strong> these indicators, though not pro-<br />

nounced, was significantly related to back fat gain, e.g. number <strong>of</strong> skin lesions (Paper III),<br />

the reason could be that including back fat in the statistical model left little variation to be<br />

explained by the other indicators. However, even after exclusion <strong>of</strong> back fat gain, no sig-<br />

nificant effects were found on any <strong>of</strong> the indicators <strong>of</strong> stress. This does not necessarily<br />

demonstrate that no relations between reproduction <strong>and</strong> these characteristics exist. As dis-<br />

cussed in Paper IV, it cannot be excluded that the indicators applied have been insufficient<br />

to express variation in levels <strong>of</strong> social stress. Special attention should be paid to developing<br />

additional indicators <strong>of</strong> social stress suitable for use in commercial sow herds. In this study,<br />

all the indicators <strong>of</strong> social stress were related to aggressive interactions (e.g. direct behav-<br />

ioural observations <strong>of</strong> aggressions <strong>and</strong> the number <strong>of</strong> skin lesions). However, not only<br />

physical contact but also treats <strong>and</strong> visual contact may cause stress in the low ranking sows<br />

(Ruis et al., 2002), <strong>and</strong> sows receiving many aggressions are not necessarily identical with<br />

the most submissive sows, as shown <strong>and</strong> discussed in paper III. Therefore, in any complementary<br />

future study, efforts should be made to estimate the rank (high, medium, low) <strong>of</strong><br />

the individual, focal sow. As already discussed, in <strong>group</strong> feeding systems, sows with limited<br />

access to the feed are probably identical to the most submissive sows. However, in systems<br />

with individual feeding, access to feed cannot necessarily be used as an indicator <strong>of</strong><br />

rank. In these systems it might be possible to estimate rank by provoking a competition<br />

situation e.g. by providing a small amount <strong>of</strong> feed in the corner <strong>of</strong> the pen.<br />

As presented in Paper II, human approach tests were used as indicators <strong>of</strong> fear, because it<br />

was hypothesized that submissive sows would react fearfully towards humans due to their<br />

negative experience during <strong>group</strong>ing. However, this has never been verified <strong>and</strong> some <strong>of</strong><br />

the research technicians actually had the impression that the most submissive sows were the<br />

ones that reacted most confidently towards humans. This indicates that human approach<br />

tests cannot be used for assessing the rank <strong>of</strong> the sows. This is further supported by the lack<br />

<strong>of</strong> associations between parity <strong>group</strong> <strong>and</strong> the outcome <strong>of</strong> the two fear tests performed after<br />

<strong>group</strong>ing <strong>of</strong> the sows (paper III).<br />

The results from the farm study indicated that back fat measurements <strong>and</strong> observations <strong>of</strong><br />

sows’ eating behaviour might be relevant indicators <strong>of</strong> reproduction problems since 1) they<br />

were possible to perform in sows <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> under practical <strong>conditions</strong> <strong>and</strong> 2) a relation<br />

was found between reproduction performance <strong>and</strong> these characteristics. Consequently,<br />

these indicators might be relevant ingredients <strong>of</strong> a management tool to analyse <strong>and</strong> improve<br />

the reproduction performance <strong>of</strong> <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows.<br />

104


- General discussion -<br />

However, an indicator is not necessarily well suited in an operational context just because<br />

the indicator is possible to perform in commercial herds <strong>and</strong> well related to the phenomena<br />

under study. Indicators applicable in a management tool should further be meaningful for<br />

the individual farmer (Halberg, 1996). It is e.g. important that the individual manager be-<br />

lieves that an indicator provides useful <strong>and</strong> relevant information, because otherwise he/she<br />

will not be motivated to use this as a management tool. Furthermore, it is essential that an<br />

indicator is possible to measure or register by farmers or advisors within reasonable time<br />

<strong>and</strong> costs (Halberg, 1996; Rousing et al., 2001; Sørensen et al., 2001). Whether the two<br />

indicators mentioned above meet these requirements will be discussed in the following.<br />

A systematic investigation <strong>of</strong> whether the indicators applied were perceived as meaningful<br />

by the farmers, e.g. by means <strong>of</strong> qualitative interviews (Vaarst, 2003) has not been carried<br />

out. However, <strong>non</strong>e <strong>of</strong> the farmers or managers questioned the relevance <strong>and</strong> usability <strong>of</strong><br />

the back fat measurements or the observations <strong>of</strong> eating behaviour at the introductive farm<br />

visits or during the recordings.<br />

Observation <strong>of</strong> eating behaviour. Time has become a very important issue in pig management,<br />

as the average labour input per animal has decreased (Madsen, 2001; Rasmussen,<br />

2003). The time necessary to observe sows’ eating behaviour was 10 to 25 minutes per<br />

batch as one <strong>group</strong> <strong>of</strong> sows were observed per batch <strong>and</strong> the recordings were carried out<br />

until the last sow had stopped eating or at longest 25 minutes after feeding start. Following<br />

four batches would then acquire between 40 m <strong>and</strong> 1.7 h plus the time required to select<br />

<strong>and</strong> mark ten r<strong>and</strong>om, focal sows. This seems as a feasible time range in most herds. However,<br />

the disadvantage <strong>of</strong> manually recording the eating behaviour is that a maximum <strong>of</strong> ten<br />

focal sows can be overviewed at a time. As a consequence here<strong>of</strong>, there is a risk that an<br />

existing problem will not be detected. This problem could, however, be overcome if the<br />

identification <strong>of</strong> sows with a low feed intake could be automated. O'Connell et al. (2003)<br />

report that low ranking sows visit the drinker more <strong>of</strong>ten than the high ranking in a <strong>group</strong> <strong>of</strong><br />

sows <strong>and</strong> according to own observations it seemed that sows with a very low feed intake<br />

