26.07.2013 Views

Child temperament and parental personality: continuity and ... - Doria

Child temperament and parental personality: continuity and ... - Doria

Child temperament and parental personality: continuity and ... - Doria

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

<strong>Child</strong> <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>parental</strong> <strong>personality</strong>:<br />

<strong>continuity</strong> <strong>and</strong> transactional change<br />

Niina Komsi<br />

Department of Psychology<br />

University of Helsinki, Finl<strong>and</strong><br />

Academic dissertation to be publicly discussed,<br />

by due permission of the Faculty of Behavioral Sciences<br />

at the University of Helsinki in Auditorium XIII, Unioninkatu 34,<br />

on the 5 th of June, 2009, at 12 o’clock<br />

UNIVERSITY OF HELSINKI<br />

Department of Psychology<br />

Studies 60: 2009


Supervisor:<br />

Professor Katri Räikkönen-Talvitie<br />

Department of Psychology, University of Helsinki, Finl<strong>and</strong><br />

Reviewers:<br />

Professor Sam Putnam<br />

Psychology Department, Bowdoin College, USA<br />

Professor Maria A. Gartstein<br />

Department of Psychology, Washington State University, USA<br />

Opponent:<br />

Professor Franz J. Neyer<br />

Institut für Psychologie, Friedrich-Schiller-Universität Jena, Germany<br />

ISSN 0781-8254<br />

ISBN 978-952-10-5593-5 (pbk.)<br />

ISBN 978-952-10-5594-2 (PDF)<br />

http://www.ethesis.helsinki.fi<br />

Helsinki University Printing House<br />

Helsinki 2009<br />

2


CONTENTS<br />

ABSTRACT 5<br />

TIIVISTELMÄ 7<br />

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS 9<br />

LIST OF ORIGINAL PUBLICATIONS 10<br />

1 INTRODUCTION<br />

1.1 Temperament dimensions <strong>and</strong> <strong>personality</strong> traits: theoretically,<br />

11<br />

temporally <strong>and</strong> practically related constructs<br />

1.2 Temperament in childhood within the developmental framework of Mary<br />

11<br />

K. Rothbart<br />

1.2.1 Continuity of <strong>temperament</strong> from infancy onwards 13<br />

1.2.2 Mothers <strong>and</strong> fathers as informants of the child’s <strong>temperament</strong> 16<br />

1.3 Personality traits in adulthood 18<br />

1.3.1 Stability <strong>and</strong> change in <strong>personality</strong> traits<br />

1.3.2 Personality traits of extraversion <strong>and</strong> neuroticism during<br />

18<br />

adulthood<br />

1.4 Transactional development of <strong>parental</strong> <strong>personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> child<br />

20<br />

<strong>temperament</strong><br />

1.4.1 Implications of child <strong>temperament</strong> for <strong>parental</strong> extraversion <strong>and</strong><br />

21<br />

neuroticism<br />

1.4.2 Implications of <strong>parental</strong> extraversion <strong>and</strong> neuroticism for child<br />

24<br />

<strong>temperament</strong><br />

1.4.3 <strong>Child</strong> <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>parental</strong> situational factors: the case of<br />

25<br />

<strong>parental</strong> stress<br />

1.4.3.1 Implications of child <strong>temperament</strong> for <strong>parental</strong> stress 28<br />

1.4.3.2 Implications of <strong>parental</strong> stress for child <strong>temperament</strong> 30<br />

1.5 Aims of the study 31<br />

1.5.1 Continuity of <strong>temperament</strong> from infancy to middle childhood 32<br />

1.5.1.1 Study I. Continuity of mother-rated <strong>temperament</strong> 32<br />

1.5.1.2 Study II. Continuity of father-rated <strong>temperament</strong><br />

1.5.2 Transactional development of parent <strong>and</strong> child characteristics<br />

34<br />

from infancy to middle childhood<br />

1.5.2.1 Study III. Parental <strong>personality</strong> traits of extraversion <strong>and</strong><br />

34<br />

neuroticism <strong>and</strong> infant/child <strong>temperament</strong><br />

1.5.2.2 Study IV. Mother’s overall stress <strong>and</strong> infant/child<br />

<strong>temperament</strong><br />

2 METHODS 37<br />

2.1 Outline of the study 37<br />

2.2 Participants 37<br />

2.3 Measures 38<br />

2.3.1 Infant <strong>temperament</strong> 38<br />

2.3.2 <strong>Child</strong> <strong>temperament</strong> 40<br />

2.3.3 Parental <strong>personality</strong> traits 41<br />

2.3.4 Perceived stress 41<br />

2.4 Statistical analyses 42<br />

3<br />

12<br />

27<br />

34<br />

35


3 RESULTS<br />

3.1 Study I. Continuity of mother-rated <strong>temperament</strong> from infancy to middle<br />

44<br />

childhood<br />

44<br />

3.1.1 Continuity of <strong>temperament</strong> subscales <strong>and</strong> superconstructs 44<br />

3.1.2 Person-level <strong>continuity</strong> of <strong>temperament</strong>: “<strong>temperament</strong> types”<br />

3.2 Study II. Continuity of father-rated <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> congruence<br />

47<br />

between mother- <strong>and</strong> father-rated <strong>continuity</strong><br />

3.2.1 Continuity of father-rated <strong>temperament</strong> subscales <strong>and</strong><br />

49<br />

superconstructs<br />

3.2.2 Congruence between mother- <strong>and</strong> father-rated <strong>continuity</strong> of<br />

49<br />

<strong>temperament</strong><br />

3.3 Study III. Transaction between <strong>parental</strong> <strong>personality</strong> traits <strong>and</strong> child<br />

52<br />

<strong>temperament</strong><br />

54<br />

3.4 Study IV. Transaction between maternal stress <strong>and</strong> child <strong>temperament</strong><br />

3.4.1 Transaction between maternal stress <strong>and</strong> individual<br />

57<br />

<strong>temperament</strong> subscales<br />

3.4.2 Transaction between maternal stress <strong>and</strong> latent <strong>temperament</strong><br />

57<br />

superconstructs<br />

58<br />

4 DISCUSSION<br />

4.1 Continuity of parent-rated <strong>temperament</strong> from infancy to middle<br />

59<br />

childhood<br />

59<br />

4.2 Developmental transaction between parent <strong>and</strong> child characteristics<br />

4.2.1 Transactional development of <strong>parental</strong> <strong>personality</strong> traits <strong>and</strong><br />

65<br />

child <strong>temperament</strong><br />

4.2.2 Transactional development of maternal stress <strong>and</strong> child<br />

65<br />

<strong>temperament</strong><br />

71<br />

4.3 General conclusions 73<br />

4.4 Weaknesses <strong>and</strong> strengths of the study 74<br />

4.5 Theoretical <strong>and</strong> clinical implications 76<br />

REFERENCES 81<br />

4


Abstract<br />

Studying the <strong>continuity</strong> <strong>and</strong> underlying mechanisms of <strong>temperament</strong> change from early<br />

childhood through adulthood is clinically <strong>and</strong> theoretically relevant. Knowledge of the<br />

<strong>continuity</strong> <strong>and</strong> change of <strong>temperament</strong> from infancy onwards, especially as perceived by<br />

both parents is, however, still scanty. Only in recent years have researchers become<br />

aware that <strong>personality</strong>, long considered as stable in adulthood, may also change.<br />

Further, studies that focus on the transactional change of child <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>parental</strong> <strong>personality</strong> also seem to be lacking, as are studies focusing on transactions<br />

between child <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> more transient <strong>parental</strong> characteristics, like <strong>parental</strong><br />

stress. Therefore, this longitudinal study examined the degree of <strong>continuity</strong> of<br />

<strong>temperament</strong> over five years from the infant’s age of six months to the child’s age of<br />

five <strong>and</strong> a half years, as perceived by both biological parents, <strong>and</strong> also investigated the<br />

bidirectional effects between child <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> parents’ <strong>personality</strong> traits <strong>and</strong><br />

overall stress experienced during that time.<br />

First, moderate to high levels of <strong>continuity</strong> of <strong>temperament</strong> from infancy to middle<br />

childhood were shown, depicting the developmental links between affectively positive<br />

<strong>and</strong> well-adjusted <strong>temperament</strong> characteristics, <strong>and</strong> between characteristics of early <strong>and</strong><br />

later negative affectivity. The <strong>continuity</strong> of <strong>temperament</strong> was quantitatively <strong>and</strong><br />

qualitatively similar in both parents’ ratings. The findings also demonstrate that infant<br />

<strong>and</strong> childhood <strong>temperament</strong> characteristics cluster to form <strong>temperament</strong> types that<br />

resemble <strong>personality</strong> types shown in child <strong>and</strong> adult <strong>personality</strong> studies.<br />

Second, the <strong>parental</strong> <strong>personality</strong> traits of extraversion <strong>and</strong> neuroticism were shown<br />

to be highly stable over five years, but evidence of change in relation to parents’ views<br />

of their child’s <strong>temperament</strong> was also shown: an infant’s higher positive affectivity<br />

predicted an increase in <strong>parental</strong> extraversion, while the infant’s higher activity level<br />

predicted a decrease in <strong>parental</strong> neuroticism over five years. Furthermore, initially<br />

higher <strong>parental</strong> extraversion predicted higher ratings of the child’s effortful control,<br />

while initially higher <strong>parental</strong> neuroticism predicted the child’s higher negative<br />

affectivity. In terms of changes in <strong>parental</strong> stress, the infant’s higher activity level<br />

predicted a decrease in maternal overall stress, while initially higher maternal stress<br />

predicted a higher level of child negative affectivity in middle childhood.<br />

5


Together, the results demonstrate that the mother- <strong>and</strong> father-rated <strong>temperament</strong> of<br />

the child shows <strong>continuity</strong> during the early years of life, but also support the view that<br />

the development of <strong>temperament</strong> is sensitive to important contextual factors such as<br />

<strong>parental</strong> <strong>personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> overall stress. While <strong>parental</strong> <strong>personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> experienced<br />

stress were shown to have an effect on the child’s developing <strong>temperament</strong>, the reverse<br />

was also true: the parents’ own <strong>personality</strong> traits <strong>and</strong> perceived stress seemed to be<br />

highly stable, but also susceptible to their experiences of their child’s <strong>temperament</strong>.<br />

6


Tiivistelmä<br />

Temperamentin ja persoonallisuuden pysyvyyden, sekä niiden muuttumiseen<br />

vaikuttavien mekanismien selvittäminen varhaisvuosien ja aikuisuuden aikana on sekä<br />

teoreettisesti että kliinisesti merkityksellistä. Tieto <strong>temperament</strong>in pysyvyydestä ja<br />

muuttumisesta vauvaiästä eteenpäin, erityisesti molempien vanhempien arvioimana on<br />

kuitenkin yhä vähäistä. Vasta viime vuosina tutkijat ovat tulleet tietoisiksi siitä, että<br />

myös aikuisiässä pysyvänä pidetty persoonallisuus saattaa muuttua. Vielä ei kuitenkaan<br />

ole tutkittu sitä miten lapsen <strong>temperament</strong>ti ja vanhemman persoonallisuus vaikuttavat<br />

toisiinsa ajan myötä. Vanhemman tilannekohtaisempien ominaisuuksien, kuten stressin<br />

määrän, ja lapsen <strong>temperament</strong>in vastavuoroisia vaikutuksia ei myöskään ole vielä<br />

riittävästi tutkittu. Tässä pitkittäistutkimuksessa tarkasteltiin lapsen <strong>temperament</strong>in<br />

pysyvyyttä kuuden kuukauden iästä viiden ja puolen vuoden ikään, lapsen molempien<br />

biologisten vanhempien arvioimana. Lisäksi tarkasteltiin kaksisuuntaista vaikutusta<br />

lapsen <strong>temperament</strong>in ja vanhemman persoonallisuuden ja stressitason välillä näiden<br />

viiden vuoden aikana.<br />

Tulokset osoittivat lapsen <strong>temperament</strong>tiominaisuuksilla olevan kohtalainen tai<br />

vahva pysyvyys vauvaiästä viiden ja puolen vuoden ikään. Kehityksellisiä yhteyksiä<br />

löydettiin sekä myönteiseen affektiivisuuteen ja sopeutuvuuteen liittyvien<br />

ominaisuuksien välillä, että negatiiviseen affektiivisuuteen liittyvien ominaisuuksien<br />

välillä. Temperamentin pysyvyys näyttäytyi samanlaisena molempien vanhempien<br />

arvioissa. Tutkimus osoitti myös miten <strong>temperament</strong>tipiirteet vauvaiästä lähtien<br />

yhdistyvät persoonallisuustutkimuksessa vakiintuneita lasten ja aikuisten<br />

persoonallisuustyyppejä vastaaviksi <strong>temperament</strong>tityypeiksi.<br />

Vanhempien ekstraversio- ja neurotisismi –persoonallisuuspiirteet olivat varsin<br />

pysyviä viiden vuoden aikana, mutta tutkimus osoitti myös piirteiden muuttumisen<br />

suhteessa vanhemman näkemykseen lapsen <strong>temperament</strong>ista: vauvan korkeammalle<br />

arvioitu positiivinen affektiivisuus ennusti vanhemman korkeampaa ekstraversiota, ja<br />

vauvan korkeampi aktiivisuustaso ennusti vanhemman alempaa neurotisismin tasoa<br />

viiden vuoden jälkeen. Samaan aikaan, vanhemman alun perin korkeampi ekstraversion<br />

taso ennusti lapsen <strong>temperament</strong>in korkeampaa tahdonalaista itsesäätelyä, kun taas alun<br />

alkaen korkeampi vanhemman neurotisismi ennusti lapsen korkeampaa negatiivista<br />

affektiivisuutta viiden vuoden jälkeen. Vanhemman näkemys vauvasta aktiivisempana<br />

7


ennusti myös äidin vähäisempää stressiä viiden vuoden jälkeen, ja äidin alun alkaen<br />

korkeampi stressitaso ennusti lapsen kasvavaa negatiivista affektiivisuutta viiden<br />

vuoden jälkeen.<br />

Kokonaisuudessaan tutkimus osoittaa lapsen <strong>temperament</strong>in pysyvyyden<br />

ensimmäisten viiden vuoden aikana, sekä äidin että isän arvioimana, mutta tukee myös<br />

käsitystä <strong>temperament</strong>in kehityksen alttiudesta tärkeille ympäristötekijöille, kuten<br />

vanhempien persoonallisuuspiirteille ja vanhemman stressin määrälle. Samalla kun<br />

vanhemman persoonallisuus ja koetun stressin määrä vaikuttivat lapsen <strong>temperament</strong>in<br />

kehitykseen, nämä vanhemman sinänsä varsin pysyviksi osoittautuneet ominaisuudet<br />

olivat myös alttiita lapsen <strong>temperament</strong>tiominaisuuksien vaikutukselle.<br />

8


Acknowledgements<br />

I would like to thank several people who have contributed to <strong>and</strong> supported me in this<br />

endeavour.<br />

My deepest gratitude I owe to my supervisor Professor Katri Räikkönen-Talvitie for<br />

offering me the opportunity to undertake this task. Without her research expertise,<br />

dedication <strong>and</strong> clarity of thought, along with her exceptional ability to solve the most<br />

insoluble problems, this work would not have been possible.<br />

I wish to thank the reviewers of this thesis, Professor Maria A. Gartstein <strong>and</strong><br />

Professor Sam Putnam, for their valuable comments that helped me improve this work.<br />

I also thank my co-authors, Docent Anna-Liisa Järvenpää <strong>and</strong> Professor Timo E.<br />

Str<strong>and</strong>berg, for their supportive collaboration in the Glaku- project.<br />

I am deeply indebted to the whole Developmental Psychology research group, <strong>and</strong><br />

especially Docent Kati Heinonen-Tuomaala <strong>and</strong> Docent Anu-Katriina Pesonen, for their<br />

unforgettable helpfulness <strong>and</strong> never-ending support. Sharing the joys <strong>and</strong> difficulties of<br />

this work with them has been a privilege. I am also deeply grateful to Researcher Pertti<br />

Keskivaara for his enthusiasm for solving statistical problems with me.<br />

This work was carried out in the Department of Psychology at the University of<br />

Helsinki, <strong>and</strong> was made financially possible by the Finnish Graduate School of<br />

Psyhcology, the Academy of Finl<strong>and</strong>, the University of Helsinki <strong>and</strong> the Signe <strong>and</strong> Ane<br />

Gyllenberg, Jahnsson <strong>and</strong> Juho Vainio foundations, which I gratefully acknowledge.<br />

I offer my heartfelt thanks to my mother Irja <strong>and</strong> my father Mauri, who have always<br />

been there for me <strong>and</strong> have had faith in me in all my endeavours. And finally, I need to<br />

express my love <strong>and</strong> gratitude to my husb<strong>and</strong> Oki, who has been the best companion I<br />

could ever dream of. He is also the most caring <strong>and</strong> trustworthy father to our children<br />

Joel <strong>and</strong> Annina. Whatever I do, they are my greatest source of happiness <strong>and</strong><br />

inspiration, <strong>and</strong> to them I dedicate this work.<br />

Helsinki, May 2009<br />

Niina Komsi<br />

9


LIST OF ORIGINAL PUBLICATIONS<br />

I Komsi, N, Räikkönen, K., Pesonen, A-K., Heinonen, K., Keskivaara, P.,<br />

Järvenpää, A-L., & Str<strong>and</strong>berg, T.E. (2006). Continuity of <strong>temperament</strong> from<br />

infancy to middle childhood. Infant Behavior & Development, 29, 494-508.<br />

II Komsi, N., Räikkönen, K., Heinonen, K., Pesonen, A-K., Keskivaara, P.,<br />

Järvenpää, A-L., & Str<strong>and</strong>berg, T.E. (2008). Continuity of father-rated<br />

<strong>temperament</strong> from infancy to middle childhood. Infant Behavior & Development,<br />

31, 239-254.<br />

III Komsi, N., Räikkönen, K., Heinonen, K., Pesonen, A-K., Keskivaara, P.,<br />

Järvenpää, A-L., & Str<strong>and</strong>berg, T.E. (2008). Transactional development of parent<br />

<strong>personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> child <strong>temperament</strong>. European Journal of Personality, 22, 553–<br />

573.<br />

IV Pesonen, A-K., Räikkönen, K., Heinonen, K., Komsi, N., Järvenpää, A-L., &<br />

Str<strong>and</strong>berg, T.E. (2008). A transactional model of <strong>temperament</strong>al development:<br />

evidence of a relationship between child <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> maternal stress over<br />

five years. Social Development, 17, 326-340.<br />

10


1 INTRODUCTION<br />

1.1 Temperament dimensions <strong>and</strong> <strong>personality</strong> traits:<br />

theoretically, temporally <strong>and</strong> practically related constructs<br />

The main assumption in <strong>personality</strong> trait theory is that traits represent partly heritable,<br />

<strong>temperament</strong> -based individual predispositions, which show consistency across time <strong>and</strong><br />

situations (McCrae et al., 2000). In other words, the structure of adult <strong>personality</strong> is<br />

presumed to emerge from early <strong>temperament</strong> that serves as a substrate for <strong>personality</strong><br />

development (Costa & McCrae, 2000). In recent literature, the phenomena described by<br />

<strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>personality</strong> constructs have been concluded to be more alike than<br />

different (Caspi, Roberts, & Shiner, 2005; Mervielde, De Clercq, De Fruyt, & Van<br />

Leeuwen, 2005), <strong>and</strong> even in the <strong>temperament</strong> dimensions emerging from infancy<br />

research, similarities with the higher order factor structures extracted from adult studies<br />

of <strong>personality</strong> can be seen (Rothbart & Ahadi, 1994; Rothbart, Ahadi, & Evans, 2000a).<br />

Therefore, it is not surprising that research on <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>personality</strong> has<br />

increasingly concentrated on questions of development.<br />

Temperament in childhood has generally been described as the constellation of<br />

inborn traits that determines a child’s unique behavioral style <strong>and</strong> the way he or she<br />

experiences <strong>and</strong> reacts to the world (Goldsmith et al., 1987; Rothbart & Bates, 2006).<br />

There is general agreement between different <strong>temperament</strong> theorists on the biologically-<br />

based individual differences that show modest to moderate degrees of <strong>continuity</strong> during<br />

infancy <strong>and</strong> from early childhood onwards (Lemery, Goldsmith, Klinnert, & Mrazek,<br />

1999; Goldsmith et al., 1987; Rothbart & Bates, 2006): <strong>temperament</strong> is thus thought to<br />

describe those aspects of behavioral response that are not due to interactions with<br />

caregivers or other environmental influences, but rather those aspects that are present<br />

from birth <strong>and</strong> biologically based. Further, although <strong>temperament</strong> is traditionally<br />

viewed as consisting of these biologically-based behavioral traits, its manifestation in<br />

behavioral patterns is of interest: it is such patterns that influence children’s interactions<br />

in the world, <strong>and</strong> to which parents, teachers, <strong>and</strong> peers respond (Calkins, Bl<strong>and</strong>on,<br />

Williford, & Keane, 2007).<br />

In developmental research, the study of the underlying mechanisms that account for<br />

stability <strong>and</strong>/or change in <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>personality</strong> from the early years onwards<br />

<strong>and</strong> during adulthood bears great theoretical as well as clinical relevance. From a<br />

11


theoretical point of view, research on temporal stability <strong>and</strong> change <strong>and</strong> its correlates<br />

can shed light on the relative influence of maturational <strong>and</strong> environmental factors on<br />

<strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>personality</strong> development. In order to be able to examine the dynamics<br />

<strong>and</strong> correlates of developmental stability <strong>and</strong> change, the researchers are, however, first<br />

challenged to detect <strong>and</strong> establish the degree of <strong>continuity</strong> <strong>and</strong> developmental courses of<br />

the manifestations of <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>personality</strong> from the very early stages onwards<br />

