26.07.2013 Views

Announcing 'Stammering Research' - Stammering Research - UCL

Announcing 'Stammering Research' - Stammering Research - UCL

Announcing 'Stammering Research' - Stammering Research - UCL

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>Stammering</strong> <strong>Research</strong>. Vol. 1.<br />

stammer, then, it is not just the elimination of unwanted silences at TRPs that is important, but also the<br />

specific manner in which this is achieved. It is, however, important to point out that the steps that people<br />

who stammer often take to reduce the impact of silent dysfluencies at TRPs are not without their<br />

drawbacks, and as Hopper (1992:99) suggests, in attempting to minimise silences we thereby make overlap<br />

more likely. The inevitable tension between these two opposites raises the possibility that the 'fear of<br />

silence' experienced by people who stammer will generate additional difficulties in terms of overlapping<br />

speech. While a consideration of the literature on simultaneous talk would serve to develop some of the<br />

issues outlined above, it may be more productive, in the current context, to elaborate upon the notion of<br />

turn-taking as a constraint by focusing on the organisation of adjacency pair sequences.<br />

4.2 Adjacency pairs<br />

One of the most basic, yet analytically powerful concepts so far devised by conversation analysts is the<br />

adjacency pair. Closely tied in with the turn-taking system, this concept has contributed greatly to our<br />

understanding of various aspects of the organization of conversations and may also be able to shed light on<br />

certain features of stammering. Adjacency pairs are fundamental to the organization of conversation (see<br />

Benson & Hughes 1983:175; Levinson 1983:304) and can be formally defined as 'adjacent utterances<br />

produced by two different speakers where the production of the first part of the pair makes the production<br />

of the second part sequentially relevant' (Benson & Hughes 1983:173). Adjacency pairs are ubiquitous in<br />

conversation and paired utterances such as question/answer, greeting/greeting, offer/acceptance, and<br />

compliment/response are familiar examples. While they also do considerable interactional work (including<br />

opening and closing, selection of speakers and repair of the turn-taking system) their use involves the same<br />

basic rule:<br />

given the recognisable production of a first pair part, on its first possible completion its<br />

speaker should stop and a next speaker should start and produce a second pair part from<br />

the pair type the first is recognisably a member of (Schegloff and Sacks 1974:239).<br />

A close examination of naturally occurring conversation reveals that Schegloff and Sack's<br />

characterization does not always hold up, and that strict adjacency is too strong a requirement. In reality it<br />

is often the case that first and second parts of adjacency pairs are separated by other talk and may even be<br />

several turns apart. Nevertheless, by employing the concept of 'conditional relevance' (Schegloff<br />

1968:1083) we are able to retain the structural import of adjacency pairs. So while the two parts of an<br />

adjacency pair may be separated by an 'insertion sequence' (see Nofsinger 1991:61-65; Levinson 1983:304-<br />

306) the second part still remains relevant and expectable. In other words, it is the expectation of the<br />

second part, rather than the actual adjacency, that bonds the pair together (Nofsinger 1991:64). The first<br />

part of an adjacency pair places a powerful conversational constraint on what follows, to the extent that a<br />

failure to respond to a first part in the appropriate fashion will be interpreted as an 'official absence'<br />

(Downes 1984:237).<br />

Although telephone openings may 'seem a peculiar object on which to lavish scholarly attention'<br />

(Schegloff 1986:111) they are probably the ideal place to begin our examination of adjacency pairs. The<br />

seemingly routine and even trivial talk that occurs in these environments conceals an elaborate organisation<br />

and, as Hopper (1992:51) observes, 'the telephone opening packs a great deal of information and<br />

communicative accomplishment into just a few seconds'. One useful function that telephone openings<br />

perform is to provide an 'anchor position' for the initiation of first topic and this is situated after a fairly<br />

standard pattern of four core opening sequences: a summons/answer sequence; an identification/ sequence;<br />

a greeting/greeting sequence; and an exchange of 'howareyou' sequences (Schegloff 1986:116). While it is<br />

the caller who typically introduces the first topic (usually the reason for the call) its placement in the anchor<br />

position is a collaborative achievement. There are procedures whereby the recipient can introduce first<br />

topic and indeed a variety of opportunities exist for either participant to initiate first topic before the anchor<br />

position and potentially control the shape of the conversation (Schegloff 1986:117). In this respect 'routine'<br />

openings are not automatic responses designed to conform to a prespecified blueprint but rather are more<br />

accurately characterised as interactional achievements.<br />

As conversational openings are constructed primarily from adjacency pairs (in that the four core<br />

opening sequences each represent a distinct type) a detailed examination of the ways in which people who<br />

stammer cope with telephone beginnings should provide a valuable insight into the relationship, if any,<br />

between stammering and adjacency pairs. Schegloff's (1968) analysis of five hundred telephone openings<br />

demonstrated that the ringing of the phone should be regarded as the initial turn in the conversation because<br />

it represents a nonlinguistic realisation of a caller's summoning act. The treatment of the telephone ring as<br />

the first part of a summons/answer adjacency pair sequence explains why it is that the person being called,<br />

the one with least information about the identity and purposes of the caller, speaks first. That the recipient<br />

is under an intense obligation to respond is even demonstrated by the only exceptional case in Schegloff's<br />

corpus, where the caller rather than the recipient spoke first. This can be understood as a reaction to the<br />

261

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!