<strong>of</strong>ten visited the drinker during feeding. Online registration <strong>of</strong> pigs’ drinking pattern as an<br />

indicator <strong>of</strong> e.g. an outbreak <strong>of</strong> diarrhoea has already been implemented as a commercial<br />

s<strong>of</strong>tware package, Farm Watch® (Madsen, 2001). Likewise, it might be possible to detect a<br />

feed-related problem in submissive sows by automatical registration <strong>of</strong> the water consumption<br />

during feeding. In that case, it would probably be possible to identify <strong>and</strong> pay special<br />

attention to ‘problem sows’ before the reproduction performance was influenced negatively.<br />

Back fat measurements. Assessment <strong>of</strong> back fat gain from weaning to three weeks after<br />

mating cannot be used for identifying individual sows with a potential reproduction prob-<br />

105


- General discussion -<br />

lem, because three weeks after mating their reproduction will already have been impaired.<br />

However, assessment <strong>of</strong> back fat gain can provide the farm manager with valuable indica-<br />

tions <strong>of</strong> whether variation in back fat gain, in general, could be a contributing reason for<br />

impaired reproduction in <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> sows. The time necessary for back fat measurements<br />

was not recorded in this farm study. However, the back fat measurements were estimated<br />

to be possible to perform within two or three minutes per sow including finding the<br />

measuring points, shaving <strong>and</strong> measuring. A recent Danish test trial showed that back fat<br />

measurements with a similar digital ultrasound back fat indicator, in average requested approximately<br />

one minute per sow if the measuring points were marked <strong>and</strong> the sows were<br />

shaved in advance (Thorup, 2004 pers. comm.). This, in combination with a low price<br />

(DKK 4,800 ~ EUR 650) makes LEAN-MEATER® a potential management tool. However,<br />

the suitability <strong>of</strong> the equipment has recently been questioned due to a large withinobserver<br />

variation, which makes it inappropriate for use at individual sow-level (Thorup,<br />

pers. comm.). More accurate two-dimensional back fat indicators are available on the market.<br />

As some <strong>of</strong> these can be used for pregnancy diagnosis too, they will perhaps be potential<br />

management tools in spite <strong>of</strong> the considerable larger price.<br />

The 14 herds involved in the farm study differed markedly with respect to the reproduction<br />

performance <strong>of</strong> the approximately 40 sows observed per herd. Return percentage varied<br />

from 0 to 45% with an average <strong>of</strong> 11% <strong>and</strong> litter size differed from 13.6 to 15.7 with an<br />

average <strong>of</strong> 14.8 total born piglets per litter. These figures support the assumption that <strong>group</strong><br />

housing <strong>of</strong> <strong>non</strong>-lactating sows does not ‘automatically’ lead to poor reproduction performance<br />

<strong>and</strong> further that it will be possible to improve the reproduction performance in some<br />

commercial herds.<br />

Improving the reproduction <strong>performances</strong> is, however, not the only challenge that managers<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> sows are facing. Group housing was implemented with the aim to improve<br />

the animal welfare in commercial pig production. However, submissive sows eating in less<br />

than 20% <strong>of</strong> all observations during feeding because they are kept away from the feed,<br />

sows having less than 10 mm back fat at farrowing <strong>and</strong> sows with more than 150 skin lesions<br />

at the end <strong>of</strong> the gestation, as found in the present farm study are not equal to animal<br />

welfare. Therefore, efforts should be made to improve not only the reproduction performance<br />

but also the animal welfare in <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> sows.<br />

106


References<br />

- General discussion -<br />

Andersen, I.L., Bøe, K., Kristiansen, A.L., 1999. The influence <strong>of</strong> different feeding arrangements <strong>and</strong> food<br />

type on competition at feeding in pregnant sows. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 65, 91-104.<br />

Brouns, F., Edwards, S.A., 1994. Social rank <strong>and</strong> feeding behaviour <strong>of</strong> <strong>group</strong>-<strong>housed</strong> sows fed competitively<br />

or ad libitum. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 39, 225-235.<br />

Csermely, D. & Wood-Gush, D.G.M. 1990. Agonistic behaviour in <strong>group</strong>ed sows. II. How social rank affects<br />

feeding <strong>and</strong> drinking behaviour. Boll. Zool. 57, 55-58.<br />

Halberg, N. 1996. Miljø- og ressourceindikatorer til brug i et etisk regnskab for husdyrbrug. Ph.D. Statens<br />

Husdyrbrugsforsøg.<br />

Madsen, T. N. 2001. Tools for monitoring growing pigs. Ph.D. The royal veterinary <strong>and</strong> agricultural univer-<br />

sity, Copenhagen.<br />

Mendl, M., Zanella, A.J., Broom, D.M., 1992. Physiological <strong>and</strong> reproductive correlates <strong>of</strong> behavioural stra-<br />

tegies in female domestic pigs. Animal Behaviour 44, 1107-1121.<br />

O'Connell, N.E., Beattie, V.E., Moss, B.W., 2003. Influence <strong>of</strong> social status on the welfare <strong>of</strong> sows in static<br />

<strong>and</strong> dynamic <strong>group</strong>s. Animal Welfare 12, 239-249.<br />

Olsson, A.-C., Svendsen, J., 1997. Effekter av olika konkurrensförhåll<strong>and</strong>en vid utfodring på suggors hälsa<br />

och produktion. SLU Rapport 113, Sveriges Lantbruksuniversitet, 37pp.<br />

Rasmussen, J.D.L.K., 2003. Erfaringer med tidsforbrug i drægtighedsstalde. Erfaring 301, Den rullende Af-<br />

prøvning, L<strong>and</strong>sudvalget for Svin, Danske Slagterier, 11pp.<br />

