26.07.2013 Views

Announcing 'Stammering Research' - Stammering Research - UCL

Announcing 'Stammering Research' - Stammering Research - UCL

Announcing 'Stammering Research' - Stammering Research - UCL

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>Stammering</strong> <strong>Research</strong>. Vol. 1.<br />

and speaker exchange is finely coordinated so that one person speaks at a time and turn-taking occurs<br />

smoothly with minimal gaps or overlaps. Given the fact that even those problems which do arise in<br />

coordination usually occur around TRPs, it is clear that timing is a major accomplishment of<br />

conversationalists. Precision timing, of course, also requires a high degree of skilled listenership and as<br />

Sacks et al. (1978:43) point out, the turn-taking system with all its various turn-allocational techniques:<br />

builds in an intrinsic motivation for listening to all utterances in a conversation,<br />

independent of other possible motivations such as interest and politeness. In the variety<br />

of techniques for arriving at a next speaker, and in their ordered character, it obliges any<br />

willing or potentially intending speaker to listen to, and analyse, each utterance across<br />

its delivery.<br />

Listeners need to monitor the conversation in case they are selected by the current speaker, and potential<br />

speakers need to be alert to upcoming TRPs if they are to intervene successfully. For instance, the second<br />

option, that of self-selection, requires a high degree of conversational competence on the part of the<br />

listener, particularly in multi-party settings. This is because the 'pressure rule' (see Downes 1984:254)<br />

comes into play here, leading to competition between potential speakers over who gets the next turn. As<br />

alluded to above, the first person to speak has exclusive rights to the turn and others who have started are<br />

obliged to stop. The speaker-continues option further increases this sense of urgency for potential speakers<br />

as any delay may result in the current speaker resuming his or her turn.<br />

While it is clear from the above that the achievement of precision timing in turn transfer requires an<br />

ability to predict an up-coming TRP, we need to examine the basis upon which this prediction is made.<br />

Robert Hopper (1992:104-106) considered three candidate indicators of transition relevance (utterance<br />

syntax, terminal pitch contour, and pauses) and placed more weight on the first, because it not only allows<br />

participants to identify transition relevance as it occurs, but also makes it possible for listeners to project<br />

unit completion slightly in advance of its occurrence. While he found evidence that terminal pitch cues<br />

were of some use, he concluded that speakers did not 'reliably distinguish transition relevance on this cue<br />

alone' (Hopper 1992: 105). They were not an aid to the prevention of overlap, nor did they make it less<br />

likely that the current speaker would continue. Similarly pauses are not reliable indicators of turn<br />

completion because a large number of pauses are not transition relevant and most turn exchanges are<br />

'pauseless' (Hopper 1992:106). However, he showed that speakers classify pauses in terms of transition<br />

relevance and points out that long pauses during telephone conversations are treated by participants as<br />

problematic and may lead them into ‘speakership’ (Hopper 1992:106). The fact that turn transfer may also<br />

be facilitated through the employment of nonverbal signals highlights one of the possible advantages of<br />

analysing data drawn from telephone rather than face-to-face interaction. As Robert Hopper (1992:103)<br />

rightly pointed out, telephone conversation is the ideal site for the investigation of the turn taking<br />

mechanism 'because its constraints simplify the next-speaker's problem: there are only two speakers in a<br />

vocal-only system' (the potential methodological benefits of analyzing the telephone talk of people who<br />

stammer are discussed in more detail below).<br />

<strong>Stammering</strong> and turn-taking: some initial considerations<br />

It should be clear from the preceding discussion that there are a number of fundamental issues which<br />

need to be addressed concerning the relationship between stammering and the turn management system. A<br />

conversation analytic focus may help us to distinguish between the various forms and features of<br />

stammering in more meaningful ways. For instance, silent blocks are likely to have very different<br />

interactional consequences than repetitions. The 'pressure rule' referred to earlier will inevitably create<br />

serious problems for those who, because of their speech impediment, can be regarded as 'slow conversation<br />

starters'. In this respect, those whose speech dysfluency consists predominantly of silent blocks are more<br />

likely to experience difficulty in securing their turn than those whose stammering episodes are more<br />

audible. This is particularly true in telephone interaction where non-verbal signs of struggle are not<br />

available to the other participant. Therefore an examination of the role of silence in the turn-taking system<br />

and the implications of this for those categorized as people who stammer may help to develop our<br />

understanding of the various characteristics of stammering and to refine the analytic distinction between<br />

silent and vocal dysfluencies.<br />

One of the most important observations that can be made with regard to silence in conversation is that<br />

not all instances of this phenomenon are treated identically. Indeed it is the relationship of a silence to the<br />

turn-taking structure that is important, and this determines whether it will be classified as a gap, pause or<br />

lapse (see Sacks et al. 1978:27). A gap refers to a silence that may occur between the end of one speaker's<br />

turn and some listener self-selecting for the next turn. This gap is 'usually brief, often about one second or<br />

less' (Nofsinger 1991:94). A lapse, on the other hand, occurs when none of the turn-taking techniques<br />

described above are employed and the conversation comes to a halt. The third category of silence identified<br />

257

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!