(Caspi & Roberts, 1999; Goldsmith, 1996; Rothbart, 1989; Rothbart & Bates, 2006).<br />

From a practical or clinical point of view, a better underst<strong>and</strong>ing of temporal stability<br />

<strong>and</strong> change in <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>personality</strong> characteristics can inform therapeutic<br />

approaches to <strong>personality</strong>-related psychological problems <strong>and</strong> help to develop<br />

interventions tailored to the needs of individual clients (Widiger, Costa, & McCrae,<br />

2002). Given the importance of such questions, it is no wonder that recent years have<br />

seen a surge in meta-analyses examining longitudinal stability <strong>and</strong> change in <strong>personality</strong><br />

traits during adulthood (see Ardelt, 2000; Roberts & DelVecchio, 2000; Roberts,<br />

Walton, & Viechtbauer, 2006), along with increasing evidence of <strong>temperament</strong>- <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>personality</strong>-related antecedents <strong>and</strong> correlates of health <strong>and</strong> adjustment at all age stages<br />

(Nigg, 2006; Hampson, 2008; Roberts, Kuncel, Shiner, Caspi, & Goldberg, 2007;<br />

Shipley, Weiss, Der, Taylor, & Deary, 2007). However, generalizations from previous<br />

studies on <strong>personality</strong> development are still limited. Our knowledge of the development<br />

of early <strong>temperament</strong> in itself is also still scanty, as is our knowledge of individual<br />

differences in change <strong>and</strong> its predictors in adult <strong>personality</strong>.<br />

1.2 Temperament in childhood within the developmental<br />

framework of Mary K. Rothbart<br />

According to Mary K. Rothbart <strong>and</strong> her colleagues, <strong>temperament</strong> refers to<br />

constitutionally-based individual differences in reactivity <strong>and</strong> self-regulation (Rothbart<br />

& Bates, 2006). Constitutional in this sense (Rothbart & Derryberry, 1981) refers to the<br />

biological basis of <strong>temperament</strong>, which becomes influenced over time by heredity,<br />

maturation, <strong>and</strong> experience. The general factor of reactivity is seen as being directly<br />

related to the responsivity of the nervous system to sensory stimulation, referring to the<br />

excitability, responsivity or arousability of the physiological <strong>and</strong> behavioral systems.<br />

12


Self-regulation, in turn, refers to the neural <strong>and</strong> behavioral processes functioning to<br />

modulate this reactivity (Rothbart & Derryberry, 1981).<br />

In Rothbart’s conceptualization, <strong>temperament</strong> in infancy refers to early differences in<br />

motor activity, smile-proneness, soothability, attention span, anger-proneness <strong>and</strong><br />

fearful distress, operationalized either in a laboratory observation (Goldsmith &<br />

Rothbart, 1991, 1996), or in parent reports (Rothbart, 1981). The subscales further form<br />

two latent superconstructs, labeled positive <strong>and</strong> negative affectivity. Not all of these<br />

infant <strong>temperament</strong> characteristics are present at birth (smile, fear), or are not<br />

sufficiently matured to function as valid predictors of later <strong>temperament</strong>al development.<br />

Therefore, <strong>temperament</strong>al <strong>continuity</strong> measured either through distinct behavioral<br />

characteristics or through positive/negative affectivity superconstructs, is assumed to<br />

become apparent only after the early months of infancy.<br />

In childhood, the central concepts of <strong>temperament</strong>, such as reactivity, arousability,<br />

<strong>and</strong> self regulation remain, but it is the individual differences in the ability to utilize<br />

inhibitory <strong>and</strong> attentional mechanisms in the service of regulation that begin to<br />

characterize childhood <strong>temperament</strong> (Rothbart & Derryberry, 1981). By the third year<br />

of life, initial reactivity as the dominant developmental theme is replaced by behavior<br />

regulation (Rothbart, Derryberry, & Posner, 1994). That is, as the child develops, initial<br />

reactivity <strong>and</strong> early efforts to regulate the reactivity will be supplemented by an<br />

increasing capacity for voluntary forms of control (Rothbart & Bates, 2006).<br />

<strong>Child</strong>hood <strong>temperament</strong> can be depicted by the individual domains of activity level,<br />

anger-proneness, positive anticipation, attentional focusing, discomfort related to<br />

sensory qualities of stimulation, soothability, fear, the amount of pleasure related to<br />

high or low stimulus intensity, impulsivity, inhibitory control, low intensity pleasure,<br />

perceptual sensitivity, sadness, shyness, <strong>and</strong> the amount of smiling <strong>and</strong> laughter<br />

(Rothbart, Ahadi, Hershey, & Fisher, 2001). These domains can be further combined<br />

into the distinct superconstructs of extraversion, effortful control <strong>and</strong> negative<br />

affectivity (Rothbart et al., 2001).<br />

1.2.1 Continuity of <strong>temperament</strong> from infancy onwards<br />

The <strong>continuity</strong> of <strong>temperament</strong>al characteristics over time <strong>and</strong> in different contexts is,<br />

by definition, among the hallmarks of Rothbart’s theory, <strong>and</strong> the early appearing<br />

13


<strong>temperament</strong>al differences are assumed to provide a foundation for later emotional <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>personality</strong> development (Derryberry & Rothbart, 1997). Within this framework, the<br />

developmental dis<strong>continuity</strong> or lawful change (Caspi & Roberts, 1999; Kagan, 1971;<br />

Rothbart & Bates, 2006) in early <strong>temperament</strong> can also be theoretically defined <strong>and</strong><br />

understood. As a developing entity with increasing <strong>and</strong> differentiating manifestations<br />

from infancy onwards (Rothbart & Bates, 2006), <strong>temperament</strong> can thus be defined in<br />

terms of both homotypic <strong>and</strong> heterotypic <strong>continuity</strong> over time. Further, also dynamic<br />

relations between genetically unrelated traits are acknowledged, i.e. the development of<br />

one trait can implicate changes in the development of another, genetically unrelated trait<br />

(Putnam et al., 2008).<br />

By the concept homotypic <strong>continuity</strong>, the literal <strong>continuity</strong>, or stability over time of a<br />

<strong>temperament</strong> characteristic is indicated, whereas heterotypic <strong>continuity</strong> refers to the<br />

<strong>continuity</strong> between phenotypically different, but genotypically related attributes of<br />

<strong>temperament</strong> (see Caspi & Roberts, 1999). To be interpreted as heterotypic <strong>continuity</strong>,<br />

or coherence, as it has also been called, <strong>temperament</strong>al <strong>continuity</strong> has to be theoretically<br />

defined a priori: the models of <strong>continuity</strong> require theoretically derived predictions<br />

relating certain types of infant <strong>temperament</strong> to specific behaviors in childhood; these<br />

predictions ought to be based upon theories of development that allow one to predict<br />

age-appropriate changes in the phenotypic expression of a common genotype (Caspi &<br />

Roberts, 1999). For example, one might predict that the same pattern of fear responses<br />

to novelty observed in infancy would be found in later childhood, depicting homotypic<br />

<strong>continuity</strong>, while heterotypic <strong>continuity</strong> would refer to relations between earlier <strong>and</strong><br />

later fearful behavior that are based upon age-appropriate <strong>and</strong> logical associations rather<br />

than similarities in exact behaviors. That is, although infant negative affectivity can be<br />

composed of fear <strong>and</strong> anger, the manifestations of negative affectivity in later childhood<br />

are proposed to encompass more behavioral characteristics, such as proneness to<br />

discomfort <strong>and</strong> expressions of sadness.<br />

As the measures for infant <strong>and</strong> childhood <strong>temperament</strong> conceptualize <strong>temperament</strong><br />

similarly, but differ in the number of dimensions, the structural <strong>continuity</strong> of<br />

<strong>temperament</strong> in this work is also not explored as defined by Caspi, i.e., the persistence<br />

of correlational patterns among a set of variables across time (Caspi & Roberts, 1999),<br />

but will rather describe associations across time between <strong>temperament</strong> constructs that<br />

14


are based on individual dimensions of <strong>temperament</strong> in infancy <strong>and</strong> on individual<br />

dimensions of <strong>temperament</strong> in childhood.<br />

Furthermore, these descriptions of variable-level <strong>continuity</strong> of <strong>temperament</strong> do not,<br />

however, mirror <strong>continuity</strong> at the person (within-subject) level. While the research<br />

focusing on <strong>temperament</strong>-variables assesses their interpersonal correlational structure,<br />

utilizing the above-mentioned concepts, the person-centered approach examines typical<br />

within-person configurations (Asendorpf & van Aken, 1999; Bergman & Magnusson,<br />

2001; see also Robins & Tracy, 2003), <strong>and</strong> suggests yet another type of <strong>continuity</strong>, i.e.,<br />

ipsative <strong>continuity</strong> (Caspi & Roberts, 1999). Essentially, ipsative <strong>continuity</strong> is<br />

equivalent to structural <strong>continuity</strong> except it is considered at the level of the individual<br />

rather than a group or population. The person-centered approach thus depicts the<br />

development of <strong>temperament</strong> by individual <strong>temperament</strong> types or profiles, where each<br />

<strong>temperament</strong> dimension derives its meaning from its relations to the other dimensions<br />

within the individual.<br />

All in all, given the fact that Rothbart’s theory is among the most prominent current<br />

theories of <strong>temperament</strong>, <strong>and</strong> that research activity emphasizing this particular approach<br />

is intensifying, studies focusing on the <strong>continuity</strong> of <strong>temperament</strong> within Rothbart’s<br />

psychobiological framework are still surprisingly scarce. Rothbart’s conceptual <strong>and</strong><br />

operational definitions have been used in studying <strong>continuity</strong> of <strong>temperament</strong> within<br />

developmental phases, i.e., during infancy (Carnicero, Pérez-López, Salinas, &<br />

Martinéz-Fuentes, 2000; Denham, Lehman, Moser, & Reeves, 1995; Gartstein et al.,<br />

2006; Lemery et al., 1999; Rothbart, 1981, 1986; Stifter, Willoughby, & Towe-<br />

Goodman, 2008), toddlerhood (Putnam, Gartstein, & Rothbart, 2006) <strong>and</strong> childhood<br />

(Kochanska & Knaack, 2003; Majd<strong>and</strong>zic & van den Boom, 2008; Rothbart et al.,<br />

2001), but to my knowledge, only three studies have extended the scope across different<br />

developmental phases from infancy to childhood (Aksan et al., 1999; Putnam, Rothbart,<br />

& Gartstein, 2008; Rothbart, Derryberry, & Hershey, 2000b).<br />

In the study by Rothbart <strong>and</strong> her colleagues (2000b), theoretically relevant <strong>continuity</strong><br />

of <strong>temperament</strong> from infancy to the child’s age of seven was shown in a sample of 26<br />

children; however, due to the small sample size, the researchers highlighted the tentative<br />

nature of the results (Rothbart et al., 2000b). Recently, Putnam <strong>and</strong> his colleagues<br />

(2008) reported significant homotypic <strong>and</strong> heterotypic <strong>continuity</strong> for mother-rated<br />

15


characteristics of positive <strong>and</strong> negative affectivity from infancy to toddler <strong>and</strong> preschool<br />

age.<br />

Further, following the ipsative, person-level approach, <strong>temperament</strong> types in<br />

childhood have previously been identified by Aksan <strong>and</strong> his colleagues (1999). They<br />

placed 15% of 488 children in “expressive-noncontrolled” <strong>and</strong> “nonexpressive-<br />

controlled” categories based on maternal ratings of the 31/2 <strong>and</strong> 41/2 year-old child’s<br />

<strong>temperament</strong>, <strong>and</strong> considered whether these childhood types were different with respect<br />

to infant <strong>temperament</strong>: relative to the “controlled-nonexpressive” children, the<br />

“noncontrolled-expressive” children were found to have exhibited higher levels of<br />

activity, distress to limitations, <strong>and</strong> fear in their infancy. However, the study did not<br />

identify whether <strong>temperament</strong> characteristics in infancy <strong>and</strong> in middle childhood<br />

together clustered to form profiles indicative of different <strong>temperament</strong> types (Aksan et<br />

al., 1999).<br />

Moreover, studies also exist where infant <strong>temperament</strong> has been used as a predictor<br />

of <strong>temperament</strong> measured in toddlerhood, by using a measure that closely resembles<br />

Rothbart’s conceptual definition (Kochanska, Murray, & Harlan, 2000; Lemery et al.,<br />

1999). Clearly, more longitudinal research is needed to explore the developmental paths<br />

of early <strong>temperament</strong> across different developmental phases within Rothbart’s<br />

theoretical framework, utilizing the conceptual <strong>and</strong> operational definitions <strong>and</strong> measures<br />

developed within this framework.<br />

1.2.2 Mothers <strong>and</strong> fathers as informants of the child’s <strong>temperament</strong><br />

In <strong>temperament</strong> research, the question of objectivity in measuring child behavior<br />

according to <strong>parental</strong> reports is open to debate (Kagan, 1994, 1998; Seifer, Sameroff,<br />

Dickstein, Schiller, & Hayden, 2004; Stifter et al., 2008). However, parent-rated<br />

<strong>temperament</strong> of the child has frequently shown higher age-to-age stability than<br />

observer-rated <strong>temperament</strong> (Pauli-Pott, Mertesacker, Bade, Haverkock, & Beckmann,<br />

2003; Rothbart et al., 2000b). Besides this greater predictive validity, recent evidence<br />

also points to greater situational consistency of parent-ratings in comparison to more<br />

objective perceptions (Majd<strong>and</strong>zic & van den Boom, 2007). From the developmental<br />

point of view, it is important to further note that, within a family, especially the<br />

16


perceptions or views that parents have of their child’s characteristics are considered<br />

essential components of development (Belsky, 1984).<br />

Despite the encouragement to use parent-reports, the majority of the research on<br />

<strong>temperament</strong>al <strong>continuity</strong> to date in Rothbart’s, as in other frameworks, still relies on<br />

maternal ratings of the child’s <strong>temperament</strong> (e.g., Aksan et al., 1999; Putnam et al.,<br />

2008; Rothbart et al., 2000b; for another framework, see also Pedlow, Sanson, Prior, &<br />

Oberklaid, 1993), or on combinations of mother-rated <strong>and</strong> observed <strong>temperament</strong><br />

(Crockenberg & Leerkes, 2006; for other frameworks, see Belsky, Fish, & Isabella,<br />

1991; Fox, Henderson, Rubin, Calkins, & Schmidt, 2001; Stifter et al., 2008). The role<br />

of fathers in the family dynamic is, however, already widely acknowledged, <strong>and</strong><br />

research on the quality of the early father–child dyad <strong>and</strong> its relation to the behavioral<br />

characteristics <strong>and</strong> developmental outcomes of the child at all ages from infancy to early<br />

adulthood is becoming more widespread (e.g., Amato, 1994; Amato & Gilbreth, 1999;<br />

Belsky, Woodworth, Crnic, 1996; Parke, 2004; Ramch<strong>and</strong>ani, Stein, & Evans, 2005;<br />

Shannon, Tamis-LeMonda, & Cabrera, 2006).<br />

Furthermore, there is mounting evidence of the predictive association between infant<br />

<strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> the dynamics of father–child interaction (Belsky, 1996; Kochanska,<br />

Friesenborg, Lange, & Martel, 2004; McBride, Shoppe, & Rane, 2002; Sirignano &<br />

Lachman, 1985). Fathers <strong>and</strong> mothers are also increasingly considered comparable<br />

informants about their children’s behavior (DeFruyt &Vollrath, 2003). Mother- <strong>and</strong><br />

father-rated <strong>temperament</strong> have, according to previous cross-sectional data, been shown<br />

to be interrelated, <strong>and</strong> show a similar structural pattern in infancy, toddlerhood <strong>and</strong><br />

childhood (Auerbach et al., 2008; Goldsmith & Campos, 1990; Kochanska et al., 1998;<br />

Leve, Scaramella, & Fagot, 2001; Majd<strong>and</strong>zic & van den Boom, 2008; Putnam et al.,<br />

2006; Rothbart, 1981; Rothbart et al., 2001; Saudino, Wertz, & Gagne, 2004).<br />

Nevertheless, evidence of the extent to which father-rated <strong>temperament</strong> shows<br />

<strong>continuity</strong> during development is scanty, <strong>and</strong> to our knowledge, only a h<strong>and</strong>ful of<br />

studies to date have focused on this issue. In other <strong>temperament</strong> frameworks, Van<br />

Egeren (2004) found consistency of father-rated characteristics of negative affectivity<br />

during the first six months, <strong>and</strong> Rubin, Nelson, Hastings, <strong>and</strong> Asendorpf (1999)<br />

reported on moderate <strong>continuity</strong> of father-rated shyness from the age of two to four<br />

17


years. Bishop, Spence, <strong>and</strong> McDonald (2003) reported significant <strong>continuity</strong> of father-<br />

rated behavioral inhibition from the age of three to five years.<br />

In the framework of Rothbart, the recent study of Putnam <strong>and</strong> his colleagues (2006)<br />

demonstrated that stability estimates for fathers ratings of <strong>temperament</strong> ranged from .26<br />

to .73 from 18 to 36 months of age, <strong>and</strong> Rothbart <strong>and</strong> her colleagues (2001) present<br />

stability estimates for father-ratings of <strong>temperament</strong> that ranged from .48 to .76 from<br />

five to seven years of age, for subscales encompassing characteristics of positive <strong>and</strong><br />

negative affectivity <strong>and</strong> effortful control. In Majd<strong>and</strong>zic’s <strong>and</strong> van den Boom’s study<br />

(2008), mother- <strong>and</strong> father-rated <strong>temperament</strong> showed similar stabilities over seven<br />

months during the child’s fourth year of age. To my knowledge, no previous study has<br />

focused on the developmental <strong>continuity</strong> of father-rated <strong>temperament</strong> over<br />

developmental phases from infancy onwards, or on comparing the mother- <strong>and</strong> father-<br />

rated developmental paths of early <strong>temperament</strong>.<br />

1.3 Personality traits in adulthood<br />

1.3.1 Stability <strong>and</strong> change in <strong>personality</strong> traits<br />

For several decades, <strong>personality</strong> research has also increasingly concentrated on<br />

questions of development. Because the descriptions about <strong>personality</strong> remain the same<br />

across time, the dimensions of adult <strong>personality</strong> are defined to be homotypically<br />

continuous (homotypic referring to literal <strong>continuity</strong> or stability over time) (Caspi &<br />

Roberts, 1999). Recent theoretical discussions have, however, focused on the relative<br />

contributions of biological as compared to environmental factors on the homotypic<br />

<strong>continuity</strong>, or temporal stability of <strong>personality</strong> (e.g., Ardelt, 2000; Johnson, McGue, &<br />

Krueger, 2005; Roberts, Walton, & Viechtbauer, 2006b); in the theoretical framework<br />

developed by McCrae <strong>and</strong> his colleagues (2000), the association between <strong>personality</strong><br />

traits <strong>and</strong> environment is expressed in the person’s characteristic adaptations, while no<br />

external influence on the basic <strong>temperament</strong>-based tendencies is acknowledged. A more<br />

differentiated perspective has also emerged, suggesting that some environment-related<br />

change in <strong>personality</strong> traits is possible even during adulthood (e.g., Caspi & Roberts,<br />

1999; Helson et al., 2002; Roberts & Mroczek, 2008; Scollon & Diener, 2006).<br />

Of the different concepts depicting <strong>personality</strong> change, normative change is<br />

interpreted as the result of intrinsic trends based on rates of species-typical brain<br />

18


maturation <strong>and</strong> age-related changes in brain function <strong>and</strong> gene expression (e.g., McCrae<br />

et al., 2000), or a consequence of universal patterns of life experiences <strong>and</strong> social roles<br />

(Elder, 1994). Non-normative or rank-order change, in turn, is viewed as the result of<br />

individual differences in genetics, injury, drug abuse, or disease processes that influence<br />

brain chemistry on an individual level (e.g., Piedmont, 2001), or as the result of<br />

individual life experiences such as career trajectories (Roberts, 1997) or relationship<br />

patterns (Robins, Caspi, & Moffitt, 2002). In other words, while traits may change in<br />

the same direction for most people (normative change in the average level on any<br />

dimension), they may also change, in magnitude or in direction, the extent of change<br />

owing more to individual <strong>and</strong> specific environmental determinants (non-normative<br />

change) (Costa & McCrae, 1988; Roberts, Walton, Bogg, & Caspi 2006; see also<br />

Tennen, Affleck, & Armeli, 2005). Similar to the variable-level homo- <strong>and</strong> heterotypic<br />

<strong>continuity</strong> of early <strong>temperament</strong>, this differential, rank-order stability is often indexed<br />

by cross-time correlation coefficients (stability coefficients) which, for the major<br />

<strong>personality</strong> trait domains, have generally been shown to be substantial in magnitude<br />

(Fraley & Roberts, 2005; Costa & McCrae, 1994). This rank-order stability also tends to<br />

decline in magnitude as the elapsed time interval increases, <strong>and</strong> to increase<br />

systematically with age (Caspi et al., 2005; Clark & Watson, 1999; see also Fraley &<br />

Roberts, 2005).<br />

With regard to demographic predictors, most researchers also agree that rank-order<br />

stability (or test–retest stability/consistency) is considerably lower among adolescents<br />

<strong>and</strong> young adults than among older age groups (Ardelt, 2000; Roberts & DelVecchio,<br />

2000). However, there is some disagreement regarding the age(s) or lifestages when the<br />

age-related increase in the magnitude of the test–retest stability coefficients peaks. Until<br />

recently, the dominant perspective has been that once adulthood is reached, which<br />

happens around 30 years of age, there is no subsequent change in <strong>personality</strong> (Costa &<br />

McCrae, 1994, 1997; Terracciano, Costa, & McCrae, 2006). Other studies have reported<br />

that stability continues to increase from early adulthood to age 50 (Roberts &<br />