Rousing, T., Bonde, M., Sørensen, J.T., 2001. Aggregating welfare indicators into an operational welfare<br />

assesment system: A bottom-up approach. Acta Agric. Sc<strong>and</strong>. , Sect. A, Animal Science Suppl. 30,<br />

53-57.<br />

Ruis, M.A.W., te Brake, J.H.A., Engel, B., Buist, W.G., Blokhuis, H.J., Koolhaas, J.M., 2002. Implications <strong>of</strong><br />

coping characteristics <strong>and</strong> social status for welfare <strong>and</strong> production <strong>of</strong> paired growing gilts. Appl.<br />

Anim. Behav. Sci. 75, 207-231.<br />

Sørensen, J.T., S<strong>and</strong>øe, P., Halberg, N., 2001. Animal welfare as one among several values to be considered<br />

at farm level: The idea <strong>of</strong> an ethical account for livestock farming. Acta Agric. Sc<strong>and</strong>. , Sect. A,<br />

Animal Science Suppl. 30, 11-16.<br />

Thorup, F. 2004. Ph.D. The National Committee for Pig Production, Danish Bacon <strong>and</strong> Meat Council. Per-<br />

sonal Communication. Axeltorv 3, 1609 Copenhagen V.<br />

Vaarst, M., 2003. Evaluating a concept for animal welfare assessment systems as decision support using<br />

qualitative interviews. Animal Welfare 12, 541-546.<br />

107


108


- Conclusions -<br />

6. CONCLUSIONS<br />

Two review papers revealed a good theoretical <strong>and</strong> experimental background for believing<br />

that individual variations in feed intake, social stress <strong>and</strong> fear could be contributing reasons<br />

for impaired reproduction seen in some <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> sows. The results <strong>of</strong> a detailed farm<br />

study including 14 commercial herds, supported the assumption that <strong>group</strong> housing in practice<br />

may lead to individual variation in feed intake severe enough to impair pregnancy rate<br />

<strong>and</strong> perhaps also litter size, as correlations between back fat gain <strong>and</strong> reproduction performance<br />

as well as between eating time <strong>and</strong> reproduction performance were found. As a<br />

consequence here<strong>of</strong>, back fat measurements <strong>and</strong> observations <strong>of</strong> the eating behaviour <strong>of</strong> the<br />

sows might be useful components in a decision-support tool to analyse <strong>and</strong> improve the<br />

reproduction performance in <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> sows. No convincing correlations were found<br />

between chance <strong>of</strong> pregnancy <strong>and</strong> litter size on one side <strong>and</strong> the indicators <strong>of</strong> social stress<br />

<strong>and</strong> fear on the other. Since stress <strong>and</strong> fear are difficult to measure, this does not necessarily<br />

demonstrate that no relations exist between reproduction <strong>and</strong> these characteristics. It does,<br />

however, indicate that the indicators applied are unsuitable to reflect the reproduction performance<br />

<strong>of</strong> <strong>group</strong> <strong>housed</strong> sows under practical <strong>conditions</strong>.<br />

109


110


- Appendix 1 -<br />

APPENDIX 1<br />

LITTER SIZE AND FARROWING RATE, WHICH PHYSIOLOGICAL PROCESSES<br />

MAY GO WRONG AND WHY?<br />

Successful reproduction, characterized by establishment <strong>and</strong> maintenance <strong>of</strong> pregnancy <strong>and</strong><br />

a high litter size, depend on a series <strong>of</strong> precisely timed physiological <strong>and</strong> endocrine events<br />

in the period from weaning to farrowing. Some <strong>of</strong> these events are illustrated in Figure 1<br />

<strong>and</strong> will be explained in the following text.<br />

Day 117<br />

Day 13-25<br />

Posterior pituitary gl<strong>and</strong> Anterior pituitary gl<strong>and</strong> Developing follicles Uterus Corpus Luteum<br />

Oxytocin<br />

Maintenance <strong>of</strong> pregnancy<br />

Implantation <strong>of</strong> ova in the<br />

uterus<br />

+ fertilisation<br />

Farrowing<br />

The mature ova pass<br />

down into the uterus<br />

Day 3.5<br />

FSH LH Oestrogen Prostagl<strong>and</strong>in Progesteron<br />

Weaning<br />

÷ fertilisation<br />

Regression <strong>of</strong><br />

corpus luteum<br />

The ruptured follicles<br />

form corpus luteum<br />

Release <strong>of</strong> the mature ova<br />

(=ovulation)<br />

The physiological events will be discussed in events happening 1) before <strong>and</strong> around mating/insemination<br />

<strong>and</strong> 2) after mating/insemination. After that the endocrine control <strong>of</strong> the<br />

physiological events will be presented. Finally, a short discussion <strong>of</strong> which parameters may<br />

influence these events will be carried out.<br />

111<br />

Development <strong>and</strong> maturation<br />

<strong>of</strong> ovarian follicles<br />

Day 1<br />

Mating/insemination<br />

Transport <strong>of</strong> semen<br />

Oestrus behaviour<br />

Figure 1. <strong>Reproduction</strong> related physiological <strong>and</strong> endocrine events in the period from weaning to<br />

farrowing (see text for further explanation)<br />

Day 0


- Appendix 1 -<br />

Physiological events before <strong>and</strong> around mating/insemination<br />

