26.07.2013 Views

Announcing 'Stammering Research' - Stammering Research - UCL

Announcing 'Stammering Research' - Stammering Research - UCL

Announcing 'Stammering Research' - Stammering Research - UCL

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>Stammering</strong> <strong>Research</strong>. Vol. 1.<br />

Commentary on "Involvement of social factors in stuttering:<br />

A review and assessment of current methodology"<br />

By A. Furnham and S. Davis<br />

Martine Vanryckeghem<br />

Department of Communicative Disorders, University of Central Florida,<br />

Orlando, FL 32816, USA<br />

martinev@mail.ucf.edu<br />

Furnham and Davis (2004) should be commended for their thorough overview of the methods and<br />

findings that relate to the role of affective and social factors in the assessment of children and adults<br />

who stutter. I wish to comment on two issues discussed in their fine article. One relates to the<br />

assessment of the speech-associated attitude of children and adults who stutter and the other on their<br />

emotional reaction to particular speech situations.<br />

In the section on preschoolers and school-age children, the authors mention that the Communication<br />

Attitude Test (Brutten, 1984; Brutten & Vanryckeghem, 2003a) has been shown to be a reliable and<br />

valid test for assessing the speech-associated beliefs of children. Indeed, research with the<br />

Communication Attitude Test (CAT), which has been translated into more than 10 languages has<br />

consistently shown that the speech-associated attitude of children who stutter is significantly more<br />

negative than that of their nonstuttering peers. Moreover, the extent of this difference, which was<br />

found to be present at the age of six (Vanryckeghem & Brutten, 1997), suggested that the betweengroup<br />

difference might be present at an even earlier time period, one closer to the time at which<br />

stuttering has its onset. This highlighted the need for early detection of mal-attitude toward speech.<br />

Indeed, as Furnham and Davis (2004) have pointed out "An instrument capable of determining the<br />

communication attitudes of children close to stuttering onset would be useful in several areas" (p.115).<br />

In this regard, as Davis and Furnham (2004) have already mentioned in their "authors' response to<br />

commentaries", a preschool-kindergarten version of the CAT has recently been designed. <strong>Research</strong><br />

with this self-report test, the KiddyCAT, (Vanryckeghem & Brutten, 2002, 2004; Vanryckeghem,<br />

Hernandez & Brutten, 2001; Vanryckeghem, Brutten & Hernandez, 2004) has been shown to<br />

distinguish the speech-associated attitude of normally fluent children, as young as three, from that of<br />

youngsters who are incipient stutterers. As such, it has served to put aside Conture's (2001) concern as<br />

to whether a "self-report questionnaire procedure could be readily and reliably administered to<br />

preschool/early elementary school-age children (between 2 and 7), the age period when most children<br />

actually begin to stutter" (p. 72). Indeed, research has shown the KiddyCAT to be a useful differential<br />

diagnostic tool for use with preschoolers. The apparent utility of the KiddyCAT is currently spawning<br />

further study of its reliability and validity.<br />

Furnham and Davis (2004) also reported that their "preliminary searches indicate that no empirical<br />

work has been conducted examining the anxiety levels of children who stutter when compared to fluent<br />

controls" (p.118). Nevertheless, it should be noted that Brutten and colleagues (Bakker, 1980, 1995;<br />

Brutten, 1973, 1981a,b, 1982; Brutten & Janssen, 1981; Ezrati-Vinacour & Levin, 2004; Hanson,<br />

Gronhovd & Rice, 1981; Vanryckeghem, 1998, 1999; Vanryckeghem & Brutten, 1998; Vanryckeghem<br />

& Verghese, 2004) have, over the last couple of decades, published research on the Speech Situation<br />

Checklist (Brutten, 1965a,b; Brutten & Vanryckeghem, 2003a,b), a state test specifically designed for<br />

use with children and adults who stutter. The Speech Situation Checklist (SSC), an internally<br />

consistent (Brutten & Vanryckeghem, 2003a,b; Ezrati-Vinacour & Levin, 2004) self-report test<br />

procedure, that is part of the Behavior Assessment Battery (Brutten & Vanryckeghem, 2003a,b), makes<br />

it possible to investigate both the extent to which various speech situations evoke negative emotional<br />

reactions and speech disruption. The results of various SSC studies (Brutten & Janssen, 1981; Brutten<br />

& Vanryckeghem, 2003a,b; Trotter, 1982, 1983; Vanryckeghem & Verghese, 2004) have shown that<br />

both the emotional and dysfluency scores of grade-schoolers and adults who stutter are significantly<br />

higher than those of people who do not stutter. Notable, too, is the finding of a high and statistically<br />

significant correlation between the negative emotional and speech disruption reports of children and<br />

adults who stutter (.82 and .87, respectively) (Brutten & Vanryckeghem, 2003a,b). The latter points to<br />

a meaningful link between the emotional and behavioral displays of youngsters and adults who stutter.<br />

In addition, it should be noted that self-reports of children and adults have been shown to significantly<br />

correlate with the extent to which stuttering has been observed to occur (Guitar, 1976; Vanryckeghem<br />

& Brutten, 1996).<br />

304

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!