DelVecchio, 2000), while some have reported curvilinear trajectories with increases<br />

until age 50, but declines in old age (Ardelt, 2000). The patterns of rank-order stability<br />

for men <strong>and</strong> women have generally been found to be similar (Roberts & DelVecchio,<br />

2000).<br />

19


1.3.2 Personality traits of extraversion <strong>and</strong> neuroticism during adulthood<br />

The current study focuses on the two well-known <strong>personality</strong> traits, extraversion <strong>and</strong><br />

neuroticism (Costa & McCrae, 1992), which are included in virtually every prominent<br />

trait model developed during the 20th century (e.g., Clark & Watson, 1999). These two<br />

traits also have similar meanings in most models of <strong>personality</strong>, referring to positive <strong>and</strong><br />

negative emotionality <strong>and</strong> their regulation. Heritability estimates of extraversion <strong>and</strong><br />

neuroticism based on twin studies typically fall in the .40 to .60 range, with a median<br />

value of approximately .50 (e.g., Bouchard & Loehlin, 2001; Jang, McCrae, Angleitner,<br />

Riemann, & Livesley, 1998).<br />

As the underst<strong>and</strong>ing of these traits has increased, it has become clear that they<br />

represent basic dimensions of <strong>temperament</strong> (Clark & Watson, 1999): two key features<br />

of <strong>temperament</strong> are that they, first, are at least partly attributable to innate biological<br />

factors <strong>and</strong> second, have emotional processes as core, defining features (Digman, 1994).<br />

Accordingly, extraversion <strong>and</strong> neuroticism have strong <strong>and</strong> systematic links to<br />

emotional experience, <strong>and</strong> are associated with the basic PA <strong>and</strong> NA emotions (e.g., Abe<br />

& Izard, 1999; Carver, Sutton, & Scheier, 2000; Gross, Sutton, Ketelaar, 1998, Larsen<br />

& Ketelaar, 1991). Also in the framework proposed by Gray (1982, 1987), links<br />

between neuroticism <strong>and</strong> behavioral inhibition (BIS), <strong>and</strong> between extraversion <strong>and</strong><br />

behavioral activation (BAS), have been reported (Jorm et al., 1999; Smits & Boeck,<br />

2006). Extraversion <strong>and</strong> neuroticism are also considered most predictive of momentary<br />

affect (Yik & Rusell, 2001) <strong>and</strong> the affective quality of one’s perception of reality<br />

(Uziel, 2006) <strong>and</strong> self-esteem (Watson, Suls, & Haig, 2002).<br />

In the adult’s <strong>personality</strong>, the level of extraversion describes the quantity <strong>and</strong><br />

intensity of interpersonal interaction, activity level, need for stimulation, <strong>and</strong> capacity<br />

for joy: a person scoring high on extraversion is considered sociable, active, talkative,<br />

person-oriented, optimistic, fun-loving, <strong>and</strong> affectionate, whereas a low-scoring person<br />

is reserved, sober, unexuberant, aloof, task-oriented, retiring, <strong>and</strong> quiet (McCrae &<br />

John, 1992). Neuroticism, in turn, assesses adjustment versus emotional instability <strong>and</strong><br />

identifies individuals prone to psychological distress, unrealistic ideas, excessive<br />

cravings or urges, <strong>and</strong> maladaptive coping responses: a person scoring high on this trait<br />

worries a lot <strong>and</strong> is nervous, emotional, insecure, <strong>and</strong> feels inadequate, whereas a person<br />

20


scoring low is calm, relaxed, unemotional, hardy, secure <strong>and</strong> self-satisfied (McCrae &<br />

John, 1992).<br />

The reports of rank-order stabilities of extraversion <strong>and</strong> neuroticism during<br />

adulthood for both genders have generally been relatively high, ranging from .61 to .80<br />

between periods from two years to several decades (e.g., Costa & McCrae, 1988;<br />

Scollon & Diener, 2006; Vaidya, Gray, & Haig, 2002; Van Aken, Denissen, Branje,<br />

Dubas, & Goossens, 2006). However, existing evidence shows that rank-order change<br />

in these two <strong>temperament</strong>-based traits during adulthood also occurs, which obviously<br />

has raised the compelling question of why these changes occur.<br />

1.4 Transactional development of <strong>parental</strong> <strong>personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> child<br />

<strong>temperament</strong><br />

There are many potential processes that can either support or discourage developmental<br />

<strong>continuity</strong> in child <strong>and</strong> adult <strong>temperament</strong>- <strong>and</strong> <strong>personality</strong>-related characteristics. These<br />

processes have been described as involving transactions between a person <strong>and</strong> the<br />

environment. During development, these transactions may lead some individuals to<br />

experience changes not shared with others in their cohort, the extent of change owing<br />

more to individual <strong>and</strong> specific, rather than shared environmental determinants.<br />

Caspi (1998) describes three transactional processes that will influence the degree of<br />

<strong>continuity</strong> of behavior over time: reactive, evocative, <strong>and</strong> proactive transactions. Within<br />

a family, these person-environment transactions may lead to discontinuities as well as<br />

continuities in <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>personality</strong>. Reactive transactions emphasize individual<br />

characteristics that impact a person’s own experience of identical environmental<br />

conditions. Applied to models of early <strong>temperament</strong>, <strong>temperament</strong>al traits will affect the<br />

ways in which environmental information is processed <strong>and</strong> thereby interpreted. These<br />

<strong>temperament</strong>ally based biases in responding to the caregiving environment may be a<br />

powerful force in maintaining <strong>and</strong> promoting early individual differences <strong>and</strong> their<br />

stability <strong>and</strong>/or change over time. Therefore, in order to underst<strong>and</strong> the origins of<br />

various <strong>temperament</strong>al outcomes, it is essential to pay attention to the period of infancy<br />

<strong>and</strong> the appearance of <strong>temperament</strong>al characteristics during that time. In adulthood,<br />

<strong>temperament</strong>-based <strong>personality</strong> characteristics that impact a person’s own experience of<br />

21


environmental conditions may also be suggested to have an effect on the way the adult<br />

reacts to, for example, his/her child’s <strong>temperament</strong>.<br />

Evocative transactions, in turn, refer to the manner in which unique individual<br />

differences evoke particular responses from the environment. Those responses further<br />

influence the manner in which the individual will respond to the environment,<br />

establishing a self-perpetuating cycle of interaction between the individual <strong>and</strong> the<br />

environment. This cycle may maintain <strong>continuity</strong>, but may also be suggested to promote<br />

change in <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>personality</strong>. For example, infants of different <strong>temperament</strong>s<br />

may evoke different caregiving behaviors from the same parent. Or, an infant’s<br />

<strong>temperament</strong> may evoke distinctive responses from different parents. These differences<br />

in child <strong>and</strong> <strong>parental</strong> behaviors could serve to either maintain or diminish the expression<br />

of <strong>temperament</strong>al <strong>and</strong> <strong>personality</strong> differences over time. The final process, proactive<br />

transactions, refers to the manner in which individuals move beyond the environments<br />

into which they are born <strong>and</strong> actively select <strong>and</strong> construct environments of their own<br />

(Caspi, 1998). Within a family, the newborn infant’s environment is largely defined by<br />

the parents, but as the child grows, both the child <strong>and</strong> the parents may be actively<br />

constructing their mutual environment. However, within a family context, one can also<br />

suggest that parents may try to construct an environment that is in harmony with the<br />

child’s characteristics, even if it is not what the parents’ own <strong>personality</strong> would lead<br />

them to choose.<br />

Stability <strong>and</strong> change of <strong>personality</strong> characteristics <strong>and</strong> relationship qualities can thus<br />

be considered both prerequisites <strong>and</strong> consequences of dynamic transactions between a<br />

person <strong>and</strong> his/her relationship experiences (Lehnart & Neyer, 2006). These dynamic<br />

transactions between individual characteristics <strong>and</strong> relationship quality are more likely<br />

to occur in stable social environments like the family, where the individual<br />

characteristics of parent <strong>and</strong> child represent the specific environmental determinants that<br />

may promote stability or rank-order change in both partners of the interaction (Belsky,<br />

1984; Caspi & Roberts, 2001; Neyer & Asendorpf, 2001). In the family, a parent <strong>and</strong> a<br />

child can be seen as active agents who, acting according to their individual<br />

characteristics, co-create their emerging <strong>and</strong> enduring relationship (Collins, Maccoby,<br />

Steinberg, Hetherington & Bornstein, 2000; Kochanska et al., 2004; Lengua & Kovacs,<br />

2005; Rubin et al., 1999; Wachs, 2006; van den Boom & Hoeksma, 1994).<br />

22


In the existing literature, the direct links between various behavioral, physiological,<br />

<strong>and</strong> genetic characteristics in parent <strong>and</strong> child have been reasonably well established<br />

(e.g., Crnic & Low, 2002; Propper & Moore, 2006). In terms of longitudinal causality,<br />

the parent’s effects on child development seem better known than the effects that<br />

children might have on their parents’ psychological characteristics. Also the<br />

bidirectional associations between parent <strong>and</strong> child characteristics over time still remain<br />

less studied. Transactional models in which individual characteristics of parent <strong>and</strong><br />

child have been shown to be mutually influential have, so far, mainly been reported in<br />

relation to clinically relevant outcomes like difficult <strong>temperament</strong>, psychosocial<br />

problems <strong>and</strong> compliance of the child, or adequacy of <strong>parental</strong> behavior (e.g., Ge et al.,<br />

1996, Lengua 2006; Patterson & Fisher, 2002).<br />

For instance, Rubin et al. (1999) reported that child shyness at the age of two<br />

predicted a decrease in maternal <strong>and</strong> paternal encouragement of independence over two<br />

years, whereas parenting behaviors towards 2-year-olds did not predict the 4-year-old<br />

child’s shyness (Rubin et al., 1999). In line with this kind of directionality, Belsky et al.<br />

(2000) reported that during the child’s third year, a child’s inhibited behavior was likely<br />

to increase <strong>parental</strong> reactivity in terms of both discouraging <strong>and</strong> encouraging behaviors,<br />

whereas <strong>parental</strong> reactions to the child’s inhibition were poor predictors of inhibited<br />

behavior. However, the existing literature of associations between <strong>parental</strong><br />

psychological characteristics (e.g., Belsky et al., 1991; Kochanska et al., 2004) <strong>and</strong><br />

subsequent child behavior indicate towards <strong>parental</strong> effects as well, <strong>and</strong> raise the<br />

question of possible bidirectional causality for these inhibition-related, as well as other<br />

aspects of child <strong>temperament</strong> from infancy onwards.<br />

Further, according to Belsky’s (1984) ecological model, the quality of parenting is<br />

especially influenced by three main classes of factors: parent, child <strong>and</strong> situational<br />

characteristics, like contextual stress <strong>and</strong> supports. Within a family system, <strong>parental</strong><br />

views of their own <strong>and</strong> the child’s characteristics, feelings <strong>and</strong> behavior can thereby be<br />

understood as psychological resources or contextual components that will determine the<br />

degree to which a parent <strong>and</strong> child will affect each other from the child’s early years<br />

onwards (Belsky, 1984). These perceived characteristics may also be differentially<br />

susceptible to each other’s influence. That is, parents’ own characteristics may lead to<br />

changes in their views of their children’s <strong>temperament</strong>-related behavior, or it may be<br />

23


that parents who view their children as initially higher in some but not other<br />

<strong>temperament</strong>al characteristics may be prone to such parenting experiences that will<br />

eventually lead to changes in their views of their own characteristics.<br />

In this study, the environment where the rank-order stability or change in <strong>parental</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> child characteristics is examined is conceptualized in terms of child <strong>temperament</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>parental</strong> <strong>personality</strong> traits <strong>and</strong>, as a more situational characteristic, the amount of<br />

overall <strong>parental</strong> stress. Even though the existing studies regarding associations between<br />

the child’s <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>parental</strong> <strong>personality</strong>-related characteristics are still<br />

relatively ambiguous in regard to causal influence, an attempt of categorizing the<br />

studies into those where child effects on the parent are indicated <strong>and</strong> those indicating<br />

parent effects on the child may be of help in forming a general picture of the current<br />

knowledge.<br />

1.4.1 Implications of child <strong>temperament</strong> for <strong>parental</strong> extraversion <strong>and</strong><br />

neuroticism<br />

So far, research on the rank-order stability of extraversion <strong>and</strong> neuroticism has already<br />

provided some evidence of possible environmental promoters of change: recent<br />

evidence shows that higher life- <strong>and</strong> work-related satisfaction associates with an<br />

increase in extraversion, while lower satisfaction, stress <strong>and</strong> unemployment relate to an<br />

increase in neuroticism (Roberts, Caspi, & Moffitt, 2003; Roberts & Chapman, 2000;<br />

Scollon & Diener, 2006; Van Aken et al., 2006). Neyer <strong>and</strong> Asendorpf (2001), in turn,<br />

have proposed that it is interpersonal relations, such as marital <strong>and</strong> family experiences,<br />

that have the highest potential to affect <strong>personality</strong> change in adulthood: they found that<br />

young adults who had recently begun dating showed greater declines in neuroticism <strong>and</strong><br />

shyness (a reverse facet of extraversion) <strong>and</strong> larger increases in extraversion compared<br />

to participants whose relationship status had not changed. Similarly, getting married <strong>and</strong><br />

living in a more satisfying relationship has been shown to be associated with an increase<br />

in emotional stability <strong>and</strong> a decrease in neuroticism, while marital tensions, lower<br />

marital satisfaction <strong>and</strong> divorce associate with an increase in neuroticism (Costa,<br />

Herbst, McCrae, & Siegler, 2000; Roberts & Chapman, 2000; Robins et al., 2002). Still,<br />

questions regarding potential promoters of change in <strong>personality</strong> during adulthood<br />

24


(Roberts et al., 2006b; Scollon & Diener, 2006; Terracciano, McCrae, Brant, & Costa,<br />

2005) have so far remained largely unresolved.<br />

Of the important stages <strong>and</strong> roles of adult life, parenting a young child can be<br />

considered a very significant <strong>and</strong> intimate interpersonal experience that has the potential<br />

to cause enduring effects on an individual. While the <strong>parental</strong> self-concept gradually<br />

changes during parenthood (Cowan, Cowan, Heming, & Miller, 1991), this identity<br />

process, involving the commitment to a new social role, is also likely to involve at least<br />

some change in <strong>parental</strong> <strong>personality</strong> over time (Roberts, Wood, & Smith, 2005).<br />

Therefore, through their unique impact on <strong>parental</strong> behavior <strong>and</strong> on the quality of the<br />

parent–child relationship (Calkins, Hungerford, & Dedmon, 2004; Collins et al., 2000;<br />

Rubin et al., 1999), <strong>temperament</strong>ally different children may, in the course of time, also<br />

have a unique impact on parents’ own <strong>personality</strong> traits.<br />

Evidence already exists that positively affective babies evoke more positive reactions<br />

from their caregivers than negatively affective babies (e.g., Kochanska et al., 2004;<br />

Scarr, 1992; Van den Boom, 1989). In terms of changes in maternal psychological well<br />

being, Gartstein <strong>and</strong> Sheeber (2004) reported that within a group of mothers having<br />

parenting difficulties with their 3 to 6-year-old children, child externalizing behavior<br />

was predictive of a decline in maternal self-perceived parenting competence: this, in<br />

turn, contributed to an increase in maternal depressive symptoms over one year. Further,<br />

associations between the child’s early <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> changes in parents’ sense of<br />

global efficacy, personal control, anxiety, <strong>and</strong> dispositional optimism over time have<br />

been shown to exist (Heinonen, Räikkönen, Scheier, Pesonen, Järvenpää, & Str<strong>and</strong>berg,<br />

2006; Sirignano & Lachman, 1985). Parenting experiences have also been shown to be<br />

associated with changes in maternal <strong>personality</strong>-related characteristics like ego<br />

resiliency, feelings of dependency <strong>and</strong> fearfulness (Paris & Helson, 2002). Still, the<br />

influence that the child may actually have on the parents’ <strong>personality</strong> traits, <strong>and</strong><br />

especially on the two ‘best-known’ traits extraversion <strong>and</strong> neuroticism, is less known.<br />

1.4.2 Implications of <strong>parental</strong> extraversion <strong>and</strong> neuroticism for child<br />

<strong>temperament</strong><br />

As Rothbart <strong>and</strong> Putnam (2002) suggested, a child’s <strong>temperament</strong> as it interacts with the<br />

environment is likely a better predictor of developmental outcomes than <strong>temperament</strong><br />

25


alone. Environmental pressures within the social context, especially at an early age,<br />

provide the structure around which <strong>temperament</strong>al dispositions will be modified. In<br />

evaluating the associations between <strong>parental</strong> <strong>personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> child <strong>temperament</strong>, it is<br />

thus essential to pay attention to the period of infancy <strong>and</strong> the early appearance of the<br />

child’s <strong>temperament</strong>al characteristics that will strongly affect the child’s concurrent <strong>and</strong><br />

subsequent behavior.<br />

The <strong>continuity</strong> estimates of childhood <strong>temperament</strong> reported so far (Putnam et al.,<br />

2008; Rothbart et al., 2000b) also leave room for contextual factors. In other words,<br />

instead of conceptualizing <strong>temperament</strong> as a set of highly stable traits inherent in the<br />

child, its <strong>continuity</strong>, especially in terms of self-regulative functions, can be seen as<br />

relating to the child’s social environment (Rothbart & Bates, 2006). Accordingly, the<br />

development of infant <strong>temperament</strong> has consistently been shown to be affected by,<br />

besides genetic influence, the unique environment of the child (e.g. Goldsmith, Lemery,<br />

Buss & Campos, 1999; Propper & Moore, 2006; Stright, Gallagher, & Kelley, 2008).<br />

Among the most significant contextual elements in a child’s environment are<br />

<strong>parental</strong> characteristics. Given the role <strong>temperament</strong> plays in later behavior <strong>and</strong><br />

adjustment problems, testing the effects of <strong>parental</strong> <strong>personality</strong> on their child’s<br />

<strong>temperament</strong> (e.g., Rothbart et al., 2000; Caspi, 2000), along with their influence in<br />

proximal interactions (Crockenberg & Smith, 2002; van den Boom & Hoeksma, 1994)<br />

<strong>and</strong> in various other processes in the child’s development (e.g., Kochanska, 1997) is of<br />

particular importance.<br />

In terms of <strong>parental</strong> extraversion <strong>and</strong> neuroticism, previous studies have revealed that<br />

parents with high extraversion <strong>and</strong> low neuroticism display more positive, supportive,<br />

<strong>and</strong> responsive parenting <strong>and</strong> less negative, controlling parenting (Belsky & Barends,<br />

2002; Belsky, Crnic, & Woodworth, 1995; Clark, Kochanska, & Ready, 2000; Losoya,<br />

Goldsmith, Callor, & Rowe, 1997; Metsäpelto & Pulkkinen, 2003). In contrast, parents’<br />

high neuroticism is often found to be associated with less sensitive, less affective <strong>and</strong><br />

less stimulating parenting (Belsky et al., 1995). Associations between higher <strong>parental</strong><br />

extraversion <strong>and</strong> more positive evaluations <strong>and</strong> more positive outcomes of the child, as<br />

well as between higher <strong>parental</strong> negative affectivity <strong>and</strong> neuroticism <strong>and</strong> more negative<br />

evaluations <strong>and</strong> outcomes of the child have also been hitherto quite consistently<br />

established (e.g., Belsky et al., 1991; Gartstein & Marmion, 2008; Kochanska et al.,<br />

26


2004; Kurdek, 2003; Propper & Moore, 2006). In general, no significant differences in<br />

the effects of parent-related determinants on maternal <strong>and</strong> paternal behavior have been<br />

detected (Metsäpelto & Pulkkinen, 2003; Verhoeven, Junger, Van Aken, Dekovic, &<br />

Van Aken, 2007).<br />

1.4.3 <strong>Child</strong> <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>parental</strong> situational factors: the case of<br />

<strong>parental</strong> stress<br />

Besides parents’ own <strong>personality</strong>- or <strong>temperament</strong>-related psychological characteristics,<br />

situational factors within the parents’ life may also have a significant effect on <strong>parental</strong><br />

feelings <strong>and</strong> behavior within a family, <strong>and</strong> especially within the parent’s relationship<br />

with the child. Despite the origin of those situational experiences, i.e., whether they<br />

originate from within or outside of the family, parents’ individual interpretations of <strong>and</strong><br />

feelings related to those experiences may further influence the parents’ <strong>and</strong> the child’s<br />

behavior at home.<br />

Because parents in modern societies are under various pressures, one of the most<br />

important <strong>and</strong> influential situational factors in an adult’s life may be the amount of<br />

perceived stress. Stress in the family context, especially when that stress is chronic <strong>and</strong><br />

present early in the child’s development, can further have detrimental effects on the<br />

wellbeing of the family <strong>and</strong> the quality of parent–child relationships. Accordingly, the<br />

existing literature shows associations between <strong>parental</strong> parenting-, daily-, life-event, <strong>and</strong><br />

other environment-related stress <strong>and</strong> parent- <strong>and</strong>/or teacher-reported child behavioral<br />

problems, adjustment difficulties, <strong>and</strong> internalizing or externalizing problems in the<br />

early childhood period (Anthony et al., 2005; Conger, McCarty, Yang, Lahey, & Kropp,<br />

1984; Coplan, Bowker, & Cooper, 2003; Creasey & Reese, 1996; Deater-Deckard &<br />

Scarr, 1996; Kliewer & Kung, 1998; Peterson & Hawley, 1998).<br />

However, the majority of research in this area has mainly focused on a specific<br />

concept, ‘parenting stress’, that is defined in terms of dysfunctional parent–child<br />

interaction, <strong>parental</strong> anxiety or depression, <strong>and</strong> ‘difficult’ or challenging child-behavior<br />

characteristics (Abidin, 1990; Crnic & Greenberg, 1990). This kind of parenting stress<br />

does not necessarily reflect the more global appraisal of stress that may result from<br />

various life domains other than parenting (e.g., work <strong>and</strong> marital discord). The existing<br />

evidence, however, implies that it is not only stress that is roused by parenting<br />