To gain pregnancy <strong>and</strong> a high litter size, onset <strong>of</strong> oestrus has to appear followed by ovulation,<br />

the number <strong>of</strong> ovulated eggs have to be high <strong>and</strong> further, the artificial insemination or<br />

mating has to bee successful resulting in fertilisation <strong>and</strong> a high number <strong>of</strong> fertilised eggs.<br />

At luteolysis (around day 16 in oestrus cycle when the luteal phase is replaced by the follicular<br />

phase) or at weaning, 15-25 follicles are recruited <strong>and</strong> selected to undergo preovulatory<br />

growth <strong>and</strong> to ovulate 4-7 days later (Prunier & Quesnel, 2000b). A few days after<br />

weaning the sow start to show oestrus. The interval from weaning to onset <strong>of</strong> estrus (WEI)<br />

<strong>and</strong> duration <strong>of</strong> estrus varies to a large degree between farms <strong>and</strong> also between sows within<br />

farms (see Table 1).<br />

Table 1. Examples <strong>of</strong> interval from weaning to onset <strong>of</strong> estrus (WEI) <strong>and</strong> duration <strong>of</strong> estrus (ED) reported<br />

in the literature<br />

WEI, hours after weaning ED, hours<br />

X, +/-SD Range X, +/-SD Range<br />

Pedersen & Navnt<strong>of</strong>t, 1996 112+/-11 - 49+/-7 -<br />

Pedersen & Navnt<strong>of</strong>t, 1996 100+/-17 - 61+/-10 -<br />

Nissen et al., 1997 92+/-13 64-134 60+/-14 30-89<br />

The interval from onset <strong>of</strong> estrus to ovulation is reported to be in the range <strong>of</strong> 10 to 85<br />

hours for individual sows, with mean values <strong>of</strong> 15 to 35 hours (review by Kemp & Soede,<br />

1997). The duration <strong>of</strong> estrus <strong>and</strong> the interval from onset <strong>of</strong> oestrus to ovulation is negative<br />

correlated to WEI (Nissen et al., 1997). The duration <strong>of</strong> ovulation ranges from one to three<br />

hours (Kemp & Soede, 1997). At ovulation the released ova are expelled into the oviduct<br />

<strong>and</strong> transported to the ampullary-isthmic junction, the site <strong>of</strong> fertilisation, perhaps in 30-45<br />

min or less (Hunter, 1990). At insemination or mating sperm cells are deposited at the<br />

utero-cervical junction. Sperm cells migrate through the uterine horns to the oviducts where<br />

they are temporarily stored in the sperm reservoir near the utero-tubal junction (Hunter,<br />

1990). This transport is believed to be dependent mainly upon uterine contractions (Scott,<br />

2000). Hunter (1981) found that within 1-2 h after mating there were enough spermatozoa<br />

in the oviduct to promote 100% fertilisation. Before the sperm are capable <strong>of</strong> fertilisation<br />

they have to undergo capacitation (maturation), which is presumed to happen in the sperm<br />

reservoir (Rodriguez-Martinez et al., 2001). Capacitation is believed to require five to six<br />

hours but can be accelerated to less than two hours if insemination is performed around<br />

ovulation (Hunter, 1990). By the time ovulation approaches, sperm are released from the<br />

reservoir to the oviduct (Hunter, 1981).<br />

The ova are only capable <strong>of</strong> being fertilised the first eight hours after ovulation (Hunter,<br />

1967) <strong>and</strong> the sperm needs to be present in the female tract for about two h or longer before<br />

112


- Appendix 1 -<br />

they are mature i.e. capable <strong>of</strong> fertilisation (see above). The fertilisation ability <strong>of</strong> the sperm<br />

in the female tract begins to decline after 12 hours (Dziuk, 1970) but can survive for more<br />

than 44 h (Kemp & Soede, 1997). Therefore a successful insemination or mating also de-<br />

pends upon clear oestrus behaviour <strong>of</strong> the sow <strong>and</strong> a co-ordination between oestrus behav-<br />

iour <strong>and</strong> ovulation, which as will be discussed later, depends on a good hormonal function.<br />

Physiological events after mating/insemination<br />

To gain pregnancy <strong>and</strong> a high litter size, the embryo survival also has to be high. A high<br />

embryo survival depends upon whether the following events are successful.<br />

Fertilised eggs are moving from the ampullary-isthmic junction (where fertilisation occurs)<br />

to the uterus. During this journey, which takes about 48 h, the eggs under go cleavage (cell<br />

division) <strong>and</strong> when they reach the uterus, they are at the four-cell stage (Dziuk, 1985;<br />

Ashworth, 1991). In the uterus the cell divisions continue (Hughes et al., 1996). On day six<br />

to day seven hatching from the zona pellucida (which is a complex glycoprotein matrix<br />

formed around each oocyte during follicular development (Dunbar & Bundman, 2003) occurs<br />

(Strob<strong>and</strong> & Lende, 1990). After hatching, the eggs become increasingly dependent<br />

upon nutrients in uterine secretions (Ashworth, 1991). Between day 7 <strong>and</strong> 12 <strong>of</strong> pregnancy<br />

the fertilised eggs redistribute themselves over the full length <strong>of</strong> both uterus horns (Dziuk,<br />

1985). Finally the elongation <strong>and</strong> the implantation (attachment to the uterine endometrium)<br />

take place. Elongation starts around day 10 <strong>of</strong> gestation (Ashworth, 1991; Geisert & Yelich,<br />

1997). During elongation the embryos undergo a morphological change from 10 mm<br />

spherical (i.e. round) to tubular (20-40 mm) <strong>and</strong> finally filamentous (up to 100 cm in<br />

length!) shapes by day 14-16 (Ashworth, 1991; van der Lende et al., 1994; Geisert & Yelich,<br />

