26.07.2013 Views

Announcing 'Stammering Research' - Stammering Research - UCL

Announcing 'Stammering Research' - Stammering Research - UCL

Announcing 'Stammering Research' - Stammering Research - UCL

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

<strong>Stammering</strong> <strong>Research</strong>. Vol. 1.<br />

vocalisation, however, they are conveying the message that it is their turn and they intend to take it.<br />

Unfortunately some people who stammer experience very real difficulty in vocalising at all and these<br />

people are particularly vulnerable at transition points. It is they who are likely to be most adversely affected<br />

by the restrictions imposed by telephone communication where visual signs of struggle are unavailable to<br />

the listener. Others make a clear choice to remain silent for fear of 'exposing' themselves as people who<br />

stammer but, perhaps ironically, risk leaving themselves relatively powerless in the turn-taking economy.<br />

The critical role played by silence in the turn-taking system creates a dilemma for people who stammer,<br />

as many develop an acute fear of speechlessness. Some of the conversational consequences of this phobia<br />

are alluded to in the following extract from Sheehan (1970:27):<br />

stuttering is not just a difficulty of starting, for many stutterers it is a difficulty of<br />

terminating. The more severe the stutterer, the more trouble he has getting started, and<br />

the more trouble he has getting stopped. An aspect of listening to a severe stutterer that<br />

exasperates many people is that he never seems to know when to stop, as hope sags that<br />

each next word will be the stutterers last.<br />

One of the hypotheses put forward for this 'clinically observed phenomenon' is that because people who<br />

stammer experience most trouble at the beginning of utterances 16 they become conditioned to fear silence.<br />

Filibustering is a common feature of stammering (Sheehan 1970) and on the basis of earlier discussion<br />

which drew attention to the severe competition that exists at TRPs, this concern to hold the floor is<br />

understandable. However, Sheehan (1970:29) suggested that people who stammer 'be taught to risk and to<br />

court the fear of silence, by permitting pauses in his speech....., pauses to permit the stutterer sufficient<br />

breath so that he is not trying to force out words on residual air'. This may appear sound advice, but given<br />

the nature of stammering and the exigencies of the turn-taking system, pausing at or near TRPs may create<br />

the wrong impression for listeners, who could justifiably interpret this as their opportunity to speak. I<br />

would suggest, therefore, that the fear of silence, which is common to many people who stammer, has a<br />

quite legitimate basis and derives, at least in part, from the operation of the turn-taking system and the<br />

constraints this imposes on conversationalists. In this respect, it was perhaps erroneous for Sheehan<br />

(1970:29) to refer to the 'time pressure set' as 'self-imposed'. Moreover, these difficulties are exacerbated in<br />

multi-party talk where the increased competition for turns makes self-selection far from automatic, a fact<br />

that may have important ramifications for the development of the conversation. For example, when a<br />

participant wishes to respond to something the current speaker is saying, it is often crucial that they get in at<br />

the next TRP. Otherwise whoever does become the next speaker may select someone else and the<br />

relevance of what they were going to say may be lost. These additional constraints only serve to illustrate<br />

the need for people who stammer to devise strategies that will allow them equal access in the turn taking<br />

economy. For some, these techniques, which have been cultivated through years of painful conversational<br />

experience, are quite effective in turn management terms. For others, however, the severity of the stammer<br />

precludes any such straightforward manipulation of the turn taking rules. An inability to control silences in<br />

any significant way leaves these people who stammer in an extremely vulnerable and powerless position<br />

within the turn taking system, especially in telephone conversations. This may help to account for the welldocumented<br />

feelings of frustration and anger that people who stammer often experience and may also<br />

explain the reluctance of many people who stammer to use the phone. Given Nofsinger's (1991:96) claim<br />

that research has consistently shown that perceptions of participants' communicative competence and even<br />

the viability of their conversation is related to the way they manage conversational silence, it is also<br />

extremely likely that many people who stammer are unfairly judged in terms of their overall<br />

communication skills.<br />

The preceding discussion has focused on silences and considered the ways in which their placement has<br />

differential implications. We have considered some of the possible strategies that people who stammer may<br />

employ in an attempt to minimise silences and commented on the specific advantages of employing these<br />

at transition places. Of course, as I have already indicated, many of these techniques are employed<br />

regularly by people who do not stammer, usually to secure a turn while formulating a response. Empirical<br />

research, however, is needed to clarify these issues. For example, to what extent do people who stammer<br />

manipulate these turn-holding techniques in an attempt to overcome difficulties which are specifically<br />

related to their fluency disorder? Nevertheless, on the basis of our current knowledge of talk-in-interaction,<br />

to crudely dismiss them as avoidance strategies (as some of the literature on stammering attempts to do)<br />

risks overlooking their possible role in helping people who stammer secure a turn at a point in the<br />

conversation where the speech difficulties put the maintenance of speakership in jeopardy.<br />

There is of course the additional possibility that by employing devices that are widely used in ‘normal’<br />

talk, people who stammer are able to conceal their disorder more effectively. For many people who<br />

16 There has been considerable research carried out into the various linguistic determinants of dysfluency<br />

(e.g Brown, 1945; Howell, Au-Yeung & Sackin, 1999; Wingate 1988) and these findings need to be taken<br />

into account in any examination of the relationship between turn-taking system and stammering.<br />

260

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!