27


experiences that may have an influence on family functioning, <strong>parental</strong> behavior <strong>and</strong><br />

dyadic interaction between a parent <strong>and</strong> a child at home (Crnic, Gaze, & Hoffman,<br />

2005; Deater-Deckard & Scarr, 1996).<br />

The stability of <strong>parental</strong> stress is an assumption that seems to underlie much of the<br />

research within the field, although, due to the relative lack of longitudinal research, it<br />

has not been established to any reliable extent. As in studies of adult <strong>personality</strong>, studies<br />

of <strong>parental</strong> stress show greater stability for shorter time periods between assessments<br />

(Crnic et al., 2005). In the study of Mulsow <strong>and</strong> her colleagues (2002), maternal<br />

parenting stress was shown to be stable between the child’s ages of 15 <strong>and</strong> 36 months<br />

(Mulsow, Caldera, Pursley, Reifman, & Huston, 2002), <strong>and</strong> in the recent Rantanen et al.<br />

(2008) study, parenting stress of mothers <strong>and</strong> fathers with children living at home<br />

showed moderate to high stability, but was more stable across a one-year period than<br />

across a six-year period (Rantanen, Kinnunen, Feldt, & Pulkkinen, 2008). Further, in<br />

Crnic et al. 2005, two indices of maternal stress, namely experiences of major life<br />

events <strong>and</strong> parenting daily hassles, appeared to be stable over two years across the<br />

preschool period: stressed mothers at child age three were likely to report higher stress<br />

at child age five. However, in the study of Östberg, Hagekull <strong>and</strong> Hagelin (2007), the<br />

individual stability of maternal parenting stress over a six-year period in a clinical<br />

intervention group with infant sleeping <strong>and</strong> feeding problems was only moderate,<br />

decreasing slightly over time from infancy to the child’s age of six to seven.<br />

Similar to the development <strong>and</strong> over-time stability reported for the more<br />

constitutionally based characteristics of child <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>parental</strong> <strong>personality</strong><br />

traits, the stabilities found for the level of <strong>parental</strong> stress make it clear that there is<br />

ample room for change. The parents’ experience of overall stress can increase or ease<br />

off over time, <strong>and</strong> many parents do change; the intriguing question is whether these<br />

changes can be predicted from some internal family factors. The idea of being able to<br />

predict or define some typical correlates of change in parents’ experience of stress<br />

makes the efforts to identify such factors within a family an exciting area for inquiry.<br />

1.4.3.1 Implications of child <strong>temperament</strong> for <strong>parental</strong> stress<br />

In evaluating the associations between <strong>parental</strong> stress <strong>and</strong> child <strong>temperament</strong>, periods in<br />

infancy <strong>and</strong> early childhood are again considered to be of greatest importance.<br />

28


Concurrent associations between so called ‘difficult <strong>temperament</strong>’ <strong>and</strong> parenting stress<br />

have provided evidence in support of the notion that infant difficultness can undermine<br />

<strong>parental</strong> functioning (Belsky, 1984; Belsky, Rha, & Park, 2000; Calkins et al., 2004;<br />

Collins, Maccoby, Steinberg, Hetherington, & Bornstein, 2000; Gartstein & Sheeber,<br />

2004; Heinonen et al., 2006; Kochanska et al., 2004; Rubin et al., 1999; van den Boom<br />

& Hoeksma, 1994), although evidence concerning these effects is not altogether<br />

coherent (Putnam, Sanson & Rothbart, 2002; Rothbart, 1986). In terms of concurrent<br />

associations between early <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>parental</strong> stress, Gelf<strong>and</strong>, Teti <strong>and</strong> Fox<br />

(1992) reported a significant association between the mothers’ parenting stress <strong>and</strong><br />

difficult <strong>temperament</strong> among three- to 13-month-old infants. Mulsow <strong>and</strong> her<br />

colleagues (2002) also reported a significant association between maternal parenting<br />

stress <strong>and</strong> infant difficultness at one month, but not at six or 15 months, <strong>and</strong> Honjo <strong>and</strong><br />

his colleagues (1999) found maternal child-rearing stress to be related to infant-<br />

<strong>temperament</strong>al difficulty at 18 months, but not at six months. Associations between<br />

<strong>parental</strong> stress <strong>and</strong> child <strong>temperament</strong> have also been reported among 21-month-old<br />

toddlers (Östberg & Hagekull, 2000) <strong>and</strong> among 48-month-old children (Coplan et al.,<br />

2003).<br />

However, the possible over-time effects of the child’s initial <strong>temperament</strong>al<br />

characteristics on the parents’ stress experience remain largely unexplored. With regard<br />

to specific caregiving-related problems during infancy, namely problems with feeding<br />

<strong>and</strong> sleeping, it has been shown that parenting stress at the child’s age of six to seven<br />

can, at least partly, be predicted by these early child <strong>and</strong> family problems <strong>and</strong> associated<br />

early stress (Östberg et al., 2007). It can thus be assumed that, similar to the changes in<br />

<strong>parental</strong> self-concept <strong>and</strong> <strong>personality</strong> characteristics induced by the parenting role<br />

(Cowan et al., 1991; Roberts et al, 2005; Paris & Helson, 2002), the <strong>parental</strong> role may<br />

also be accompanied by changes in the level of <strong>parental</strong> experience of stress over time.<br />

The existing evidence further shows, that mothers <strong>and</strong> fathers are generally more<br />

similar than different in their specific levels of stress experience (Creasey & Reese,<br />

1996; Deater-Deckard & Scarr, 1996) <strong>and</strong> associated experiences (Deater-Deckard,<br />

Scarr, McCartney, & Eisenberg, 1994). For example, Crnic <strong>and</strong> Booth (1991) reported<br />

no differences in the absolute number or intensity of the everyday parenting hassles that<br />

mothers <strong>and</strong> fathers experienced. The stability of <strong>parental</strong> stress <strong>and</strong> the associations<br />

29


etween stress <strong>and</strong> child outcomes have also generally been shown to be independent of<br />

the parent’s or child’s gender (Mulsow et al., 2002; Rantanen et al., 2008; Räikkönen et<br />

al., 2006).<br />

1.4.3.2 Implications of <strong>parental</strong> stress for child <strong>temperament</strong><br />

Both <strong>parental</strong> overall stress <strong>and</strong> parenting-related stress have been proven to be<br />

significant predictors of parent <strong>and</strong> child behavior <strong>and</strong> dyadic interaction (e.g.,<br />

Bergman, Sarkar, Glover, & O'Connor, 2008; Conger et al., 2002; Crnic et al., 2005;<br />

Repetti & Wood, 1997), but so far only a few studies have investigated the associations<br />

between a more global experience of <strong>parental</strong> stress <strong>and</strong> child <strong>temperament</strong>. Especially<br />

in terms of early <strong>temperament</strong>, the causality of associations between <strong>parental</strong> stress <strong>and</strong><br />

infant/child <strong>temperament</strong> over time is, to a large degree, still an unexplored area.<br />

Support for the idea that it is parenting stress that plays a causal role in the<br />

emergence of problematic child <strong>temperament</strong> is provided by the evidence of predictive<br />

associations between maternal prenatal stress <strong>and</strong> subsequent negative views of the<br />

child’s <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> mental health morbidity. Huizink <strong>and</strong> her colleagues (2002)<br />

reported associations between increased maternal prenatal <strong>and</strong>/or postnatal stress <strong>and</strong><br />

infant ‘unadaptability’ (Huizink, Robles de Medina, Mulder, Visser, & Buitelaar, 2002).<br />

In the same cohort, increased levels of maternal prenatal stress further appeared to be<br />

associated with <strong>temperament</strong>al <strong>and</strong> behavioral problems at the child’s age of two years<br />

(Gutteling et al., 2005). Likewise, in the study of Pesonen <strong>and</strong> her colleagues (2005),<br />

mothers who reported continuous high stress from the pre- to the postnatal period over<br />

six months perceived the <strong>temperament</strong> of their six-month-old infants as more negatively<br />

<strong>and</strong> less positively tuned than did mothers who experienced increasing, decreasing or<br />

continuous low stress over the same periods. Maternal stress across child ages two to<br />

three years has also been shown to play a causal role in the emergence of problematic<br />

child behavior (Belsky et al., 1996). Also in the recent studies of Robinson, Oddy et al.<br />

(2008) <strong>and</strong> Bergman, Sarkar et al. (2008), high or increasing stress during pregnancy<br />

was predictive of child mental health morbidity or fearfulness in toddlerhood <strong>and</strong> at the<br />

age of five.<br />

In terms of a more global experience of stress, evidence of the predictive link<br />

between <strong>parental</strong> overall stress <strong>and</strong> subsequent child <strong>temperament</strong> can be drawn from<br />

30


the above-mentioned studies of prenatal stress (Bergman et al., 2008; Huizink et al.,<br />

2002; Pesonen et al., 2005; Robinson et al., 2008). Further, Räikkönen <strong>and</strong> her<br />

colleagues (2006) reported associations between parents’ higher perceived global stress<br />

<strong>and</strong> more negatively tuned perceptions of the child’s <strong>temperament</strong> during infancy. In the<br />

study of Crnic, Gaze <strong>and</strong> Hoffman (2005), the intercorrelation between two indices of<br />

<strong>parental</strong> stress, namely more general life stress <strong>and</strong> more specific parenting daily hassles<br />

at the child’s age of three, was low, indicating that those two indexes of stressfulness<br />

were predominantly independent, but they were both still predictive of parent <strong>and</strong> child<br />

behavior <strong>and</strong> dyadic interaction at the child’s age of five. The authors thus concluded<br />

that cumulative stress, resulting from parenting hassles or other life events, may build<br />

across developmental periods to create an increased risk for parenting <strong>and</strong> child<br />

functioning (Crnic et al., 2005).<br />

However, studies that focus on the predictive associations between <strong>parental</strong> overall<br />

stress <strong>and</strong> child <strong>temperament</strong> from infancy to childhood still seem to be lacking.<br />

Further, even though <strong>temperament</strong> is a neutral construct that covers a wide variance of<br />

behavior, there still seems to be a lack of longitudinal studies moving from global<br />

‘child-difficultness’, or problematic behavior –measures to linking <strong>parental</strong> overall<br />

stress with their perceptions of a wider range of infant/child <strong>temperament</strong>al<br />

characteristics over time.<br />

1.5 Aims of the study<br />

The overall aim of the current study was to examine the degree of <strong>continuity</strong> of<br />

<strong>temperament</strong> over five years from the infant’s age of six months to the child’s age of<br />

five <strong>and</strong> a half years, <strong>and</strong> to investigate the bidirectional effects between child<br />

<strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> parents’ <strong>personality</strong> traits <strong>and</strong> experienced overall stress during that<br />

time. Figure 1 shows the general outline of the current study.<br />

31


Figure 1. General outline of the study. Roman numerals refer to individual substudies (please see the<br />

original list of publications on page 10) that have allowed examination of the specific research questions<br />

1.5.1 Continuity of <strong>temperament</strong> from infancy to middle childhood<br />

1.5.1.1 Study I. Continuity of mother-rated <strong>temperament</strong><br />

First, I addressed the question of <strong>temperament</strong>al <strong>continuity</strong> among 231 Finnish children<br />

within the theoretical framework <strong>and</strong> operational definitions developed by Rothbart <strong>and</strong><br />

her co-workers (e.g., Rothbart & Bates, 2006; Rothbart et al., 2001). I assessed<br />

differential homotypic <strong>and</strong> heterotypic <strong>continuity</strong> between individual subscales <strong>and</strong><br />

latent superconstructs of <strong>temperament</strong> as rated by the mother when the child was aged<br />

six months <strong>and</strong> when the child was aged five <strong>and</strong> a half years. Based primarily on<br />

Rothbart’s theory emphasizing developmental <strong>continuity</strong> <strong>and</strong> structural change of<br />

<strong>temperament</strong>, <strong>and</strong> partly on the prior findings (Rothbart & Bates, 2006), I hypothesized<br />

that all individual <strong>temperament</strong> subscales in infancy would show differential homotypic<br />

<strong>and</strong>/or heterotypic <strong>continuity</strong> up until middle childhood.<br />

32


As for the latent <strong>temperament</strong> superconstructs, I hypothesized that significant<br />

continuities would be found between infant positive affectivity <strong>and</strong> middle childhood<br />

extraversion, <strong>and</strong> between infant <strong>and</strong> middle childhood negative affectivity. I also<br />

hypothesized that infant positive affectivity would predict effortful control in middle<br />

childhood, as the positive affectivity construct contains early characteristics of self-<br />

regulation, i.e. duration of orienting (Kochanska et al., 1998). Additionally, based on<br />

previous literature on the associations between early activity <strong>and</strong> later extraversion<br />

(Eaton, 1994; Hagekull & Bohlin, 1998), specific effects of infant activity were also<br />

tested by including specific paths from infant activity to the middle childhood<br />

superconstructs in the model of latent superconstructs.<br />

Moreover, by taking an ipsative, person-level approach to <strong>temperament</strong>al <strong>continuity</strong>,<br />

I aimed to explore whether <strong>temperament</strong> from infancy to middle childhood clustered to<br />

form profiles indicative of the <strong>personality</strong> types characterized in research on <strong>personality</strong>.<br />

As there are both empirical <strong>and</strong> conceptual links between <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>personality</strong><br />

traits, I hypothesized that <strong>temperament</strong> in infancy <strong>and</strong> in childhood would cluster to<br />

form profiles indicative of three different <strong>temperament</strong> types that would correspond<br />

conceptually to the <strong>personality</strong> types consistently identified in childhood <strong>and</strong> adulthood,<br />

i.e., the resilient, undercontrolled <strong>and</strong> overcontrolled <strong>personality</strong> types (Asendorpf et al.,<br />

2001; Asendorpf & van Aken, 1999; Caspi & Silva, 1995; Robins et al., 1996). Infant<br />

activity level was included as a separate dimension in the cluster formation.<br />

I hypothesized that, first, the <strong>temperament</strong> type corresponding to the resilient type<br />

would be marked by a relatively high level of positive emotionality in infancy <strong>and</strong> low<br />

levels of negative affectivity both in infancy <strong>and</strong> in childhood, <strong>and</strong> relatively high<br />

effortful control in childhood. Second, the <strong>temperament</strong> type corresponding to the<br />

undercontrolled type would be characterized by relatively high levels of negative<br />

affectivity both in infancy <strong>and</strong> in childhood, <strong>and</strong> a high level of extraversion <strong>and</strong><br />

relatively weak self-regulative capacities in childhood. Finally, the overcontrolled<br />

<strong>temperament</strong> type would be marked by relatively high negative affectivity in infancy<br />

<strong>and</strong> by low extraversion <strong>and</strong> relatively high self-regulative capacities in childhood The<br />

level of infant activity was hypothesized to be positively associated with childhood<br />

extraversion <strong>and</strong>, accordingly, be relatively high in the second, more undercontrolled<br />

type, <strong>and</strong> low in the third, more overcontrolled type.<br />

33


1.5.1.2 Study II. Continuity of father-rated <strong>temperament</strong><br />

Second, I aimed to fill the gap in our knowledge of father-rated <strong>temperament</strong>al<br />

<strong>continuity</strong> from infancy to middle childhood within a sample of 115 father-infant-child<br />

dyads. Within this second study, I also examined whether the developmental paths in<br />

father-rated <strong>temperament</strong> were similar to those of mother-rated <strong>temperament</strong> within a<br />

sample of 109 father–mother–infant/child -triads. The smaller sample size in father-<br />

ratings, however, restricted me to the variable level analyses, <strong>and</strong> did not allow a<br />

reliable enough ipsative level analysis.<br />

Because mother- <strong>and</strong> father-rated <strong>temperament</strong> have, according to previous cross-<br />

sectional data, been shown to be interrelated, <strong>and</strong> show a similar structural pattern in<br />

infancy, toddlerhood <strong>and</strong> childhood, I hypothesized that father-rated <strong>temperament</strong><br />

would also show significant homo- <strong>and</strong> heterotypic <strong>continuity</strong> from infancy to five <strong>and</strong><br />

a half years of age. I also hypothesized that paternal <strong>and</strong> maternal ratings of<br />

<strong>temperament</strong> would show congruent <strong>continuity</strong>.<br />

1.5.2 Transactional development of parent <strong>and</strong> child characteristics from<br />

infancy to middle childhood<br />

After demonstrating the <strong>continuity</strong> of parent-rated <strong>temperament</strong> in itself from infancy to<br />

middle childhood, the next step of the present study was to capture the transactions<br />

between <strong>parental</strong> characteristics <strong>and</strong> child <strong>temperament</strong> over the same period of time.<br />

The bidirectional effects between child <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>parental</strong> characteristics were<br />

investigated in terms of parents’ <strong>personality</strong> traits <strong>and</strong> overall stress experienced during<br />

that time.<br />

1.5.2.1 Study III. Parental <strong>personality</strong> traits extraversion <strong>and</strong> neuroticism <strong>and</strong><br />

infant/child <strong>temperament</strong><br />

In the third study, I aimed to examine rank-order stability <strong>and</strong> transactional associations<br />

between parents’ <strong>personality</strong> traits extraversion <strong>and</strong> neuroticism (McCrae & Costa,<br />

1987; McCrae & John, 1992) <strong>and</strong> the parents’ views of their infant’s/child’s<br />

<strong>temperament</strong> within a sample of 109 mother-father-infant/child triads. The stability <strong>and</strong><br />

change in <strong>parental</strong> extraversion <strong>and</strong> neuroticism were examined in relation to parent-<br />

rated infant activity <strong>and</strong> sumscores of positive <strong>and</strong> negative affectivity <strong>and</strong> ratings of the<br />

34


child’s <strong>temperament</strong>al sumscores extraversion, effortful control, <strong>and</strong> negative<br />

affectivity.<br />

In line with existing literature, <strong>parental</strong> extraversion <strong>and</strong> neuroticism were expected<br />

to show high stability over five years. In terms of <strong>parental</strong> <strong>personality</strong> change, I<br />

hypothesized that <strong>parental</strong> ratings of the infant’s higher positive affectivity would<br />

predict an increase in <strong>parental</strong> extraversion <strong>and</strong> a decrease in neuroticism, <strong>and</strong> that<br />

ratings of the infant’s higher negative affectivity would predict an increase in <strong>parental</strong><br />

neuroticism <strong>and</strong> a decrease in extraversion. Infant activity was examined with no<br />

specific direction of change in <strong>parental</strong> <strong>personality</strong> hypothesized. Further, initially<br />

higher <strong>parental</strong> extraversion was expected to predict higher parent-rated extraversion<br />

<strong>and</strong> effortful control <strong>and</strong> lower negative affectivity in the child, <strong>and</strong> initially higher<br />

<strong>parental</strong> neuroticism was expected to predict ratings of lower extraversion <strong>and</strong> effortful<br />

control, <strong>and</strong> higher negative affectivity in the child.<br />

1.5.2.2 Study IV. Mother’s overall stress <strong>and</strong> infant/child <strong>temperament</strong><br />

Next, I aimed to depict the over-time stability <strong>and</strong> transactional associations between<br />

mothers’ perceived overall stress <strong>and</strong> infant/child <strong>temperament</strong> over the same time<br />

period within a sample of 231 mothers <strong>and</strong> their infants/children. In this fourth study,<br />

the first aim was to test the transactional associations between maternal stress <strong>and</strong> six<br />

individual <strong>temperament</strong> subscales that represented conceptual equivalents in the infants’<br />

as in the children’s <strong>temperament</strong> questionnaires. The subscales activity level,<br />

anger/frustration, fear, soothability, attentional focusing, <strong>and</strong> smiling <strong>and</strong> laughter were<br />

chosen for the analyses. Second, I aimed to test whether there were transactions<br />

between maternal stress <strong>and</strong> the latent infant/child <strong>temperament</strong> superconstructs.<br />

I hypothesized that <strong>temperament</strong> characteristics reflecting higher negative<br />

emotionality <strong>and</strong> lower positive affectivity in infants would contribute to an increase in<br />

maternal stress over five years beginning in infancy, <strong>and</strong> that higher maternal stress<br />

would contribute to an increase in the child’s negative affectivity <strong>and</strong> a decrease in<br />

positive affectivity characteristics <strong>and</strong> self-regulative functioning over the same period.<br />

In sum, the current study first aimed at establishing the <strong>continuity</strong> of <strong>temperament</strong><br />

within a family context, as perceived by both parents, <strong>and</strong> examining whether the<br />

developmental paths of infant/child <strong>temperament</strong> are similar in both parents’ eyes.<br />

35


Second, the study aimed to examine the transactional associations between <strong>parental</strong><br />

<strong>personality</strong> traits of extraversion <strong>and</strong> neuroticism <strong>and</strong> infant/child <strong>temperament</strong>, <strong>and</strong><br />

further, explore the transactional development of maternal perceived stress <strong>and</strong> maternal<br />

ratings of infant/child <strong>temperament</strong> over this five-year period.<br />

36


2 METHODS<br />

2.1 Outline of the study<br />

The ongoing Glaku project was initiated in 1998. It was designed to examine the<br />

neonatal <strong>and</strong> early childhood predictors of hypertension development, as well as<br />

normative psychological development in childhood. The Glaku project is a collaborative<br />

study involving the Departments of Psychology <strong>and</strong> Medicine at Helsinki University,<br />

<strong>and</strong> the Department of Pediatrics <strong>and</strong> Neonatology at Helsinki City Maternity hospital.<br />

At the baseline of the study in 1998 the infants averaged six months of age (M= 6.3,<br />

S.D. = 1.4 months). A follow-up was conducted five years later in 2003–2004, at which<br />

point the children averaged five <strong>and</strong> a half years of age (M= 66.1, S.D. = 2.9 months).<br />