1997). The implantation starts around day 13-14 <strong>of</strong> pregnancy <strong>and</strong> is completed about<br />

24 days after fertilisation (Crombie, 1970; Ashworth, 1991). Each embryo is surrounded by<br />

separate fluid filled membranes, the amnion <strong>and</strong> chorion, which form the placenta<br />

(Ashworth, 1991). From day 18 to day 30 <strong>of</strong> pregnancy the volume <strong>of</strong> this fluid increases<br />

markedly (Ashworth, 1991) <strong>and</strong> it is the presence or absence <strong>of</strong> this fluid that can be detected<br />

by the commonly used ultrasound pregnancy diagnosis instruments. On day 35 <strong>of</strong><br />

pregnancy the embryo is approximately four cm long (van der Lende, 1989) (see Figure 2).<br />

113


- Appendix 1 -<br />

Figure 2. A pig embryo approximately 30 days old (Maddox-Hyttel, 2003)<br />

The major part <strong>of</strong> prenatal mortality occurs in the first 35 days <strong>of</strong> pregnancy <strong>and</strong> perhaps<br />

especially before day 18 <strong>of</strong> pregnancy (Pope & First, 1985; van der Lende et al., 1994). The<br />

average embryo mortality is in the range <strong>of</strong> 20-30 % but with large variation between <strong>and</strong><br />

within populations (van der Lende et al., 1994). The last mentioned is illustrated in a study<br />

by van der Lende (1989) where the embryo mortality varied from 0 to 67 % in 71 gilts.<br />

Hormonal control <strong>of</strong> reproduction<br />

The above-mentioned physiological events are under sharp endocrine control <strong>of</strong> the hypothalamo-piturity-ovarian<br />

axis (Turner et al., 2002). Gonadotrophin-releasing hormone<br />

(GnRH) is released from the hypothalamus <strong>and</strong> transported to the anterior pituitary gl<strong>and</strong><br />

where it stimulates the release <strong>of</strong> luteinizing hormone (LH) <strong>and</strong> follicle-stimulating hormone<br />

(FSH). Luteinizing hormone <strong>and</strong> FSH act on the ovaries to stimulate the development<br />

<strong>of</strong> the pre-ovulatory follicle (Foxcr<strong>of</strong>t & Hunter, 1985). FSH is necessary to support follicular<br />

growth up to 5-6mm whereas LH is necessary for the final stages <strong>of</strong> follicle maturation<br />

(Britt et al., 1985). These developing follicles produce oestrogen, which is the primary<br />

trigger <strong>of</strong> oestrus behaviour (Hughes et al., 1996). The increased level <strong>of</strong> oestrogen influences<br />

the hypothalamus to increase the secretion <strong>of</strong> LH. The LH peak introduces changes in<br />

the follicle wall eventually leading to ovulation (Hughes et al., 1996).<br />

The ratio <strong>of</strong> oestrogen <strong>and</strong> progesterone <strong>and</strong> the level <strong>of</strong> prostagl<strong>and</strong>in influence the transport<br />

<strong>of</strong> the released ova to the site <strong>of</strong> fertilisation by causing contractions <strong>of</strong> the oviduct<br />

(Fr<strong>and</strong>son & Spurgeon, 1992). Oestrogen <strong>and</strong> prostagl<strong>and</strong>in increase uterine activity <strong>and</strong><br />

progesterone decreases uterine activity (Langendijk, 2001). Prostagl<strong>and</strong>in is also involved<br />

in the transport <strong>of</strong> semen from the uterus to the site <strong>of</strong> fertilisation (Soede, 1993). This<br />

transport is furthermore depending on small amounts <strong>of</strong> oxytocin, released from the posterior<br />

gl<strong>and</strong> at the onset <strong>of</strong> oestrus, causing strong contractions <strong>of</strong> the uterus (Hughes & Varley,<br />

1980). The oxytocin release is augmented by both external (the presence <strong>of</strong> a boar) <strong>and</strong><br />

internal stimulation (from seminal oestrogens) (Soede, 1993).<br />

114


- Appendix 1 -<br />

Soon after ovulation the granulosa cells lining the follicle wall begins to multiply to form a<br />

corpus luteum (CL) (Anderson, 2000). If fertilisation does not occur, prostagl<strong>and</strong>in pro-<br />

duced by the uterus is transferred to the ovary were it induces regression <strong>of</strong> CL. Corpus<br />

Luteum is essential for maintenance <strong>of</strong> pregnancy because CL is the only source <strong>of</strong> proges-<br />

terone in the sow <strong>and</strong> progesterone is essential during the whole period <strong>of</strong> pregnancy (van<br />

der Lende, 1989). The main function <strong>of</strong> CL is the secretion <strong>of</strong> progesterone. Progesterone<br />

stimulates uterine secretions (among other things IGF-1) <strong>and</strong> progesterone is the primary<br />

director <strong>of</strong> uterine development <strong>and</strong> secretion (Geisert & Yelich, 1997). Furthermore pro-<br />

gesterone is necessary to prevent contractions <strong>of</strong> the uterus, which would be caused by ele-<br />

vated levels <strong>of</strong> oestrogen (Hughes et al., 1996). Both oestrogen (Strob<strong>and</strong> & Lende, 1990)<br />

<strong>and</strong> progesterone are involved in preparing the uterine wall for the attachment <strong>of</strong> the em-<br />

bryos (Hughes & Varley, 1980). There are indications that LH is essential for maintenance<br />

<strong>of</strong> the CL beyond the first 12 days <strong>of</strong> pregnancy but that this requirement for LH decreases<br />

after day 29 <strong>of</strong> pregnancy (Peltoniemi et al., 1995; Hughes et al., 1996). To avoid regres-<br />

sion <strong>of</strong> CL at day 16 in oestrus cycle the embryo around day 11-12 secretes oestrogen<br />

