26.07.2013 Views

Announcing 'Stammering Research' - Stammering Research - UCL

Announcing 'Stammering Research' - Stammering Research - UCL

Announcing 'Stammering Research' - Stammering Research - UCL

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>Stammering</strong> <strong>Research</strong>. Vol. 1.<br />

The achievement of multi-unit turns described in this section is also contingent upon speaker-recipient<br />

collaboration but here recipients play a more pro-active role. For instance, by passing the opportunity to<br />

take a turn at talk at a transition relevance place, a recipient may be seen to initiate a possible multi-unit<br />

turn by the speaker. Failure to take a turn at talk, of course, does not necessarily suggest complete<br />

inactivity, and recipients often produce some form of behaviour, including vocalisations such as 'uh huh',<br />

'mm hmm', and 'yeah' and associated non-verbal behaviours like head nods. Schegloff questions the<br />

traditional linguistic treatment of these as evidence of attention, interest, and understanding, based as they<br />

are on a consideration of these events taken in the aggregate with each removed from its context of<br />

occurrence, and advocates instead the analysis of particular instances. The following quote from Schegloff<br />

(1982:80) serves to illustrate the methodological distinctions between the two approaches:<br />

Although appeals to signalling attention, interest, and/or understanding appear<br />

equivocal when invoked on behalf of the aggregated occurrence of tokens such as “uh<br />

huh”, “yeah”, and the like removed from their particular environments, such accounts<br />

may be viable and strong when introduced for delimited and described cases in which<br />

the relevance of these for the parties to the conversation at that point in the talk can be<br />

shown. Appropriate sets of such analysed single cases may then be assembled to<br />

display recurrent practices, themes, structures, etc.<br />

Schegloff (1982:81) goes on to assert that, by passing an opportunity to produce a full turn at talk, 'uh<br />

huh' and cognate utterances 'exhibit on the part of its producer an understanding that an extended unit of<br />

talk is underway by another, and that it is not yet....complete'. In categorising these items as 'continuers', he<br />

draws our attention to some important issues. First, bearing in mind the nature of the turn-taking system, a<br />

TRP is an appropriate position for participants to display their understanding of the current state of talk.<br />

Second, it should be pointed out that continuers do not merely 'claim' an understanding that an extended<br />

turn is underway but actually display this understanding by declining to produce a full turn at talk. Finally,<br />

only a limited set of behavioural productions have the ability to achieve such an outcome (i.e. display this<br />

understanding without constituting a full turn in their own right) and fulfil the conditions of a continuer.<br />

This characterisation is extremely pertinent to the situation that people who stammer often find<br />

themselves in and the ambiguity that dysfluencies often introduce into the conversational proceedings<br />

means that such a display of understanding is all the more important. Apart from their function as<br />

'continuers', Schegloff suggests that tokens like 'mm hmm' and 'uh huh' can also be employed to pass an<br />

opportunity to initiate repair. We have already seen how such tokens are categorized as continuers on the<br />

grounds that they pass an opportunity to take a full turn at talk. In a similar way their treatment as<br />

indications of understanding and agreement is based on the assumption that if there were problems of<br />

understanding or agreement in relation to prior talk, opportunities to initiate repair would be taken up.<br />

However, as Schegloff (1982:88) emphasises, there is a clear distinction between this usage and the<br />

continuer usage:<br />

With respect to the understanding of, and agreement with, what a prior speaker has said<br />

and done, "uh huh" is merely a claim of understanding. Such a claim may turn out to be<br />

incorrect; and passing one opportunity to initiate repair is compatible with initiating<br />

repair later. The status of "uh huh" as an indication of understanding or agreement is<br />

equivocal in a way in which its status as a continuer is not.<br />

As most people who stammer are only too well aware from past experience that dysfluent talk has a<br />

high potential to be a source of interactional trouble, it is likely that they will be on the lookout for 'otherinitiated<br />

repair' 17 . Similarly, those recipients who are sensitive to the difficulties experienced by people<br />

who stammer may want to offer reassurance that they have understood what has been said, and one way in<br />

which this claim of understanding can be achieved is through the production of certain response tokens.<br />

Schegloff's treatment of discourse as an interactional achievement, then, appears to provide a solid<br />

framework on which to build an analysis of conversations that are characterised by stammering. Where<br />

fluent speech is disrupted by repetitions and blocks of various types the usual signals which promote<br />

smooth interaction may be distorted. In such circumstances the nature of discourse as an interactional<br />

achievement is brought into sharp relief.<br />

The differential deployment of response tokens<br />

Gail Jefferson's (1984) analysis of 'acknowledgement tokens' can be viewed as a development of<br />

Schegloff's (1982) study in that it takes up an issue referred to but not addressed in that paper, namely the<br />

difference of meaning or usage between tokens such as 'uh huh' and 'yeah'. A consideration of the kinds of<br />

work that these tokens do, raises a number of important issues, many of which have a direct bearing on the<br />

problem of stammering. Jefferson (1984:200) suggests that a clear distinction can be made between certain<br />

17 For an explanation of this concept see Schegloff, Jefferson and Sacks (1990). Levinson (1983:339-345)<br />

and Nofsinger (1991:124-132) provide good summaries of many of the key issues relating to repair.<br />

264

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!