These two time-points served as the sample for the current study. A second follow-up<br />

was conducted in 2006, <strong>and</strong> a further follow-up will be conducted in 2009.<br />

The initial study sample was collected between March 1 st <strong>and</strong> November 30 th of 1998<br />

in Helsinki City Maternity hospital (Kätilöopiston sairaala). The hospital is one of the<br />

principal maternity hospitals in Helsinki, with approximately 4500 births per year. The<br />

midwives in four regular maternity wards were asked to give a questionnaire to all<br />

women with singleton healthy births in which both parents were of Finnish origin.<br />

During this period there were 2746 births that would have been eligible. For reasons<br />

connected with the vacation period of the midwives, some mothers did not receive the<br />

questionnaire (only 30 of the invited mothers refused to participate). However, it is not<br />

likely that this involved selection bias, because the midwives were unaware of the exact<br />

aims of the study (Str<strong>and</strong>berg, Järvenpää, Vanhanen, & McKeigue, 2001).<br />

2.2 Participants<br />

In 1998, a total of 1049 mothers completed the questionnaire, which included questions<br />

on antenatal stress <strong>and</strong> nutrition, <strong>and</strong> on other lifestyle variables, as well as a question<br />

eliciting permission to examine their maternity records. Of these 1049 mothers, the first<br />

500, along with the biological fathers of the baby, were invited to participate in a<br />

psychological survey on child development.<br />

Of these 500 families, a total of 328 (65.6%) family units (mother <strong>and</strong>/or father)<br />

returned the questionnaire sent to them by mail approximately six months after the<br />

delivery. Of these family units, data on both biological parents was simultaneously<br />

37


available on 180 units, 141 questionnaires were returned only by the mother <strong>and</strong> seven<br />

only by the father. In total, 321 mothers <strong>and</strong> 187 fathers returned the questionnaire.<br />

Details of the research protocol <strong>and</strong> of the recruitment of the initial sample are described<br />

in Str<strong>and</strong>berg et al. (2001). The participants in Studies I – IV were taken from the same<br />

sample consisting of families that participated in the Glaku project at baseline in 1998<br />

when the infant was six months of age <strong>and</strong> again at follow-up in 2003-2004, at the<br />

child’s age of five <strong>and</strong> a half years. The institutional Review Board at the University of<br />

Helsinki approved this project, <strong>and</strong> the participating parents gave their informed<br />

consent.<br />

A total of 231 mother-infant/child dyads providing data at both data-collection points<br />

formed the sample for Study I. A total of 115 father-infant/child dyads, providing data<br />

at both data-collection points formed the sample for the first aim of Study II, <strong>and</strong> a total<br />

of 109 father–mother–infant/child triads, providing data at both data-collection points<br />

formed the sample for the second aim of Study II. A sample of 109 mother-father-<br />

infant/child triads providing complete <strong>parental</strong> <strong>and</strong> child data at both time points formed<br />

the sample for Study III, <strong>and</strong> a total of 231 mother–infant/child dyads providing<br />

complete <strong>parental</strong> <strong>and</strong> child data at both time points formed the Study IV sample.<br />

2.3 Measures<br />

2.3.1 Infant <strong>temperament</strong><br />

Parental ratings of infant <strong>temperament</strong> were derived using the Infant Behavior<br />

Questionnaire (the IBQ, Rothbart, 1981). The questionnaire is designed to refer to<br />

specific, concrete behaviors of the infant, occurring during the past week (previous two<br />

weeks for some items).The 96 items in the IBQ are evaluated on a seven-point scale<br />

reflecting the relative frequency of specified infant reactions. There are six subscales<br />

(Rothbart, 1986). Activity level describes the level of the infant’s gross motor activity,<br />

including movement of the arms <strong>and</strong> legs, squirming, <strong>and</strong> locomotor activity. Smiling<br />

<strong>and</strong> laughter is related to expressions of joy. Soothability (the recovery parameter of<br />

distress) is defined as the effectiveness of soothing techniques when the child is<br />

experiencing negative affects. Duration of orienting refers to sustained involvement<br />

with a single object or activity when there has been no sudden change in stimulation.<br />

Distress to limitations refers to distress when goal achievement has been blocked or a<br />

38


desirable object removed. Fear is expressed by crying or fussing <strong>and</strong>/or extended<br />

latency to approach intense or novel stimuli.<br />

Infant positive affectivity superconstruct has in previous studies been comprised of<br />

slightly differing combinations of smiling <strong>and</strong> laughter, duration of orienting,<br />

soothability <strong>and</strong> activity level (see, e.g., Goldsmith & Campos, 1990; Kochanska et al.,<br />

1998; Rothbart, 1986; Worobey & Blajda, 1989), while distress to limitations <strong>and</strong> fear<br />

have quite consistently been used in defining a negative affectivity superconstruct (see,<br />

e.g., Kochanska et al., 1998; Rothbart, 1986). Infant activity has been shown to<br />

associate, besides positive affectivity, with negative affectivity as well (Goldsmith &<br />

Campos, 1990; Goldsmith et al., 1999; Kochanska et al., 1998; Lemery et al., 1999;<br />

Rothbart, 1986; Worobey & Blajda, 1989).<br />

The <strong>temperament</strong> superconstructs in the first, second <strong>and</strong> fourth study were devised<br />

by defining the latent factors in the structural equation model, <strong>and</strong> in the third study by<br />

computing the sumscores. The infant positive affectivity factor was defined by smiling<br />

<strong>and</strong> laughter, duration of orienting <strong>and</strong> soothability, <strong>and</strong> infant negative affectivity was<br />

defined by distress to limitations <strong>and</strong> fear, while infant activity was allowed to load on<br />

both positive <strong>and</strong> negative affectivity latent factor in the structural equation model. In<br />

the cluster analysis in Study I <strong>and</strong> in the multilevel analysis in Study III, infant positive<br />

<strong>and</strong> negative affectivity superconstructs were computed as described in previous studies<br />

(Kochanska et al., 1998; Rothbart et al., 2001), with the exception that infant activity<br />

was not included in the superconstructs, but was used as a separate dimension in the<br />

analyses.<br />

Previous studies have reported good test-retest <strong>and</strong> alpha reliabilities for the IBQ<br />

(Gartstein, Slobodskaya, & Kinsht, 2006; Gartstein & Rothbart, 2003; Rothbart, 1986).<br />

Convergence between laboratory observation ratings of <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> the IBQ scales<br />

has also been reported (Bridges, Palmer, Morales, Hurtado & Tsai, 1993; Goldsmith &<br />

Rothbart, 1991). In the current study the Cronbach’s alphas for the subscales ranged<br />

from 0.74 to 0.93. Moreover, the confirmatory factor analyses conducted in Studies I<br />

<strong>and</strong> II confirmed the construct validity in the Finnish sample.<br />

39


2.3.2 <strong>Child</strong> <strong>temperament</strong><br />

Parental ratings of the child’s <strong>temperament</strong> were elicited by using the <strong>Child</strong>ren’s<br />

Behavior Questionnaire (the CBQ, Rothbart et al., 2001). The 195 items in the CBQ are<br />

evaluated on a seven-point scale reflecting the relative frequency of specified child<br />

reactions in concrete situations in previous weeks. There are 15 subscales. Activity level<br />

describes the level of the child’s gross motor activity. Anger/Frustration refers to<br />

negative affectivity related to the interruption of an ongoing task, or goal blocking.<br />

Approach anticipation refers to the amount of excitement <strong>and</strong> anticipation expressed<br />

toward expected pleasurable activities. Attentional focusing refers to the capacity to<br />

maintain attentional focus on task-related channels. Discomfort means negative<br />

affectivity related to sensory qualities of stimulation, including intensity, rate, <strong>and</strong><br />

complexities of light, movement, sound <strong>and</strong> texture. Falling reactivity <strong>and</strong> soothability<br />

(the recovery parameter of distress) is the rate of recovery from peak distress,<br />

excitement, or general arousal. Fear is expressed by negative affectivity including<br />

unease, worry or nervousness, which is related to anticipated pain or distress <strong>and</strong>/or<br />

potentially threatening situations. High intensity pleasure refers to enjoyment related to<br />

situations involving high stimulus intensity, rate, complexity, novelty, <strong>and</strong> incongruity.<br />

Impulsivity is the speed of response initiation. Inhibitory control is the capacity to plan<br />

<strong>and</strong> to suppress inappropriate approach responses under instructions or in novel or<br />

uncertain situations. Low intensity pleasure means enjoyment related to situations<br />

involving low stimulus intensity, rate, complexity, novelty, <strong>and</strong> incongruity. Perceptual<br />

sensitivity is the detection of slight, low-intensity stimuli from the external<br />

environment. Sadness refers to negative affectivity <strong>and</strong> lowered mood <strong>and</strong> energy<br />

related to exposure to suffering, disappointment, <strong>and</strong> object loss. Shyness refers to slow<br />

or inhibited speed of approach <strong>and</strong> discomfort in social situations. Finally, Smiling <strong>and</strong><br />

laughter is related to positive affect in response to daily happenings.<br />

According to Rothbart et al. (2001), high levels of impulsivity, high intensity<br />

pleasure, activity level, smiling <strong>and</strong> laughter, <strong>and</strong> positive anticipation, <strong>and</strong> a low level<br />

of shyness form an extraversion superconstruct; high levels of low intensity pleasure,<br />

inhibitory control, perceptual sensitivity <strong>and</strong> attentional focusing form an effortful<br />

control superconstruct; <strong>and</strong> high levels of fear, sadness, discomfort, <strong>and</strong><br />

anger/frustration <strong>and</strong> a low level of soothability form a negative affectivity<br />

40


superconstruct. These superconstructs were devised in the current study by defining the<br />

latent factors in the structural equation models <strong>and</strong> computing the sumscores. Previous<br />

studies have reported good test-retest <strong>and</strong> alpha reliabilities for the CBQ (Rothbart et<br />

al., 2001). In the current study, the Cronbach’s alphas for the subscales ranged from<br />

0.65 to 0.90. Moreover, the confirmatory factor analyses conducted in Studies I <strong>and</strong> II<br />

confirmed the construct validity in the Finnish sample.<br />

2.3.3 Parental <strong>personality</strong> traits<br />

Parental <strong>personality</strong> traits were assessed on the extraversion <strong>and</strong> neuroticism scales<br />

derived from the authorized adaptation of the NEO <strong>personality</strong> inventory (NEO-PI;<br />

Costa & McCrae, 1985; Pulver, Allik, Pulkkinen, & Hämäläinen, 1995). The self-report<br />

questionnaire contains 48 items measuring extraversion <strong>and</strong> 48 items measuring<br />

neuroticism, <strong>and</strong> these 96 items were included in the assessment of <strong>parental</strong> <strong>personality</strong><br />

at both time points. Extraversion is defined in terms of warmth, gregariousness,<br />

assertiveness, activity, excitement-seeking <strong>and</strong> positive emotions, <strong>and</strong> neuroticism in<br />

terms of anxiety, anger, depression, self-consciousness, impulsiveness, <strong>and</strong><br />

vulnerability. The items are scored on a five-point Likert scale (strongly disagree to<br />

strongly agree), <strong>and</strong> support for the validity <strong>and</strong> reliability of the domains has been<br />

consistent (Costa & McCrae, 1992; McCrae & Costa, 1987). In the current study, the<br />

Cronbach’s alphas for extraversion <strong>and</strong> neuroticism ranged from .91 to .94 at both data-<br />

collection points.<br />

2.3.4 Perceived stress<br />

The mothers self-reported their level of perceived overall stress approximately six<br />

months postpartum <strong>and</strong> again five years later using the shortened version of the original<br />

14-item perceived stress scale (the PSS; Cohen, Kamarck, & Mermelstein, 1983). PSS<br />

is sensitive to chronic stress deriving from ongoing life circumstances, expectations<br />

concerning future events, or from any life-events. The items are designed to tap the<br />

degree to which respondents find their lives unpredictable, uncontrollable, <strong>and</strong><br />

overwhelming. These three issues have been repeatedly found to be central components<br />

of the experience of stress (Cohen et al., 1983).<br />

41


The short version, constructed as suggested by Cohen <strong>and</strong> his colleagues (1983),<br />

consisted of the four items that were originally most highly correlated with the 14-item<br />

scale: the scale thus includes statements such as ‘How often have you found that you<br />

could not cope with all the things that you had to do’ <strong>and</strong> ‘How often have you felt that<br />

you were on top of things (a reversed item)’. In the current study, an additional fifth<br />

question, derived from the original scale, asking directly about the level of experienced<br />

stress was added: ‘In the last month, how often have you felt nervous <strong>and</strong> stressed’?<br />

Mothers evaluated the degree to which they had appraised situations in their lives as<br />

stressful (unpredictable, uncontrollable, overwhelming) in recent weeks on these five<br />

items, using a scale ranging from never (1) to all the time (5). The scale has shown good<br />

reliability <strong>and</strong> validity in prior studies (Cohen et al., 1983). Cronbach’s alpha in the<br />

current study was .79.<br />

2.4 Statistical analyses<br />

Structural equation modeling was the main statistical method used in all four studies.<br />

Several analyses were used for comparative purposes, such as the t-tests for independent<br />

<strong>and</strong> dependent variables. In Study I, the k-means clustering algorithm was used to test<br />

how the study variables (sumscores) in infancy <strong>and</strong> in middle childhood clustered to<br />

profiles. Univariate analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used in verifying that the<br />

clusters were significantly different on the criterion variables. Structural equation<br />

modeling <strong>and</strong> path analyses were performed using the Mplus statistical package 2.13-<br />

4.1 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998 - 2006). St<strong>and</strong>ard model-fitting procedures <strong>and</strong> the<br />

maximum-likelihood-estimation method were adopted. Table 1 summarizes the<br />

information on participants, research design, measures, <strong>and</strong> main statistical analyses in<br />

the current study.<br />

42


Table 1. Sample, research design, measures <strong>and</strong> main statistical methods used in Study I – IV<br />

Study Participants Measures Main statistical methods<br />

I N=231<br />

motherinfant/child<br />

-dyads<br />

II N=115<br />

fatherinfant/childdyads;<br />

N=109<br />

father–<br />

mother–<br />

infant/child-<br />

triads<br />

III N=109<br />

motherfatherinfant/childtriads<br />

IV N=231<br />

motherinfant/childdyads<br />

Baseline 1998, infant age 6 months:<br />

Infant Temperament Questionnaire<br />

(IBQ)<br />

Follow-up 2004, child age 5 ½ years:<br />

<strong>Child</strong>ren’s Behavior Questionnaire<br />

(CBQ)<br />

Baseline 1998, infant age 6 months:<br />

Infant Temperament Questionnaire<br />

(IBQ)<br />

Follow-up 2004, child age 5 ½ years:<br />

<strong>Child</strong>ren’s Behavior Questionnaire<br />

(CBQ)<br />

Baseline 1998, infant age 6 months:<br />

Infant Temperament Questionnaire<br />

(IBQ)<br />

NEO <strong>personality</strong> inventory (NEO-PI)<br />

Follow-up 2004, child age 5 ½ years:<br />

<strong>Child</strong>ren’s Behavior Questionnaire<br />

(CBQ)<br />

NEO <strong>personality</strong> inventory (NEO-PI)<br />

Baseline 1998, infant age 6 months:<br />

Infant Temperament Questionnaire<br />

(IBQ)<br />

Perceived Stress Scale (PSS)<br />

Follow-up 2004, child age 5 ½ years:<br />

<strong>Child</strong>ren’s Behavior Questionnaire<br />

(CBQ)<br />

Perceived Stress Scale (PSS)<br />

43<br />

Confirmatory factor analysis,<br />

Structural equation modeling,<br />

t-test for independent samples,<br />

k-means clustering algorithm,<br />

ANOVA, post hoc t-tests<br />

(Pearson correlations)<br />

Confirmatory factor analysis,<br />

Structural equation modeling,<br />

t-tests for independent <strong>and</strong><br />

dependent samples,<br />

(Pearson correlations)<br />

Confirmatory factor analysis,<br />

Structural equation modeling<br />

multilevel technique,<br />

t-tests for independent <strong>and</strong><br />

dependent samples,<br />

(Pearson correlations)<br />

Confirmatory factor analysis,<br />

Structural equation modeling,<br />

(Pearson correlations)


3 RESULTS<br />

The main results of the four separate studies are summarized below. The details are to<br />

be found in the original publications.<br />

3.1 Study I. Continuity of mother-rated <strong>temperament</strong> from<br />

infancy to middle childhood<br />

3.1.1 Continuity of <strong>temperament</strong> subscales <strong>and</strong> superconstructs<br />

Pearson correlations were computed to test differential homotypic <strong>and</strong> heterotypic<br />

<strong>continuity</strong> between the individual subscales of <strong>temperament</strong> measured in infancy (IBQ)<br />

<strong>and</strong> in middle childhood (CBQ) over five years (Table 2). Activity level, smiling <strong>and</strong><br />

laughter, distress to limitations <strong>and</strong> fear showed significant homo- <strong>and</strong> heterotypic<br />

<strong>continuity</strong> from infancy to middle childhood, whereas there was evidence only of<br />

heterotypic <strong>continuity</strong> for soothability <strong>and</strong> duration of orienting. Heterotypic <strong>continuity</strong><br />

was shown, for example, between higher infant activity <strong>and</strong> later aspects of extraversion<br />

(e.g. approach anticipation <strong>and</strong> high-intensity pleasure) <strong>and</strong> between higher infant<br />

smiling <strong>and</strong> laughter <strong>and</strong> later low-intensity pleasure, <strong>and</strong> between higher levels of<br />

infant distress to limitations <strong>and</strong> fear <strong>and</strong> later sadness. Associations indicating<br />

developmental interaction between traits that are controlled by separate genetic<br />

mechanisms were also shown, for example, between higher infant soothability <strong>and</strong> later<br />

high-intensity pleasure, <strong>and</strong> between higher infant fear <strong>and</strong> lower levels of low-intensity<br />

pleasure <strong>and</strong> smiling <strong>and</strong> laughter in middle childhood.<br />

Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was used to verify the factor structures of infant<br />

positive <strong>and</strong> negative affectivity (Kochanska et al., 1998) <strong>and</strong> middle childhood<br />

extraversion, effortful control <strong>and</strong> negative affectivity latent superconstructs (Rothbart<br />

et al., 2001), <strong>and</strong> the confirmed factor structure was used as a foundation for the<br />

structural analyses. Figure 2 shows the a priori hypothesized model including all<br />

possible paths from the positive <strong>and</strong> negative affectivity latent superconstructs in<br />

infancy to the extraversion, effortful control <strong>and</strong> negative affectivity latent<br />

superconstructs in middle childhood.<br />

The positive affectivity latent superconstruct in infancy predicted extraversion <strong>and</strong><br />

effortful control in middle childhood, while negative affectivity in infancy predicted<br />

44


negative affectivity in middle childhood. The model showed an adequate fit ( 2 /df = 1.9;<br />

RMSEA= 0.06; CFI = 0.914), <strong>and</strong> <strong>temperament</strong> in infancy accounted for 4.6, 22.3 <strong>and</strong><br />

6.0% of the variance in extraversion, effortful control, <strong>and</strong> negative affectivity in middle<br />

childhood, respectively.<br />

An additional SEM involving the specific effects of infant activity, as an addition to<br />

paths from infancy to middle childhood latent superconstructs, is presented in Figure 3.<br />

The model with specific effects of infant activity showed an acceptable fit ( 2 /df = 1.9;<br />

RMSEA= 0.06; CFI = 0.922), <strong>and</strong> the 2 difference test showed that the model with the<br />

specific effects of infant activity fitted the data significantly better than the initial a<br />

priori hypothesized model that did not involve the specific effects. The model involving<br />

specific effects revealed significant paths from infant activity to later extraversion <strong>and</strong><br />

negative affectivity, while the path to effortful control was not significant. In this<br />

model, infant <strong>temperament</strong> accounted for 8.1, 23.0, <strong>and</strong> 11.7% of the variance in<br />

extraversion, effortful control, <strong>and</strong> negative affectivity in middle childhood,<br />

respectively. Adding the specific paths of infant activity in the model eliminated the<br />

significant path from infant positive affectivity to extraversion.<br />

45


Table 2. Pearson correlations between the <strong>temperament</strong> subscales measured in infancy <strong>and</strong> in<br />

middle childhood (N = 231 mother-infant/child-dyads)<br />

Activity<br />

Level<br />

Smiling /<br />

Laughter<br />

Figure 2. Structural equation model showing associations between latent <strong>temperament</strong> superconstructs<br />

measured in infancy <strong>and</strong> in middle childhood. St<strong>and</strong>ardized values are given for the path coefficients.<br />

All the nonsignificant paths are also presented ( 2 /df = 1.9; RMSEA= 0.06; CFI = 0.914)<br />

46<br />

Infancy: IBQ<br />

Soothability Duration of<br />

Orienting<br />

Distress to<br />

Limitations<br />

Middle childhood:<br />

CBQ<br />

Activity Level .26** .09 .05 .01 .02 .03<br />

Anger/Frustration .02 -.05 .03 .04 .16* .12<br />

Approach/<br />

Anticipation<br />

.17** .29** .20** .19** -.01 -.00<br />

Attentional<br />

Focusing<br />

-.10 .10 .12 .05 -.01 -.12<br />

Discomfort<br />

Falling Reactivity<br />

-.03 -.12 -.02 -.00 .16* .10<br />

& Soothability -.01 .20** .06 .07 -.34** -.11<br />

Fear -.05 -.04 -.04 .01 .16* .23**<br />

High-Intensity<br />

Pleasure<br />

.18** .09 .15* .01 .02 -.08<br />

Impulsivity .25** .08 .14* -.01 .02 .05<br />

Inhibitory Control -.13* .02 .06 .08 -.07 -.05<br />

Low-Intensity<br />

Pleasure<br />

.05 .20** .11 .08 -.02 -.14*<br />

Perceptual<br />

Sensitivity<br />

.01 .20** .12 .26** -.09 -.03<br />

Sadness -.05 .01 .05 .11 .14* .18**<br />

Shyness -.17* -.06 -.11 .02 .09 -.02<br />

Smiling/Laughter<br />

p < .05. * p < .01. **<br />

.20** .34** .15* .13* -.11 -.19**<br />

Fear


Figure 3. Structural equation model showing associations between latent <strong>temperament</strong> superconstructs<br />

measured in infancy <strong>and</strong> in middle childhood, with specific paths from infant activity to the middle<br />

childhood superconstructs included in the model. St<strong>and</strong>ardized values are given for the path coefficients.<br />