(Bazer et al., 1982; Geisert & Yelich, 1997). This is also called ‘the embryonic signal’<br />

(Peltoniemi et al., 2000) or ‘the maternal recognition signal’ (Geisert et al., 1987). Accord-<br />

ing to Bazer et al. (1982;1984) oestrogen produced by the embryo directs the prostagl<strong>and</strong>in<br />

synthesised in the uterus into the uterine lumen which prevent their release to the uterine<br />

vascular bed <strong>and</strong> therefore prevents the regression <strong>of</strong> CL. Oxytocin (Geisert & Yelich,<br />

1997), which secretion from the endometrium is stimulated by oestrogen, <strong>and</strong> prolactin<br />

(Gross et al., 1990 cf Geisert et al., 1994) may also play a role in this redirection <strong>of</strong> pros-<br />

tagl<strong>and</strong>in. Bazer et al. (1984) suggested that prostagl<strong>and</strong>in is only luteolytic on day 12 or<br />

later after onset <strong>of</strong> oestrus. However, Estill et al. (1993) found that repeated treatment with<br />

prostagl<strong>and</strong>in from day five to day ten also resulted in luteolysis. Geisert et al. (1994) hy-<br />

pothesise that, before the first embryonic signal, progesterone controls the release <strong>of</strong> pros-<br />

tagl<strong>and</strong>in. It seems that a second prolonged embryonic oestrogen signal is required after<br />

day 14 to maintain pregnancy beyond day 30 (Geisert et al., 1987).<br />

At the time <strong>of</strong> parturition foetal cortisol stimulate the prostagl<strong>and</strong>in production in the<br />

uterus, which causes regression <strong>of</strong> the CL followed by a drop in progesterone level (Hughes<br />

et al., 1996; Anderson, 2000).<br />

Discussion<br />

The above-mentioned emphasize that a successful reproduction depends upon a series <strong>of</strong><br />

precisely timed physiological <strong>and</strong> endocrine events. These events may be disturbed by<br />

various factors. There are thus indications that the endocrine regulation may be disturbed<br />

by e.g. stress, as indicated by increased cortisol. In several studies, administration <strong>of</strong> corti-<br />

115


- Appendix 1 -<br />

sol in oestrus cycle impaired the pre-ovulatory oestrogen surge, the preovulatory LH surge<br />

<strong>and</strong> the timing between oestrus behaviour <strong>and</strong> LH surge in female pigs (Turner et al.,<br />

2002). Likewise did administration <strong>of</strong> AdrenoCorticoTrophic Hormone (ACTH), followed<br />

by an increase in cortisol in very early pregnancy impede the cleavage rate <strong>of</strong> the embryos<br />

(Razdan et al., 2002).<br />

Furthermore, there are also indications that the endocrine regulation may be influenced by<br />

energy intake. A positive correlation is believed to exist between the levels <strong>of</strong> the energy<br />

related hormones, insulin <strong>and</strong> IGF-1, <strong>and</strong> the levels <strong>of</strong> LH <strong>and</strong> FSH (Hughes & Pearce,<br />

1989; Peltoniemi et al., 1995). Finally, there are indications that a high level <strong>of</strong> feed intake<br />

may reduce the level <strong>of</strong> progesterone (Hughes & Pearce, 1989; Foxcr<strong>of</strong>t, 1997).<br />

It therefore seems that both stress <strong>and</strong> energy intake may influence the reproduction physiology<br />

<strong>of</strong> the sow. These factors might be very much affected by the <strong>group</strong> housing system.<br />

In commercial <strong>group</strong> housing systems, mixing <strong>of</strong> unfamiliar sows <strong>of</strong>ten occur <strong>and</strong> in a<br />

number <strong>of</strong> studies, mixing <strong>of</strong> unfamiliar sows have lead to a more or less long-lasting elevated<br />

level <strong>of</strong> cortisol in plasma (Pedersen et al., 1993; Tsuma et al., 1996; Olsson &<br />

Svendsen, 1997b). Likewise, there are several indications that <strong>group</strong> housing may lead to<br />

individual variation in energy intake between sows (Brouns & Edwards, 1994; Olsson &<br />

Svendsen, 1997a; Andersen et al., 1999).<br />

References<br />

Andersen, I.L., Bøe, K., Kristiansen, A.L., 1999. The influence <strong>of</strong> different feeding arrangements <strong>and</strong> food<br />

type on competition at feeding in pregnant sows. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 65, 91-104.<br />

Anderson, L.L., 2000. Pigs. In: Hafez, E. S. E., Hafez, B. (Eds.), <strong>Reproduction</strong> in farm animals. Lippincott<br />

Williams & Wilkins, New York, pp. 182-191.<br />

Ashworth, C.J., 1991. Embroyo development. Pig News <strong>and</strong> Information 12[4], 551-554.<br />

Bazer, F.W., Geisert, R.D., Thatcher, W.W., Robert, R.M., 1982. The establishment <strong>and</strong> maintenance <strong>of</strong><br />

pregnancy. In: Cole, D. J. A., Foxcr<strong>of</strong>t, G. R. (Eds.), Control <strong>of</strong> Pig reproduction. Butterworth Scientific,<br />

London, pp. 227-252.<br />

Bazer, F.W., Marengo, S.R., Geisert, R.D., Thatcher, W.W., 1984. Exocrine versus endocrine secretion <strong>of</strong><br />

prostagl<strong>and</strong>in F2a in the control <strong>of</strong> pregnancy in swine. Animal <strong>Reproduction</strong> Science 7, 115-132.<br />

Britt, J.H., Armstrong, J.D., Cox, N.M., Esbenshade, K.L., 1985. Control <strong>of</strong> follicular development during<br />

<strong>and</strong> after lactation in sows. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Reproduction</strong> <strong>and</strong> Fertility, Suppl. 33, 37-54.<br />

Brouns, F., Edwards, S.A., 1994. Social rank <strong>and</strong> feeding behaviour <strong>of</strong> <strong>group</strong>-<strong>housed</strong> sows fed competitively<br />

or ad libitum. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 39, 225-235.<br />

116


- Appendix 1 -<br />

Crombie, P.L., 1970. Ultrastructure <strong>of</strong> the foetal-maternal attachment in the pig. J. Physiol. Lond. 210, 101-<br />

102P.<br />

Dunbar, B.S., Bundman, D.S., 2003. Evidence for a role <strong>of</strong> the major glycoprotein in the structural mainte-<br />

nance <strong>of</strong> the pig zona pellucida. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Reproduction</strong> <strong>and</strong> Fertility 81, 363-376.<br />

Dziuk, P., 1970. Estimation <strong>of</strong> optimum time for insemination <strong>of</strong> gilts <strong>and</strong> ewes by double-mating at certain<br />

times relative to ovulation. J. Reprod. Fert. 22, 277-282.<br />

Dziuk, P., 1985. Effect <strong>of</strong> migration, distribution <strong>and</strong> spacing <strong>of</strong> pig embroys on pregnancy <strong>and</strong> fetal survival.<br />

Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Reproduction</strong> <strong>and</strong> Fertility, Suppl. 33, 57-63.<br />

Estill, C.T., Britt, J.H., Gadsby, J.E., 1993. Repeated Administration <strong>of</strong> Prostagl<strong>and</strong>in F2a during the Early<br />

Luteal Phase Causes Premature Luteolysis in the Pig. Biology <strong>of</strong> <strong>Reproduction</strong> 49, 181-185.<br />

Foxcr<strong>of</strong>t, G.R., 1997. Mechanisms mediating nutritional effects on embryonic survival in pigs. Journal <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>Reproduction</strong> <strong>and</strong> Fertility, Suppl. 52, 47-61.<br />

Foxcr<strong>of</strong>t, G.R., Hunter, W.G., 1985. Basic physiology <strong>of</strong> follicular maturation in the pig. J. Reprod. Fert. ,<br />

Suppl. 33, 1-19.<br />

Fr<strong>and</strong>son, R. D. <strong>and</strong> T. L. Spurgeon. 1992. Anatomy <strong>and</strong> physiology <strong>of</strong> farm animals. Lea & Febiger, Mal-<br />

vern, Pennsylvania.<br />

Geisert, R.D., Short, E.C., Morgan, G.L., 1994. Establishment <strong>of</strong> Pregnancy in Domestic Farm Species. Em-<br />

broynic Mortality in domestic species Kapitel 2, 51pp.<br />

Geisert, R.D., Yelich, J.V., 1997. Regulation <strong>of</strong> conceptus development <strong>and</strong> attachment in pigs. Journal <strong>of</strong><br />

<strong>Reproduction</strong> <strong>and</strong> Fertility, Suppl. 52[133], 149.<br />

Geisert, R.D., Zavy, M.T., Wettemann, R.P., Biggers, B.G., 1987. Length <strong>of</strong> pseudopregnancy <strong>and</strong> pattern <strong>of</strong><br />

uterine protein release as influenced by time <strong>and</strong> duration <strong>of</strong> oestrogen administration in the pig.<br />

Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Reproduction</strong> <strong>and</strong> Fertility 79, 163-172.<br />

Hughes, P.E., Pearce, G.P., 1989. The endocrine basis <strong>of</strong> nutrition-reproduction interactions. In: Barnett, J. L.,<br />

Hennessy, D. P. (Eds.), Manipulating Pig Production II. Australasian Pig Science Association, Werribee,<br />

pp. 290-295.<br />

Hughes, P.E., Pearce, G.P., Hemsworth, P.H., 1996. <strong>Reproduction</strong>. In: Taverner, M. R., Dunkin, A. C. (Eds.),<br />

Pig Production.pp. 67-88.<br />

Hughes, P. E. <strong>and</strong> M. A. Varley. 1980. <strong>Reproduction</strong> in the pig. Butterworth & Co, London.<br />

Hunter, R.H.F., 1967. The effects <strong>of</strong> delayed insemination on fertilization <strong>and</strong> early cleavage in the pig. J.<br />

Reprod. Fert. 13, 133-147.<br />

Hunter, R.H.F., 1981. Sperm transport <strong>and</strong> reservoirs in the pig oviduet in relation to the time <strong>of</strong> ovulation.<br />

Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Reproduction</strong> <strong>and</strong> Fertility 63, 109-117.<br />

117


- Appendix 1 -<br />

Hunter, R.H.F., 1990. Fertilization <strong>of</strong> pig eggs in vivo <strong>and</strong> in vitro. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Reproduction</strong> <strong>and</strong> Fertility,<br />

Suppl. 40, 211-226.<br />

Kemp, B., Soede, N.M., 1997. Consequences <strong>of</strong> variation in interval from insemination to ovulation on fertili-<br />

zation in pigs. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Reproduction</strong> <strong>and</strong> Fertility, Suppl. 52, 79-89.<br />

Langendijk, P. 2001. Is there af future for the boar? The role <strong>of</strong> boar stimuli in reproductive processes around<br />

estrus in the pig. Ph.d. Department <strong>of</strong> Animal Sciences, Wageningen Institute <strong>of</strong> Animal Sciences,<br />