All the nonsignificant paths are also presented ( 2 /df = 1.9; RMSEA= 0.06; CFI = 0.922)<br />

3.1.2 Person-level <strong>continuity</strong> of <strong>temperament</strong>: “<strong>temperament</strong> types”<br />

A k- means clustering algorithm was used to test whether the <strong>temperament</strong><br />

superconstructs (sumscores) in infancy <strong>and</strong> in middle childhood cluster to form<br />

theoretically meaningful <strong>and</strong> coherent profiles representing different <strong>temperament</strong><br />

types. The reliability of the cluster solution was tested as recommended by<br />

Breckenridge (2000) <strong>and</strong> M<strong>and</strong>ara (2003), by examining whether a statistically<br />

significant agreement existed in a cross-validation between two r<strong>and</strong>omly divided<br />

cluster solutions. As separate SEM <strong>and</strong> cluster analyses for girls <strong>and</strong> boys did not show<br />

substantial differences, all the analyses were conducted with gender groups combined.<br />

Three “<strong>temperament</strong> types”, illustrated in Figure 4, profiling infants <strong>and</strong> children<br />

throughout the five-year-period were found: children belonging to the first cluster,<br />

named as “resilient”, were characterized by above-average levels of activity, positive<br />

affectivity, extraversion <strong>and</strong> effortful control, <strong>and</strong> below-average levels of negative<br />

affectivity in infancy <strong>and</strong> middle childhood (n = 73). Those in the second cluster, named<br />

“undercontrolled”, were characterized by an above average level of activity <strong>and</strong> a<br />

47


elow-average level of positive affectivity in infancy, by above-average levels of<br />

negative affectivity in infancy <strong>and</strong> middle childhood, by an above-average level of<br />

extraversion, <strong>and</strong> a well below average level of effortful control in middle childhood<br />

(n = 79). Those in the third cluster, named “overcontrolled”, were characterized by a<br />

well below average level of activity <strong>and</strong> a below-average level of positive affectivity,<br />

<strong>and</strong> an average level of negative affectivity in infancy. In middle childhood, they<br />

exhibited above-average levels of effortful control <strong>and</strong> negative affectivity, <strong>and</strong> well<br />

below the average level of extraversion (n = 79).<br />

Figure 4. Cluster analysis identifying three <strong>temperament</strong> “types” based on sumscores of <strong>temperament</strong><br />

superconstructs measured in infancy <strong>and</strong> in middle childhood. Note. All ps < 0.001 in ANOVAs testing<br />

the significance of differences between the clusters; all ps < 0.001 in post hoc pair-wise comparisons,<br />

except for p = 0.34 between “resilient” <strong>and</strong> “undercontrolled” in activity; p = 0.45 between<br />

“undercontrolled” <strong>and</strong> “overcontrolled” in positive affectivity; p = 0.04 between “undercontrolled” <strong>and</strong><br />

“overcontrolled” in infant negative affectivity; ps = 1.0 between “resilient” <strong>and</strong> “undercontrolled” in<br />

extraversion <strong>and</strong> between “resilient” <strong>and</strong> “overcontrolled” in effortful control; p = 0.08 between<br />

“undercontrolled” <strong>and</strong> “overcontrolled” in childhood negative affectivity<br />

48


3.2 Study II. Continuity of father-rated <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

congruence between mother- <strong>and</strong> father-rated <strong>continuity</strong><br />

3.2.1 Continuity of father-rated <strong>temperament</strong> subscales <strong>and</strong><br />

superconstructs<br />

Pearson correlations were computed to test differential homotypic <strong>and</strong> heterotypic<br />

<strong>continuity</strong> between the individual subscales of <strong>temperament</strong> measured in infancy (IBQ)<br />

<strong>and</strong> in middle childhood (CBQ) over five years. Father-rated activity level, smiling <strong>and</strong><br />

laughter <strong>and</strong> distress to limitations showed significant homo- <strong>and</strong> heterotypic <strong>continuity</strong><br />

from infancy to middle childhood, whereas there was evidence only of heterotypic<br />

<strong>continuity</strong> for soothability, duration of orienting <strong>and</strong> fear (Table 3). Heterotypic<br />

<strong>continuity</strong> was shown, for example, between higher infant smiling <strong>and</strong> laughter <strong>and</strong><br />

later low-intensity pleasure <strong>and</strong> between higher infant distress to limitations <strong>and</strong> later<br />

discomfort <strong>and</strong> fear. Associations indicating developmental interaction between<br />

different traits were also shown: for example, higher infant activity level associated with<br />

lower levels of inhibitory control <strong>and</strong> soothability in middle childhood, <strong>and</strong> higher<br />

infant fear <strong>and</strong> distress to limitations associated with lower levels of control-related<br />

abilities <strong>and</strong> less smiling <strong>and</strong> laughter in middle childhood.<br />

The CFA presented in the first study was used in verifying the factor structures of<br />

latent infant <strong>and</strong> child <strong>temperament</strong> superconstructs, <strong>and</strong> the confirmed factor structure<br />

was used as a foundation for the structural analyses. In the SEM, father-rated positive<br />

affectivity in infancy was shown to predict, as hypothesized, effortful control in middle<br />

childhood, but it did not predict later extraversion. The SEM with fit indexes is<br />

illustrated in Figure 5. As hypothesized, negative affectivity in infancy predicted<br />

negative affectivity in middle childhood, but also showed a developmental trend<br />

towards a low level of effortful control. The respective variances accounted for by<br />

father-rated positive <strong>and</strong> negative affectivity in infancy were 5.1, 22.7, <strong>and</strong> 10.0% for<br />

extraversion, effortful control <strong>and</strong> negative affectivity, respectively.<br />

As in the first study, when infant activity was added to the model as a specific path,<br />

the significance of a high level of infant activity as a specific determinant of later<br />

extraversion was revealed (Figure 6). The predictive associations between negative<br />

affectivity in infancy <strong>and</strong> in middle childhood <strong>and</strong> between positive affectivity <strong>and</strong> later<br />

effortful control were not affected by the specific activity path. For fathers, adding the<br />

49


specific effect of infant activity improved the fit of the model only slightly (²(1) = 3.8,<br />

p > .05).The variances accounted for by activity <strong>and</strong> positive <strong>and</strong> negative affectivity in<br />

infancy in this additional model were 8.0, 23.7, <strong>and</strong> 9.8% for extraversion, effortful<br />

control <strong>and</strong> negative affectivity, respectively.<br />

Table 3. Pearson correlations between the father-rated <strong>temperament</strong> subscales measured in infancy <strong>and</strong> in<br />

middle childhood (N = 115 father-infant/child dyads)<br />

Activity<br />

Level<br />

Smiling/<br />

Laughter<br />

Infancy: IBQ<br />

Soothability Duration of<br />

Orienting<br />

50<br />

Distress to<br />

Limitations<br />

Middle childhood:<br />

CBQ<br />

Activity Level .19* .06 .07 -.10 .03 .01<br />

Anger/Frustration<br />

Approach<br />

.20* -.12 .00 -.04 .27** .14<br />

Anticipation<br />

Attentional<br />

.14 -.09 -.03 .09 .16 -.05<br />

Focusing<br />

-.13 .19* .10<br />

.09 -.11 -.11<br />

Discomfort<br />

Falling reactivity<br />

.01 -.04 -.14 .08 .08 -.04<br />

& Soothability -.20* .20* -.01 .02 -.23* -.15<br />

Fear<br />

High-Intensity<br />

-.08 -.05 .05 .07 .18* .10<br />

Pleasure<br />

.13 -.04 .15 -.01 .06 .02<br />

Impulsivity<br />

Inhibitory<br />

.11 -.02 .17 -.16 .14 -.04<br />

Control<br />

Low-Intensity<br />

-.27** .17 .02<br />

.07 -.31** -.17<br />

Pleasure<br />

Perceptual<br />

-.01 .30** .19* .27** -.14 -.19*<br />

Sensitivity<br />

-.03<br />

.16<br />

.01<br />

.21*<br />

-.13<br />

Fear<br />

-.27**<br />

Sadness -.05 -.18* -.16 -.02 .06 .02<br />

Shyness .01 -.11 -.14 -.04 .05 .18<br />

Smiling/ Laughter .04 .23* .16 .07 -.12 -.27**<br />

p < .05. * p < .01. **


Figure 5. Structural equation model showing associations between father-rated latent <strong>temperament</strong><br />

superconstructs measured in infancy <strong>and</strong> in middle childhood (n = 115). St<strong>and</strong>ardized values are given for<br />

the path coefficients. All the nonsignificant paths are also presented ( 2 /df = 1.3; RMSEA= .05;<br />

CFI=.927)<br />

Figure 6. Structural equation model showing associations between father-rated latent <strong>temperament</strong><br />

superconstructs measured in infancy <strong>and</strong> in middle childhood, with the specific path from infant activity<br />

to the middle childhood extraversion superconstruct included in the model. St<strong>and</strong>ardized values are given<br />

for the path coefficients. All the nonsignificant paths from infant negative <strong>and</strong> positive affectivity latent<br />

superconstructs are presented 2 /df = 1.3; RMSEA= .05; CFI = .931)<br />

51


3.2.2 Congruence between mother- <strong>and</strong> father-rated <strong>continuity</strong> of<br />

<strong>temperament</strong><br />

Congruence between parents’ ratings was investigated within a sample of 109 mother-<br />

father-infant/child –triads. Fathers rated their infant higher than mothers in fear,<br />

duration of orienting <strong>and</strong> negative affectivity, <strong>and</strong> in middle childhood, they rated their<br />

child higher in activity <strong>and</strong> impulsivity, <strong>and</strong> lower in approach anticipation, low-<br />

intensity pleasure, perceptual sensitivity, smiling <strong>and</strong> laughter <strong>and</strong> effortful control.<br />

Significant agreement between parents’ ratings of individual <strong>temperament</strong> subscales,<br />

except for soothability in infancy was shown (Table 4). Paternal <strong>and</strong> maternal ratings<br />

did not differ by child’s gender.<br />

At the level of <strong>temperament</strong> subscales, <strong>continuity</strong> of father-rated <strong>temperament</strong> was<br />

compared with the <strong>continuity</strong> shown for mothers in Study I. In terms of homotypic<br />

<strong>continuity</strong>, the predictive associations in the father-ratings were similar to mother-rated<br />

<strong>continuity</strong> in the first study. In terms of heterotypic <strong>continuity</strong>, similar developmental<br />

tendencies were also shown, for example, between early activity <strong>and</strong> later aspects of<br />

extraversion, between early smiling <strong>and</strong> laughter, soothability <strong>and</strong> duration of orienting<br />

<strong>and</strong> later aspects of effortful-control, <strong>and</strong> between early distress to limitations <strong>and</strong> later<br />

fear.<br />

Indicators of developmental interaction between different traits were also similar, for<br />

example, between early negative affectivity -subscales <strong>and</strong> lower levels of effortful<br />

control -related subscales <strong>and</strong> smiling <strong>and</strong> laughter. Some difference in the salience of<br />

associations between specific infant <strong>and</strong> middle childhood subscales could also be<br />

detected; for example, father-rated infant distress to limitations <strong>and</strong> fear showed more<br />

salient inverse associations with later effortful control –related subscales.<br />

At the level of latent <strong>temperament</strong> superconstructs, no statistically significant<br />

differences between the father- <strong>and</strong> mother-rated predictive paths between infancy <strong>and</strong><br />

middle childhood were found. Figure 7 displays the predictive paths between mother-<br />

<strong>and</strong> father-rated latent <strong>temperament</strong> superconstructs from infancy to middle childhood,<br />

including the specific effect of infant activity. The developmental links between<br />

affectively positive <strong>and</strong> well adjusted characteristics in infancy (i.e., smiling <strong>and</strong><br />

laughter, orienting capacities <strong>and</strong> soothability) <strong>and</strong> later effortful control were especially<br />

noteworthy in both father- <strong>and</strong> mother-ratings, being in line with the literature on the<br />

52


associations between positive affectivity <strong>and</strong> well functioning, adaptive control abilities.<br />

However, even though no statistically significant difference existed between mother-<br />

<strong>and</strong> father-rated paths, the developmental link between higher infant negative affectivity<br />

<strong>and</strong> a lower level of effortful control in middle childhood was more pronounced in<br />

father-ratings.<br />

Table 4. Mean values of the infant’s (IBQ) <strong>and</strong> 5.5 -year-old child’s (CBQ) <strong>temperament</strong> variables<br />

separately for the mothers (n = 109) <strong>and</strong> the fathers (n = 109), comparisons of the variables within<br />

couples, <strong>and</strong> intraclass correlations between parents’ ratings of their child<br />

Mothers Fathers t (paired) r (intraclass)<br />

M SD M SD<br />

Infancy: IBQ<br />

Activity level 4.54 0.79 4.59 0.69 -0.55 .45**<br />

Smiling/Laughter 5.25 0.82 5.35 0.78 -1.09 .53***<br />

Soothability 4.95 1.03 4.80 1.15 1.09 .25<br />

Duration of Orienting 3.77 1.10 4.10 1.17 -2.59* .46**<br />

Distress to Limitations 2.92 0.70 3.05 0.69 -1.84 .61***<br />

Fear<br />

Superconstruct sumscores<br />

2.16 0.65 2.33 0.68 -2.17* .47**<br />

Positive Affectivity 4.66 0.71 4.75 0.80 -1.04 .39**<br />

Negative Affectivity<br />

Middle childhood: CBQ<br />

2.54 0.56 2.69 0.57 -2.46* .58***<br />

Activity level 4.68 0.78 4.82 0.75 -2.13* .74***<br />

Anger/Frustration 3.81 0.92 3.80 0.83 0.20 .66***<br />

Approach/Anticipation 5.27 0.64 5.06 0.57 3.71*** .69***<br />

Attentional Focusing 5.16 0.76 5.17 0.69 -.06 .64***<br />

Discomfort 3.70 0.98 3.57 0.83 1.50 .65***<br />

Falling Reactivity &<br />

Soothability<br />

5.12 0.79 4.96 0.67 1.96 .46**<br />

Fear 3.63 1.02 3.50 0.84 1.40 .64***<br />

High-Intensity Pleasure 4.96 0.86 4.94 0.80 0.20 .72***<br />

Impulsivity 4.21 0.87 4.40 0.75 -2.82** .77***<br />

Inhibitory Control 5.18 0.87 5.03 0.80 1.48 .46**<br />

Low-Intensity Pleasure 5.81 0.61 5.51 0.56 4.46*** .41**<br />

Perceptual sensitivity 5.32 0.85 5.09 0.66 2.95** .67***<br />

Sadness 3.49 0.84 3.41 0.69 0.94 .52***<br />

Shyness 3.20 1.17 3.24 1.08 -0.53 .81***<br />

Smiling/Laughter 5.86 0.59 5.65 0.66 2.90** .44**<br />

Superconstruct sumscores<br />

Extraversion 4.96 0.53 4.94 0.51 0.67 .85***<br />

Effortful Control 5.37 0.56 5.20 0.49 2.98** .56***<br />

Negative Affectivity 3.50 0.70 3.46 0.57 0.59 .62***<br />

p < .05. * p < .01. ** p < .001***<br />

53


Figure 7. Structural equation model showing associations between latent <strong>temperament</strong> superconstructs<br />

measured in infancy <strong>and</strong> in middle childhood for mothers (n = 109) <strong>and</strong> fathers (n = 109), with the<br />

specific path from infant activity to the middle childhood extraversion superconstruct included in the<br />

model (path coefficients: mother/father). All the nonsignificant paths from infant negative <strong>and</strong> positive<br />

affectivity latent superconstructs are presented.<br />

Note. While the SEM allowing for father- <strong>and</strong> mother-rated across-time paths to form freely fitted the<br />

data well ( 2 /df = 1.3; RMSEA= .05; CFI = .902), so did the model where the across-time paths were<br />

constrained ( 2 /df = 1.3; RMSEA= .05; CFI = .901); the 2 difference test demonstrated no significant<br />

difference between these two models. In addition to intra-<strong>parental</strong> across-time paths, the models also<br />

specified cross-<strong>parental</strong> across-time paths. None of the cross-<strong>parental</strong> paths between the latent<br />

superconstructs were significant, <strong>and</strong> are, for the clarity of illustration, excluded from the figure<br />

3.3 Study III. Developmental transaction between <strong>parental</strong><br />

<strong>personality</strong> traits <strong>and</strong> child <strong>temperament</strong><br />

Preliminary analyses of the concurrent associations between study variables<br />

demonstrated significant agreement between parents’ ratings of the child’s <strong>temperament</strong><br />

that also increased over time, while no association between maternal <strong>and</strong> paternal<br />

<strong>personality</strong> traits extraversion <strong>and</strong> neuroticism within a family was shown. At baseline,<br />

mothers’ <strong>and</strong> fathers’ higher extraversion correlated significantly with higher infant<br />

positive affectivity, rs = .23 <strong>and</strong> .35, respectively, <strong>and</strong> parents’ higher neuroticism with<br />

higher infant negative affectivity, .23 <strong>and</strong> .46, respectively. At follow-up, mothers’ <strong>and</strong><br />

fathers’ higher extraversion correlated significantly with higher child effortful control,<br />

rs = .24 <strong>and</strong> .26, respectively, <strong>and</strong> for fathers also with higher child extraversion,<br />

54


= .24 <strong>and</strong> for mothers with lower child negative affectivity, r = -.19. Mothers’ <strong>and</strong><br />

fathers’ higher neuroticism correlated significantly with higher child negative<br />

affectivity, rs = 42 <strong>and</strong> .35, respectively, <strong>and</strong> for fathers, with lower child effortful<br />

control, r = -.22 (for mothers, this association was nearly significant).<br />

There were significant over-time Pearson correlations between <strong>parental</strong> <strong>personality</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> infant/child <strong>temperament</strong>: mothers’ <strong>and</strong> fathers’ higher extraversion at baseline<br />

associated significantly with the child’s higher effortful control five years later, rs = .24<br />

<strong>and</strong> .20, respectively, <strong>and</strong> the parents’ higher neuroticism at baseline associated with the<br />

child’s higher negative affectivity five years later, rs = .34 <strong>and</strong> .25, respectively. The<br />

infants’ higher positive affectivity associated significantly with the mothers’ <strong>and</strong><br />

fathers’ higher extraversion 5 years later, rs = .29 <strong>and</strong> .42, respectively, <strong>and</strong> the infants’<br />

higher negative affectivity associated with parents’ higher neuroticism, rs = .19 <strong>and</strong> .39,<br />

respectively (for mothers, the association was nearly significant), <strong>and</strong> with lower<br />

extraversion in fathers, r = -.25.<br />

Multi-level analysis: Before proceeding to the multi-level analysis, a 2 -significance<br />

test was performed showing that the cross-lagged paths between <strong>parental</strong> <strong>personality</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> the infant’s/child’s <strong>temperament</strong> were similar in the maternal <strong>and</strong> paternal ratings<br />

(p >.10). The multilevel model including statistically significant paths of the<br />

stability/homotypic <strong>continuity</strong> of <strong>parental</strong> <strong>personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> the heterotypic <strong>continuity</strong> of<br />

child’s <strong>temperament</strong>, <strong>and</strong> the statistically significant cross-lagged paths between the<br />

child’s <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>parental</strong> <strong>personality</strong> over five years are presented in Figure 8.<br />

The model showed an excellent fit ( 2 /df =1.6; RMSEA =.05; CFI =.989).<br />

While <strong>parental</strong> <strong>personality</strong> (ps


neuroticism significantly predicted the child’s higher negative affectivity (p


3.4 Study IV. Transaction between maternal stress <strong>and</strong> child<br />

<strong>temperament</strong><br />

3.4.1 Transaction between maternal stress <strong>and</strong> individual <strong>temperament</strong><br />

subscales<br />

Maternal stress measured in the infancy period correlated significantly with maternal<br />

stress five years later, r = .48, <strong>and</strong> mother’s initially higher stress was also correlated<br />

with the infant’s higher fear r = .18, distress to limitations, r = .31, <strong>and</strong> lower smiling<br />

<strong>and</strong> laughter r = -.15. Maternal higher stress experience in middle childhood correlated<br />

with the child’s higher fear, r = .21, <strong>and</strong> anger, r = .21, <strong>and</strong> with lower soothability,<br />

r = -.18, <strong>and</strong> attentional focusing, r = -.14.<br />

In the SEM, only one infant <strong>temperament</strong> subscale significantly predicted change in<br />

maternal stress over five years: a higher level of infant activity predicted a decrease in<br />

maternal stress from infancy to middle childhood when stress in the infancy period was<br />

controlled for (p


3.4.2 Transaction between maternal stress <strong>and</strong> latent <strong>temperament</strong><br />

superconstructs<br />

The factor structure for the latent superconstructs of positive affectivity <strong>and</strong> negative<br />

affectivity in infancy, <strong>and</strong> for those of extraversion, effortful control, <strong>and</strong> negative<br />

affectivity in middle childhood were confirmed in Study I <strong>and</strong> II. Figure 10 shows the<br />

path coefficients for a transactional model of maternal stress <strong>and</strong> latent <strong>temperament</strong><br />

constructs in infancy <strong>and</strong> middle childhood. The model gave a good fit to the data<br />

2 /df = 1.6; RMSEA = .04; CFI = .92), <strong>and</strong> showed that maternal stress experience in<br />

the infancy period predicted an increase in negative affectivity from infancy to middle<br />

childhood when initial infant negative affectivity was controlled for, but it did not<br />

predict extraversion or effortful control in middle childhood when infant <strong>temperament</strong>al<br />

affectivity was controlled. Infant positive <strong>and</strong> negative affectivity did not predict any<br />

changes in maternal stress experience over time.<br />

Figure 10. A Transactional model of maternal stress <strong>and</strong> infant <strong>and</strong> child <strong>temperament</strong>al superconstructs.<br />

Straight lines are statistically significant * p < .05, *** p < .001.<br />

Note. Non-significant paths have been omitted from this final model with the exception of the path for the<br />

<strong>continuity</strong> from infant to child negative affectivity that controls for the <strong>continuity</strong> in estimating the change<br />

in negative affectivity ( 2 /df = 1.6, RMSEA = .04, CFI = .92)<br />

58


4 DISCUSSION<br />

The current study aimed, first, to establish the <strong>continuity</strong> of <strong>temperament</strong> from the<br />

infant’s age of six months to the child’s age of five <strong>and</strong> a half years as perceived by both<br />

parents, <strong>and</strong> examine whether the developmental paths of infant/child <strong>temperament</strong> are<br />

similar in both parents’ eyes. Second, the study aimed to explore the transactional<br />

development of <strong>parental</strong> <strong>personality</strong> traits extraversion <strong>and</strong> neuroticism <strong>and</strong> ratings of<br />

infant/child <strong>temperament</strong>. Further, the transactional associations between <strong>parental</strong> more<br />

situational characteristics, namely maternal perceived overall stress, <strong>and</strong> ratings of<br />

infant/child <strong>temperament</strong> over this five-year period were examined.<br />

4.1 Continuity of parent-rated <strong>temperament</strong> from infancy to<br />

middle childhood<br />

The present study conceptualized <strong>temperament</strong> as constitutional individual differences<br />

in reactivity <strong>and</strong> self- regulation, influenced over time by the interactions of heredity,<br />

maturation, <strong>and</strong> experience (Rothbart & Bates, 2006). Compared to, for example, the<br />

early stylistic framework of Chess <strong>and</strong> Thomas (1986), <strong>and</strong> the more dimensional<br />

approaches that identify basic <strong>temperament</strong>al traits, like Buss <strong>and</strong> Plomin’s (1984)<br />

genetically based emotionality-activity-sociability model, Goldsmith’s primary-<br />

emotion-based reactions (Goldsmith et al., 1987), or Kagan’s (1994) individual<br />

differences in behavioral inhibition to the unfamiliar, the present conceptualization of<br />

<strong>temperament</strong> also included self-regulative processes that serve to modulate (enhance or<br />

inhibit) reactivity (Rothbart & Bates, 2006). As the arousal <strong>and</strong> intensity of basic<br />

emotions <strong>and</strong> reactions in the child’s life are influenced by the child’s regulative<br />

capacities, defining <strong>and</strong> measuring these capacities supplements the picture of<br />

<strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> the measurement of predictive associations between early <strong>and</strong> later<br />

<strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> adjustment.<br />

Besides depicting <strong>temperament</strong> as a within-person characteristic, <strong>temperament</strong> was<br />

also presented as an interactive construct. First, the child’s expressed <strong>temperament</strong> is<br />

assumed to affect <strong>and</strong> be affected by others; for example, the child’s level of reactivity<br />

may not stay the same during development, but be permanently <strong>and</strong> profoundly changed<br />

by regulative capacities that develop within interaction with the developmental context.<br />

Second, developmental interaction between <strong>temperament</strong>al behaviors that are controlled<br />

59


y separate genetic mechanisms is also acknowledged; for example, the child’s<br />

developing regulative capacities may affect the child’s tendency to express fear or<br />

distress over time, or negative affectivity may also downplay the development of<br />

effortful control.<br />

In the present study, theoretically relevant associations between <strong>temperament</strong>al<br />

dimensions at six months of age <strong>and</strong> at the age of five-<strong>and</strong>-a-half years were shown, <strong>and</strong><br />

the results of the homotypic <strong>and</strong> heterotypic <strong>continuity</strong> of individual <strong>temperament</strong><br />

subscales as perceived by both parents were, to a great extent, similar. Significant<br />

homotypic <strong>continuity</strong> was shown for infant activity level, smiling <strong>and</strong> laughter, distress<br />

to limitations <strong>and</strong> fear (nearly significant in father-ratings), while significant heterotypic<br />

<strong>continuity</strong> was shown for infant soothability <strong>and</strong> duration of orienting in both parents’<br />

ratings. In general, the <strong>continuity</strong> of <strong>temperament</strong> subscales from infancy to middle<br />

childhood fulfilled the criteria of heterotypic <strong>continuity</strong> or coherence, reflecting, besides<br />

over-time stability, also theory-based lawful change (Caspi & Roberts, 2001):<br />

associations of infant <strong>temperament</strong> were found with phenotypically different but closely<br />

correlated attributes, i.e., with attributes belonging to the same <strong>temperament</strong><br />

superconstruct in middle childhood, <strong>and</strong> subscales belonging to the broad categories of<br />

infant positive <strong>and</strong> negative affectivity showed differential developmental paths. It<br />

must, however, be acknowledged, that in Rothbart’s more recent work, a more fine-<br />

grained measure of infant <strong>temperament</strong> has been developed (Gartstein & Slobodskaya,<br />

2003); if this more recent version of IBQ with more subscales had been used, some of<br />

the present heterotypic continuities might even have proved to be homotypic in nature.<br />

As an example of heterotypic <strong>continuity</strong>, higher ratings of infant activity level<br />

predicted, besides higher activity in middle childhood, also aspects of later extraversion,<br />

e.g., approach anticipation <strong>and</strong> high-intensity pleasure, especially in mother-ratings.<br />

However, higher infant activity also showed developmental associations with a lower<br />

level of inhibitory control <strong>and</strong>, especially for fathers, with a higher tendency to express<br />

anger <strong>and</strong> less ability to soothe oneself. In developmental research, a high activity level<br />

has typically been associated with the development of extraversion, but also to<br />

impulsivity <strong>and</strong> poor behavioral control in young children (Hagekull & Bohlin, 1998;<br />

John, 1990; see also Caspi et al., 2005; Eaton, 1994). The present associations are thus<br />

in line with previous research, linking the activity-aspect of surgency with anger <strong>and</strong><br />

60


externalizing behavior (Putnam & Stifter, 2005; Rothbart et al., 2001; Rothbart et al.,<br />

2000b). With the father, the child’s initially higher activity level may have, more often<br />

than with the mother, resulted in manifestations of this less controlled <strong>and</strong> more<br />

negative aspect of extraversion. For example, during a more active, outgoing <strong>and</strong><br />

challenging interaction with the father (Paquette, 2004), even an active <strong>and</strong> energetic<br />

infant/child may be also susceptible to frustration <strong>and</strong> anger.<br />

Higher infant smiling <strong>and</strong> laughter in maternal <strong>and</strong> paternal ratings also predicted<br />

aspects of effortful control, like a stronger tendency to get pleasure from low-intensity<br />

stimuli, <strong>and</strong> higher levels of self-soothing <strong>and</strong> attentional capacities. This sign of<br />

positive affectivity in the infant may be a marker of self-regulatory capacity, i.e. reflect<br />

the developmental change from a largely reactive dimension in infancy towards a more<br />

effortfully controlled behavior in the service of cultural st<strong>and</strong>ards during childhood<br />

(Ahadi et al., 2003; Putnam et al., 2001; Rothbart et al., 2001). Early positive affect may<br />

also allow for the development of self-regulation by allowing more frequent encounters<br />

with experiences that are cognitively <strong>and</strong> affectively challenging, leading to greater<br />

opportunities to develop willful control, or, it may be that infants who display more<br />

positive emotionality elicit guidance from parents that may facilitate the development of<br />

attentional skills underpinning effortful control (Putnam et al., 2008).<br />

For infant fear <strong>and</strong> distress to limitations, that reflect the broad construct of negative<br />

affectivity, especially the associations of infant distress to limitations with a broader<br />

range of characteristics of negative affectivity in middle childhood supported earlier<br />

findings, showing different paths of development from infant fear <strong>and</strong><br />

frustration/distress-proneness (Derryberry & Rothbart, 2001; Goldsmith et al., 1999;<br />

Rothbart et al., 2000b). Further, while higher infant distress/frustration predicted later<br />

fear, depicting heterotypic <strong>continuity</strong> in both parents’ ratings, it also predicted lower<br />

soothability, while initially higher fear, in turn, predicted also fewer signs of low<br />

intensity pleasure <strong>and</strong> smiling <strong>and</strong> laughter in both parents’ ratings. These inverse<br />

associations between early negative affect <strong>and</strong> later difficulties in the ability to recover<br />

from distress or excitement <strong>and</strong> a lack of expressed positive affect are in concordance<br />

with the suggestions of developmental interaction between traits, i.e., that<br />

<strong>temperament</strong>al negative reactivity may compromise the effective development of<br />

61


attentional <strong>and</strong> inhibitory mechanisms, or that early negative reactivity may<br />

compromise the development of expressions of positive affect (Putnam et al., 2008).<br />

Further, the heterogeneity of subscale-level behaviors, reflecting common underlying<br />

processes suggests the value of aggregating across narrowly delineated <strong>temperament</strong><br />

characteristics. Accordingly, at the level of latent <strong>temperament</strong> superconstructs, no<br />

statistically significant differences between the mother- <strong>and</strong> father- rated predictive<br />

paths between infancy <strong>and</strong> middle childhood were found. The developmental links<br />

between affectively positive <strong>and</strong> well adjusted characteristics in infancy (i.e., smiling<br />

<strong>and</strong> laughter, orienting capacities <strong>and</strong> soothability) <strong>and</strong> later effortful control were<br />

especially noteworthy in both parents’ ratings, being, again, in line with the literature on<br />

the associations between positive affectivity <strong>and</strong> well functioning, adaptive control<br />

abilities. These early-emerging tendencies to focus attention, to enjoy calm activities,<br />

<strong>and</strong> benefit from <strong>parental</strong> soothing may also contribute to later regulation of negative<br />

emotions (Gaertner, Spinrad, & Eisenberg, 2008; Gartstein & Rothbart, 2003; Putnam et<br />

al., 2008; Robins, John, Caspi, Moffitt, & Stouthamer-Loeber, 1996; Rothbart et al.,<br />

2001).<br />

Continuity of negative affectivity from infancy to middle childhood was also<br />

consistently shown in father- <strong>and</strong> mother-rated <strong>temperament</strong>. However, for the father-<br />

rated high negative affectivity (i.e., distress to limitations <strong>and</strong> fear) in infancy, a<br />

developmental trend towards low levels of effortful control-related characteristics (e.g.,<br />

inhibitory control, low-intensity pleasure, perceptual sensitivity) in middle childhood<br />

appeared, as also in subscale-level associations, somewhat more emphasized. Here, too,<br />

early negative reactivity can be assumed to compromise the development of effortful<br />

control (Putnam et al., 2008). It has, for example, been suggested that excess crying<br />

during early infancy may hamper the development of self-regulatory skills (Stifter &<br />

Spinrad, 2002).<br />

In developmental literature, it has been shown that the child’s self-regulative<br />

capacities develop <strong>and</strong> manifest in transaction with the caregiving environment (e.g.,<br />

Cole, Teti, & Zahn-Waxler, 2003; Eisenberg, Zhou, & Spinrad, 2005; Feldman,<br />

Greenbaum, & Yirmiya, 1999), <strong>and</strong> some variance in parent ratings of the<br />

manifestations of effortful control has been suggested to emerge due to caregivers’<br />

unique experiences with the child (Putnam et al., 2006; Carson & Parke, 1996; Paquette,<br />

62


2004). Parental agreement of the child’s effortful control in the current study was also a<br />

bit lower than for the other scales, a trend seen in other research as well: effortful-<br />

control-related characteristic are suggested to be somewhat less discernible or more<br />

variably expressed in the presence of different interaction partners (Majd<strong>and</strong>zic & van<br />

den Boom, 2008).<br />

While the child’s <strong>temperament</strong> is the same, where ever the child is, its overt<br />

manifestations may, to some extent, depend on the circumstances. In that respect, it can<br />

be suggested that mothers <strong>and</strong> fathers elicit somewhat different aspects of their child’s<br />

<strong>temperament</strong>al behavior in their individual style of interaction with the child. Due to<br />

these behavioral differences in the contexts of relationships, both parents can also be<br />

“right” about their child’s <strong>temperament</strong>, while the two views may represent somewhat<br />

different aspects of reality, especially in the characteristics of effortful control.<br />

For example, observations comparing father–child <strong>and</strong> mother-child interaction have<br />

found that fathers frequently engage in play behaviors aimed at making the child more<br />

excited (see Paquette, 2004). Hence, with a playfully stimulating father, who may<br />

actively encourage the child’s exploration of the environment, these amplifying<br />

interactions may ultimately reach a limit; the over-stimulation of a more fearful or<br />

distress-prone infant/child may lead to a situation where the child’s control abilities do<br />

not quite meet the challenge. Some children may also find it more difficult to calm<br />

down the emotional aftermath of the situation (Paquette, 2004; Parke, 1994; Rorthbart<br />

et al., 2001). Further, the traditional role of fathers may, in some families, mean that the<br />

father usually interacts with the infant/child after his day at work, i.e., in the evening<br />

time, when the child’s mood <strong>and</strong> control abilities, as well as energy level may vary as a<br />

function of fatigue.<br />

Further, in the combined model of 109 fathers <strong>and</strong> 109 mothers, the predictive<br />

association between high activity <strong>and</strong> later extraversion (c.f. Eaton, 1994) was similar,<br />

but it did not reach statistical significance. However, in the models involving only<br />

mothers (n = 231) or only fathers (n = 115), the paths from infant activity to later<br />

extraversion were statistically significant. This predictive path from early activity to<br />

later extraversion was also shown in the recent study of Putnam <strong>and</strong> his colleagues<br />

(2008), where infant’s higher activity was associated with later characteristics belonging<br />

under the construct of extraversion (activity, high-intensity pleasure, impulsivity).<br />

63


Enriching <strong>and</strong> complementing the current study, the person-centered approach<br />

(Asendorpf & van Aken, 1999; Bergman & Magnusson, 2001) to <strong>continuity</strong> of mother-<br />

rated <strong>temperament</strong> types further showed that <strong>temperament</strong> characteristics in infancy <strong>and</strong><br />

in middle childhood clustered to form profiles indicative of three theoretically-based<br />

<strong>temperament</strong> types: relatively high levels of activity <strong>and</strong> positive characteristics in<br />

infancy <strong>and</strong> a lack of negative affectivity in infancy <strong>and</strong> middle childhood clustered in a<br />

type labeled as “resilient”. High activity in infancy <strong>and</strong> high negative affectivity in<br />

infancy <strong>and</strong> middle childhood, combined with relatively weak child self-regulative<br />

capacities, clustered in a type labeled “undercontrolled”. Furthermore, characteristics of<br />

infant activity <strong>and</strong> positive affectivity that did not rise to the average, accompanied with<br />

an average level of infant negative affectivity <strong>and</strong> with low childhood extraversion <strong>and</strong><br />

relatively high levels of effortful control <strong>and</strong> negative affectivity clustered in a type<br />

labeled “overcontrolled”. These three profiles parallel the three widely established types<br />

depicting <strong>personality</strong> in childhood <strong>and</strong> in adulthood, namely the resilient,<br />

undercontrolled, <strong>and</strong> overcontrolled profiles, respectively (Asendorpf et al., 2001;<br />

Asendorpf & van Aken, 1999; Caspi & Silva, 1995; Robins et al., 1996).<br />

The current results thus provide further support for the <strong>continuity</strong> of childhood<br />

<strong>personality</strong> types by offering a theoretically fruitful way of illustrating how the infant<br />

<strong>and</strong> childhood <strong>temperament</strong> characteristics cluster to form stable <strong>temperament</strong> types that<br />

resemble <strong>personality</strong> types shown in <strong>personality</strong> research. The distinction between the<br />

internalizing (“overcontrolled”) <strong>and</strong> the externalizing (“undercontrolled”) negative<br />

affectivity in the profiles is likely to be related to the level of the infant’s initial activity,<br />

<strong>and</strong> to the activity of approach <strong>and</strong> self-regulation systems. The less intensive, relatively<br />

passive <strong>and</strong> fearful children may, for instance, begin to regulate their fear through<br />

inhibition (Kochanska & Knaack, 2003; van Brakel, & Muris, 2006) which, along with<br />

the originally low activity level, serves to further restrict approach behaviors <strong>and</strong> overt<br />

expression <strong>and</strong> sharing of affect. Inhibition may also begin to attenuate the child’s<br />

positive affect, resulting in internalizing emotions (Derryberry & Rothbart, 1997).<br />

It has also been suggested that extraversion (high activity <strong>and</strong> approach tendency)<br />

gives direction to manifestations of negative emotions in frustrating situations: while a<br />

high level of extraversion may lead the child to increase his or her external force, a<br />

lower level of extraversion may lead to more internalized reactions (Putnam et al., 2001,<br />

64


2008). The different types of negative affectivity are thus assumed to result from<br />

combined initial reactivity <strong>and</strong> developmental interactions between reactivity <strong>and</strong> self-<br />

regulation (Derryberry & Rothbart, 1997, 2001).<br />

4.2 Developmental transaction between parent <strong>and</strong> child<br />

characteristics<br />

4.2.1 Transactional development of <strong>parental</strong> <strong>personality</strong> traits <strong>and</strong> child<br />

<strong>temperament</strong><br />

The <strong>continuity</strong> estimates of infant/child <strong>temperament</strong> reported in the first two studies<br />

still leave room for contextual factors. Thereby, the third study aimed at complementing<br />

our knowledge of reciprocal child-to-parent <strong>and</strong> parent-to-child effects in the<br />

development of <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>parental</strong> <strong>personality</strong> traits. More specifically, I<br />

examined stability <strong>and</strong> change in <strong>parental</strong> self-rated <strong>personality</strong> traits of extraversion<br />

<strong>and</strong> neuroticism in relation to the parents’ views of their child’s <strong>temperament</strong>al<br />

affectivity sumscores <strong>and</strong> activity level from the infant’s age of six months to the<br />

child’s age of five <strong>and</strong> a half years.<br />

The concurrent <strong>and</strong> over-time correlations between <strong>parental</strong> <strong>personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> child<br />

<strong>temperament</strong> were essentially similar in both parents’ ratings, but also showed some<br />

differences in the salience of associations. Both parents’ higher extraversion at both<br />

time points associated with the infant’s higher positive affectivity <strong>and</strong> the child’s higher<br />

effortful control, <strong>and</strong> fathers’ higher extraversion at follow-up also associated with the<br />

child’s higher extraversion, while mothers’ higher extraversion at follow-up associated<br />

with the infant’s/child’s lower negative affectivity. This may be suggested to reflect the<br />

fathers’ more playful <strong>and</strong> exploration-encouraging interaction with the child (Paquette,<br />

2004), that may elicit similar behavior from the child; for mothers, positive interaction<br />

with the child may attenuate the risk of overstimulation-related negative affect. Further,<br />

the over-time associations between infant’s higher negative affectivity <strong>and</strong> <strong>parental</strong><br />

lower extraversion <strong>and</strong> higher neuroticism at follow-up were more salient for fathers:<br />

the infant’s higher negative affect may have reduced the opportunities for paternal<br />

positive interaction with the child, resulting in lower paternal extraversion <strong>and</strong> higher<br />

neuroticism five years later. However, when the differences in the mother- <strong>and</strong> father-<br />

rated cross-paths between <strong>parental</strong> <strong>personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> the infant’s/child’s <strong>temperament</strong><br />

65


were tested, no statistically significant differences between maternal <strong>and</strong> paternal<br />

ratings were shown.<br />

In the final multilevel model, parents’ self-rated <strong>personality</strong> traits extraversion <strong>and</strong><br />

neuroticism exhibited the expected high stability over five years, but also changed over<br />

time in relation to parents’ views of their child’s <strong>temperament</strong>. As hypothesized,<br />

perceiving the infant as higher in positive affectivity predicted an increase in <strong>parental</strong><br />

extraversion over five years. Furthermore, <strong>parental</strong> views of the infant’s higher activity<br />

level predicted a decrease in <strong>parental</strong> neuroticism over five years. Higher infant<br />

negative affectivity was not, however, significantly associated with an increase in<br />

<strong>parental</strong> neuroticism or a decrease in extraversion.<br />

Johnson et al. (2005) proposed that stability <strong>and</strong> intensification of a <strong>personality</strong> trait<br />

may result from a combination of genetic <strong>and</strong> continuing environmental influences.<br />

This goes well with the current results showing an increase in <strong>parental</strong> extraversion in<br />

response to perceived higher infant positive affectivity. Trait extraversion in itself<br />

associates with attention to positive interpersonal information <strong>and</strong> feedback (Uziel,<br />

2006). Living with a <strong>temperament</strong>ally positive child may have acted as an<br />

environmental constancy factor for the extraverted parent (Fraley & Roberts, 2005;<br />

Sameroff, 1995), thus contributing to the stability of <strong>and</strong> increase in <strong>parental</strong><br />

extraversion. The more extraverted parent may also have engaged in a parent–child<br />

interaction that fits <strong>and</strong> strengthens his/her own extraversion-related characteristics<br />

(Scarr & McCartney, 1983).<br />

To underst<strong>and</strong> the predictive association between the infant’s higher <strong>temperament</strong>al<br />

activity <strong>and</strong> a decrease in <strong>parental</strong> neuroticism, the potential significance of the infant’s<br />

activity for the parent–child relationship should be considered. First, children’s activity<br />

level has been shown to be relatively stable from infancy onwards in the current data as<br />

in other studies (Putnam et al., 2006), <strong>and</strong> it may therefore be considered an important<br />