Wageningen University, The Netherl<strong>and</strong>s.<br />

Maddox-Hyttel, P., 2003. Befrugtningens indre univers. L<strong>and</strong>sudvalget for Svin, Danske slagterier.<br />

Reproduktionsseminar 13-03-2003. Pp 1-9.<br />

Nissen, A.K., Soede, N.M., Hyttel, P., Schmidt, M., D'Hoore, L., 1997. The influence <strong>of</strong> time <strong>of</strong> insemination<br />

relative to time <strong>of</strong> ovulation on farrowing frequency <strong>and</strong> litter size in sows, as investigated by ultra-<br />

sonography. Theriogenology 47, 1571-1582.<br />

Olsson, A.-C., Svendsen, J., 1997a. Effekter av olika konkurrensförhåll<strong>and</strong>en vid utfodring på suggors hälsa<br />

och produktion. SLU Rapport 113, Sveriges Lantbruksuniversitet, 37pp.<br />

Olsson, A.-C., Svendsen, J., 1997b. The importance <strong>of</strong> familiarity when <strong>group</strong>ing gilts, <strong>and</strong> the effect <strong>of</strong> fre-<br />

quent <strong>group</strong>ing during gestation. Swedish J. agric. Res. 27, 33-43.<br />

Pedersen, L.J., Rojkittikhun, T., Einarsson, S., Edqvist, L.-E., 1993. Postweaning <strong>group</strong>ed sows: effects <strong>of</strong><br />

aggression on hormonal patterns <strong>and</strong> oestrous behaviour. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 38, 25-<br />

39.<br />

Pedersen, P.N., Navnt<strong>of</strong>t, D., 1996. Brunstforhold hos søer. Erfaring 9602, Den rullende Afprøvning, L<strong>and</strong>sudvalget<br />

for Svin, Danske Slagterier, 6pp.<br />

Peltoniemi, O.A.T., Easton, B.G., Love, R.J., Klupiec, C., Evans, G., 1995. Effect <strong>of</strong> chronic treatment with a<br />

GnRH agonist (Goserelin) on LH secretion <strong>and</strong> early pregnancy in gilts. Animal <strong>Reproduction</strong> Science<br />

40, 121-133.<br />

Peltoniemi, O.A.T., Tast, A., Love, R.J., 2000. Factors affecting reproduction in the pig: seasonal effects <strong>and</strong><br />

restricted feeding <strong>of</strong> the pregnant ilt <strong>and</strong> sow. Animal <strong>Reproduction</strong> Science 60, 173-184.<br />

Pope, W.F., First, N.L., 1985. Factors affecting the survival <strong>of</strong> pig embryos. Theriogenology 23[1], 91-105.<br />

Prunier, A., Quesnel, H., 2000. Nutritional influences on the hormonal control <strong>of</strong> reproduction in female pigs.<br />

Livestock Production Science 63, 1-16.<br />

Razdan, P., Mwanza, A.M., Kindahl, H., Rodriguez-Martinez, H., Hultén, F., Einarsson, S., 2002. Effect <strong>of</strong><br />

repeated ACTH-stimulation on early embryonic development <strong>and</strong> hormonal pr<strong>of</strong>iles in sows. Animal<br />

<strong>Reproduction</strong> Science 70, 127-137.<br />

Rodriguez-Martinez, H., Tienthai, P., Suzuki, K., Funahashi, H., Ekwall, H., Johannisson, A., 2001. Involvement<br />

<strong>of</strong> oviduct in sperm capacitation <strong>and</strong> oocyte development in pigs. <strong>Reproduction</strong> Supplement 58,<br />

129-145.<br />

118


- Appendix 1 -<br />

Scott, M.A., 2000. A glimpse at sperm function in vivo: sperm transport <strong>and</strong> epithelial interaction in the fe-<br />

male reproduction tract. Animal <strong>Reproduction</strong> Science 60-61[337], 348.<br />

Soede, N.M., 1993. Boar stimuli around insemination affect reproductive processes in pigs: a review. Animal<br />

<strong>Reproduction</strong> Science 32, 107-125.<br />

Strob<strong>and</strong>, H.W.J., van der Lende, T., 1990. Embryonic <strong>and</strong> uterine development during early pregnancy in<br />

pigs. Journal <strong>of</strong> <strong>Reproduction</strong> <strong>and</strong> Fertility, Suppl. 40, 261-277.<br />

Tsuma, V.T., Einarsson, S., Madej, A., Kindahl, H., Lundeheim, N., Rojkittikhun, T., 1996. Endocrine chan-<br />

ges during <strong>group</strong> housing <strong>of</strong> primaparous sows in early pregnancy. Acta vet. Sc<strong>and</strong>. 37, 481-490.<br />

Turner, A.I., Hemsworth, P.H., Tilbrook, A.J., 2002. Susceptibility <strong>of</strong> reproduction in female pigs to impair-<br />

ment by stress <strong>and</strong> the role <strong>of</strong> the hypothalamo-pituitary axis. Reproduc. Fertil. Dev. 14, 377-391.<br />

van der Lende, T. 1989. Impact <strong>of</strong> early pregnancy on prenatal development in the pig. Ph.D. Department <strong>of</strong><br />

Animal Husb<strong>and</strong>ry, Wageningen University.<br />

van der Lende, T., Soede, N.M., Kemp, B.V., 1994. Embryo mortality <strong>and</strong> prolificacy in the pig. In: Cole, D.<br />

J. A., Wiseman, J., Varley, M. A. (Eds.), Principles <strong>of</strong> pig science. Nothingham University Press,pp.<br />

297-317.<br />

119

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!