<strong>and</strong> enduring component at the core of the parent–child relationship. This characteristic<br />

of the relationship, stemming from the infant’s constitutional characteristics, is also<br />

beyond the control <strong>and</strong> selection of the parent (Johnson et al., 2005). Second, active<br />

infants have also been shown to be in less need of adult stimulation, with respect to the<br />

development of their cognitive abilities <strong>and</strong> mastery motivation (Wachs, 1987, 1992) or<br />

exploratory behavior (G<strong>and</strong>our, 1989). Thereby, with an initially active infant, the<br />

66


effects that <strong>parental</strong> neuroticism might have on the interaction with the child may not be<br />

able to reduce the infant’s lively <strong>and</strong> initiative behavior towards the parent.<br />

In <strong>personality</strong> research, the closest parallel to <strong>temperament</strong>al activity is perhaps in<br />

the energy or activity dimension within the extraversion factor (McCrae & John, 1992).<br />

Because high <strong>temperament</strong>al activity has thus been shown to underpin later extraversion<br />

(Blatny, Jelinek, & Osecka, 2007; Eaton, 1994; Hagekull & Bohlin, 1998), the child’s<br />

initially higher activity may also be accompanied by higher levels in other extraversion-<br />

related characteristics like positive affect, approach anticipation, lack of shyness <strong>and</strong><br />

sociability (Blatny et al., 2007; Rothbart et al., 2001). In the current data, the infant’s<br />

activity was, likewise, more strongly associated with the infant’s positive than negative<br />

affectivity. These extraversion-related characteristics of <strong>temperament</strong> have also been<br />

shown to be highly stable over the early years (Berdan, Keane, Calkins, 2008). Like a<br />

more active child, a child high in positive reactivity may also become activated at lower<br />

levels of <strong>parental</strong> stimulation (Rothbart, 1986).<br />

On the other h<strong>and</strong>, as Neyer <strong>and</strong> Lehnart (2007) propose, higher neuroticism may in<br />

itself include a high motivation for change. It is also important to acknowledge that<br />

higher <strong>parental</strong> neuroticism <strong>and</strong> related characteristics do not necessarily or<br />

automatically decrease the positive qualities of parenting, such as <strong>parental</strong><br />

responsiveness, tracking, or compliance to the children (Kochanska et al., 2004). It may<br />

be suggested that an infant, seen by the parent as high in activity, may promote positive<br />

qualities in a parent, or offer a chance for change by continuously challenging the parent<br />

to be more active <strong>and</strong> responsive. This process between the parent <strong>and</strong> the child could,<br />

over time, facilitate a change for the better in the parent’s neuroticism-related<br />

characteristics <strong>and</strong> feelings.<br />

One explanation for the lack of effect of infant negative affectivity in increasing<br />

<strong>parental</strong> neuroticism could be that the negative affectivity factor comprises the separate<br />

components of distress to novelty (fear) <strong>and</strong> distress to limitations. These<br />

characteristics, which at the age of six months are not differentiated enough to show any<br />

independent effects, may still differ somewhat in their overt expression, resulting in a<br />

somewhat less consistent effect on feelings <strong>and</strong> behaviors related to <strong>parental</strong><br />

neuroticism (see e.g., Rubin et al., 1999). The existing literature does not report entirely<br />

67


consistent associations between infant’s negative <strong>temperament</strong>al characteristics <strong>and</strong><br />

parenting experience either (Boivin, Pérusse, & Dionne, 2005; Rohtbart, 1986).<br />

The current results further suggest that extraversion <strong>and</strong> neuroticism in adulthood<br />

may differ in terms of their susceptibility to environmental influence (see Fleeson,<br />

2007). In other words, experiences may differentially influence an individual’s<br />

development, because the impact of the environment is mediated by individual<br />

differences in the organism; even though Belsky’s differential susceptibility hypothesis<br />

originally refers to differences in the susceptibility of individuals high or low in specific<br />

<strong>temperament</strong>- or <strong>personality</strong> characteristics (Belsky, 1997), it could also be suggested<br />

that <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>personality</strong> characteristics per se, for example positive or negative<br />

affectivity, activity, neuroticism or extraversion, are differentially susceptible to<br />

environmental influence. Parental extraversion <strong>and</strong> neuroticism may thus differ in terms<br />

of the degree to which parenting experiences in general may affect, i.e. increase or<br />

decrease, the level of that trait. Or, developmental changes in these <strong>parental</strong> traits may<br />

also differ according to specific environmental characteristics, like the child’s unique<br />

<strong>temperament</strong>.<br />

In terms of social investment theory, the parenting role comes with its own set of<br />

expectations <strong>and</strong> contingencies that calls for behaving in a certain fashion (Roberts et<br />

al., 2006a). In this study, the roles that the parents were committed to may have differed<br />

according to their perceptions of <strong>temperament</strong>al differences in their children (see e.g.,<br />

Paris & Helson, 2002; Sameroff & Fiese, 2000; Sirignano & Lachman, 1985), <strong>and</strong> these<br />

roles may have also differently contributed to the patterns of <strong>continuity</strong> <strong>and</strong> change in<br />

extraversion <strong>and</strong> neuroticism. It can also be suggested that parents may change simply<br />

through learning generalization: while in the middle of the everyday interaction with the<br />

child, parents observe themselves acting in a certain way, <strong>and</strong> gradually come to a<br />

different opinion about themselves that eventually generalizes across domains (Roberts<br />

et al., 2006a).<br />

In Costa <strong>and</strong> McCrae’s <strong>personality</strong> model (McCrae et al., 2000), an individual’s self-<br />

concept is considered largely as a function of basic <strong>temperament</strong>al tendencies. The<br />

dynamic interaction of basic tendencies with the environment produces characteristic<br />

adaptations that comprise, for example, social relationships, skills, <strong>and</strong> habits.<br />

Successful coping with parenting situations with <strong>temperament</strong>ally unique children can<br />

68


e assumed to require a permanent reorganization of one’s priorities <strong>and</strong> characteristic<br />

adaptations to the environment: one could thus ask whether these characteristic<br />

adaptations can, over time, moderate parents’ own feelings <strong>and</strong> behaviors, <strong>and</strong> thereby<br />

also gradually modify their more ‘stable’ or basic self-concept, reflected in measures of<br />

extraversion <strong>and</strong> neuroticism. Or, in other words, characteristics of extraversion <strong>and</strong><br />

neuroticism may shift over time along a continuous dimension extending from the<br />

trait’s core to its surface, <strong>and</strong> the core trait may become more unstable due to significant<br />

relationship experiences (Asendorpf & Van Aken, 2003).<br />

In addition to showing evidence of change in <strong>parental</strong> <strong>personality</strong>, our findings<br />

suggest that parents’ <strong>personality</strong> traits may change their views of their child’s<br />

<strong>temperament</strong> over time. We found that parent’s own higher-rated extraversion promoted<br />

higher ratings in the child’s effortful control, while higher <strong>parental</strong> neuroticism<br />

promoted ratings of the child’s higher negative affectivity. These predictive associations<br />

are well in line with the voluminous literature showing that the more positive <strong>parental</strong><br />

characteristics contribute positively to the child’s development, while the less favorable<br />

<strong>parental</strong> characteristics may increase the risk for adverse development of the child (e.g.,<br />

Gartstein & Bateman, 2008; Propper & Moore, 2006; Spinrad et al., 2007; Stright et al.,<br />

2008). For example, parents’ extraversion-related positive affect during play may<br />

stimulate children’s interest in a task or refocus waning attention, as well as create a<br />

positive environment that increases enjoyment of such efforts (Gaertner et al., 2008).<br />

In this transactional setting, parents’ ratings of positive <strong>and</strong> negative affectivity in<br />

infants also showed theoretically relevant heterotypic <strong>continuity</strong> over five years (see<br />

also Putnam et al., 2008). The predictive paths between <strong>temperament</strong> sumscores, i.e.,<br />

the paths from higher positive affectivity in infancy to higher effortful control in middle<br />

childhood, <strong>and</strong> the paths from higher negative affectivity to lower effortful control <strong>and</strong><br />

higher negative affectivity in middle childhood were as hypothesized, <strong>and</strong> similar to<br />

those shown between latent <strong>temperament</strong> sumscores in Studies I <strong>and</strong> II. In addition, in<br />

the multilevel analysis, the infant’s higher negative affectivity sumscore also predicted<br />

lower extraversion in middle childhood.<br />

In recent literature, the construct positive affectivity has been shown to be a<br />

heterogenic composite, associated concurrently <strong>and</strong> longitudinally with both<br />

extraversion- <strong>and</strong> effortful control-related characteristics (Kochanska, Aksan, Penney,<br />

69


& Doobay, 2007). The infant’s own basic predisposition to positive affect, along with<br />

early signs of self-control, may explain the association between positive affect <strong>and</strong><br />

effortful control especially in the parent–child interaction (see Kochanska et al., 2007);<br />

as suggested by Putnam <strong>and</strong> his colleagues (2008), early emerging tendencies to focus<br />

attention <strong>and</strong> benefit from <strong>parental</strong> soothing may contribute to later regulation of<br />

emotions, while positive affect (tendency to smile <strong>and</strong> laugh) may also lead the child to<br />

experiences that promote the development of willful control <strong>and</strong> attention, or elicit<br />

guidance from parents that facilitates the development of attentional skills underpinning<br />

effortful control.<br />

The stability of parents’ views of the child’s negative affectivity over five years may<br />

reflect developmental <strong>continuity</strong> in the child’s negatively tuned reactions <strong>and</strong> behavior<br />

within the family context, due to the child’s own constitutional characteristics, as well<br />

as environmental characteristics (Scarr & McCartney, 1983). The path from the infant’s<br />

perceived higher negative affect sumscore to lower extraversion <strong>and</strong> effortful control in<br />

middle childhood may thus reflect initial difficulties in controlling fear <strong>and</strong> frustration<br />

within everyday situations, or a developmental process where negative emotion <strong>and</strong><br />

attentional processes continually <strong>and</strong> mutually influence one another across time,<br />

leading to a somewhat compromised development of attentional <strong>and</strong> inhibitory<br />

mechanisms in the more negatively affective infant (Gaertner et al., 2008; Stifter &<br />

Spinrad, 2002). At least, the challenge to control him- or herself <strong>and</strong> to approach new<br />

situations may be harder for the easily frustrated or fearful infant.<br />

In this transactional model, the child’s extraversion-sumscore did not contain aspects<br />

of anger-frustration or self-control which, in the other three studies, loaded significantly<br />

on the latent extraversion-superconstruct, presumably explaining the association<br />

between higher infant negative affect <strong>and</strong> higher child extraversion in those three<br />

studies. In this third study, the association between higher infant negative affectivity<br />

<strong>and</strong> lower child extraversion-sumscore may be explained by predictive associations<br />

between infant’s higher fearfulness <strong>and</strong> subsequent higher shyness <strong>and</strong> lack of<br />

extraverted characteristics like approach anticipation <strong>and</strong> high-intensity pleasure.<br />

I also examined whether <strong>parental</strong> <strong>personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> their views of the child’s<br />

<strong>temperament</strong> showed parallel change over time (i.e., simultaneous change in parent <strong>and</strong><br />

child that was not explained by the predictive associations between infant <strong>and</strong> parent<br />

70


characteristics at baseline <strong>and</strong> at follow-up). Obviously, I cannot determine which, that<br />

is the parent or the child, or some other environmental or individual aspect of the parent<br />

or the child promoted the change. However, I found that an increase in the ratings of the<br />

child’s negative affectivity <strong>and</strong> a decrease in positive-affectivity- <strong>and</strong> attention-related<br />

characteristics from infancy to middle childhood were associated with an increase in<br />

<strong>parental</strong> neuroticism, while <strong>parental</strong> extraversion <strong>and</strong> infant/child <strong>temperament</strong> did not<br />

show any parallel change. These associations are well in line with what is reported in<br />

the literature (e.g., Belsky et al., 1991; Kochanska et al., 2004; Propper & Moore,<br />

2006), <strong>and</strong> complement the picture of transactional development between <strong>parental</strong><br />

<strong>personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> infant/child <strong>temperament</strong>.<br />

4.2.2 Transactional development of maternal stress <strong>and</strong> child<br />

<strong>temperament</strong><br />

In the fourth study, maternal perceived overall stress was considered as an important<br />

situational factor that may, besides the more constitutional <strong>personality</strong> characteristics,<br />

influence mothers’ behavior <strong>and</strong> feelings at home. Maternal overall stress also belongs<br />

to the likely significant contextual elements in the early development of <strong>temperament</strong>,<br />

even though the child’s <strong>temperament</strong>al characteristics may, in turn, also act as<br />

significant contributors of stability or change in the level of mothers’ perceived amount<br />

of stress. Therefore, the fourth study aimed to explore the longitudinal relations between<br />

maternal perceptions of stress <strong>and</strong> mother-rated <strong>temperament</strong>al development of the child<br />

from infancy to middle childhood.<br />

Support for the hypotheses that mothers’ experience of overall stress would predict<br />

an increase in mothers’ ratings of <strong>temperament</strong>al maladjustment over time, <strong>and</strong> at the<br />

same time, mother-rated <strong>temperament</strong> traits in infancy would predict either an increase<br />

or a decrease in maternal stress was found. However, these effects were not shown in<br />

the same model, i.e., <strong>temperament</strong> did not influence <strong>and</strong> was not influenced by maternal<br />

stress in the same model. These results essentially complement prior, mainly cross-<br />

sectional, findings on the relations between <strong>parental</strong> stress <strong>and</strong> child behavior, by<br />

extending the period in question to cover the most important developmental phases in<br />

early childhood.<br />

71


Maternal experience of stress proved to be a rather stable mental state from the<br />

infant’s age of six months to five <strong>and</strong> a half years. In terms of changes in maternal stress<br />

over time due to child <strong>temperament</strong>, we found only one significant path from infant<br />

activity level to decreased maternal stress in the childhood period, controlling for the<br />

level of initial maternal stress. This finding that a decrease in maternal stress was<br />

associated with the parents’ views of their infant as high in <strong>temperament</strong>al activity goes<br />

well in line with the finding that higher infant activity also reduced <strong>parental</strong> neuroticism<br />

over this five-years period, <strong>and</strong> justifies the suggestions of the potential significance <strong>and</strong><br />

favorable effects of the infant’s activity for the parent–child relationship.<br />

The more energetic <strong>and</strong> active the child is, the more likely he/she will also be to<br />

continue interaction-eliciting behavior with the mother, regardless of the mother’s level<br />

of stress. Thereby, as in the case of <strong>parental</strong> neuroticism, the effects that maternal<br />

overall stress might have on her behavior towards the child, for example a tendency to<br />

withdraw from interaction (Repetti & Wood, 1997), may not be able to reduce the<br />

infant’s lively <strong>and</strong> initiative behavior. For the mother, focusing on the active child may<br />

serve as an everyday means of forgetting the other stresses of life: these less stressed,<br />

frequently occurring <strong>and</strong> intensive moments between mother <strong>and</strong> child may thus<br />

eventually attenuate maternal perceptions of her overall stress. Further, the more active<br />

child may not raise as much worry in a parent as a child that seems more vulnerable,<br />

timid or fearful does.<br />

It seemed that the changes in <strong>temperament</strong>al development from infancy to middle<br />

childhood originating in maternal stress during the infancy period were more prominent<br />

than changes in maternal stress. Changes in <strong>temperament</strong> included an increase in anger<br />

<strong>and</strong> a decrease in attentional focusing, as well as a decrease in soothability. At the level<br />

of latent <strong>temperament</strong> superconstructs of positive <strong>and</strong> negative affectivity, defined as in<br />

the first two studies, maternal stress in the infancy period predicted a higher level of<br />

child negative affectivity over time when infant negative affectivity was controlled for.<br />

These results are in line with prior knowledge of parent-effects on child development<br />

(Crnic et al., 2005). Maternal stress is thus likely to affect <strong>temperament</strong>al development<br />

in terms of both reactive (e.g., anger) <strong>and</strong> self-regulative functioning (attentional<br />

focusing <strong>and</strong> soothability). Similarly, the increase found in <strong>temperament</strong>al negative<br />

affectivity encompasses both internalizing aspects of child behavior (e.g., fearfulness,<br />

72


shyness, sadness, <strong>and</strong> discomfort) as well as indicators of low self-regulation (low<br />

attentional focusing, low soothability, <strong>and</strong> low inhibitory control).<br />

Earlier research has shown how <strong>parental</strong> stress experiences are related to parent–<br />

child interactions (Patterson, 1983; Snyder, 1991). Repetti <strong>and</strong> Wood’s (1997) notion of<br />

maternal withdrawal from the interaction due to stress is of particular interest with<br />

reference to the current findings. It could be hypothesized that <strong>parental</strong> stress-originated<br />

withdrawal reduces the moments of shared attention between the dyad, moments that<br />

are essential for the development of attention <strong>and</strong> self-regulation (Morales, Mundy,<br />

Crowson, Neal, & Delgado, 2005). In the current study, the predictive associations<br />

between higher maternal stress <strong>and</strong> the child’s reduced attentional <strong>and</strong> self-soothing<br />

capacities <strong>and</strong> ability to control anger/frustration complement these suggestions.<br />

4.3 General conclusions<br />

The current longitudinal study provided evidence for the theoretically relevant<br />

<strong>continuity</strong> of mother- <strong>and</strong> father-rated <strong>temperament</strong> from infancy to middle childhood,<br />

within the developmental psychobiological framework of Rothbart <strong>and</strong> her colleagues<br />

(Rothbart & Bates, 2006). It was also shown that the types characterizing childhood<br />

<strong>and</strong> adulthood <strong>personality</strong> may share common ground with <strong>temperament</strong> types from<br />

infancy onwards. Furthermore, the view that the development of children’s<br />

<strong>temperament</strong> is also sensitive <strong>and</strong> differentially susceptible to variability in contextual<br />

factors, represented in this study by <strong>parental</strong> <strong>personality</strong> traits <strong>and</strong> perceived overall<br />

stress (Belsky, 1997; Van den Boom, 1994; Wachs, 2006), was also supported.<br />

For the adult <strong>personality</strong> traits extraversion <strong>and</strong> neuroticism, high stability over five<br />

years was shown. However, it was also shown that the unique environmental forces<br />

within a family that individually affect <strong>temperament</strong>al development in a child may not<br />

disappear in adulthood: the development of parents’ <strong>personality</strong> traits seemed to be<br />

susceptible to their perceptions of the child’s <strong>temperament</strong>. Similarly, parents’<br />

experience of more situational factors, represented in this study by maternal perceptions<br />

of overall stress, also seemed to be susceptible to experiences with the child.<br />

The continuities <strong>and</strong> associations between parents’ <strong>and</strong> infants’/children’s<br />

characteristics found in this study were independent of child gender. The cross-lagged<br />

paths between <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>personality</strong> were also similar for mothers <strong>and</strong> fathers,<br />

73


fitting with evidence showing that the rank-order stability of adult <strong>personality</strong> (Caspi et<br />

al., 2005), <strong>and</strong> the extent to which various environmental factors affect individual<br />

differences in men’s <strong>and</strong> women’s personalities <strong>and</strong> associated characteristics (e.g.,<br />

Ansell & Pincus, 2004; Costa & McCrae, 1988; Neyer & Asendorpf, 2001) are<br />

unaffected by gender. In terms of stress, the associations between maternal stress <strong>and</strong><br />

child <strong>temperament</strong> were, as in previous studies (Deater-Deckard & Scarr, 1996), also<br />

independent of child gender. While the current study investigated only maternal stress,<br />

similar indications <strong>and</strong> associations can be assumed for both parents’ stress (Deater-<br />

Deckard, & Scarr, 1996; Deater-Deckard et al., 1994).<br />

4.4 Weaknesses <strong>and</strong> strengths of the study<br />

There are limitations to this study that that should be borne in mind in the interpretation<br />

of the findings. First, there were no intermediate assessments of <strong>temperament</strong> between<br />

infancy <strong>and</strong> five-<strong>and</strong>-a-half years of age available: intermediate assessments certainly<br />

would have specified more exactly the developmental paths of <strong>temperament</strong>. For the<br />

third <strong>and</strong> fourth study, examining the developmental transaction between <strong>parental</strong><br />

<strong>personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> stress <strong>and</strong> the child’s <strong>temperament</strong>, one limitation is related to the lack<br />

of micro-level interactional patterns between parent–child dyads that would illuminate<br />

the paths between the predictor <strong>and</strong> outcome variables in a more detailed fashion.<br />

In <strong>temperament</strong> research, the question of objectivity in measuring child behavior<br />

according to <strong>parental</strong> reports is also still open to debate (Kagan, 1994, 1998; Seifer et<br />

al., 2004). Kagan (1998), for example, noted that parents may be prone to form an<br />

unintended but consistent disposition towards the child, although the prior experiences<br />

of the child may not have been that consistent. However, sources of observational error<br />

exist also in artificial laboratory settings, for example errors related to the characteristics<br />

of the rater, or effects of the situation or measure on child behaviors, or interactions<br />

between rater characteristics <strong>and</strong> child behavior (Rothbart & Goldsmith, 1985). Further,<br />

in the more objective assessment settings, only a limited set of behaviors can be evoked.<br />

Especially in the case of positive affectivity, some children could be less likely to<br />

express positive affect in the laboratory, relative to other more familiar settings where<br />

the parents are able to see this behavior (Gartstein & Marmion, 2008; Rothbart &<br />

Goldsmith, 1985). The unfamiliar laboratory setting may also make the child exhibit<br />

74


more fearful reactions than what is representative of his/her general level of fearfulness<br />

in everyday life (Rothbart & Goldsmith, 1985). Accordingly, recent evidence points to<br />

greater predictive validity <strong>and</strong> greater situational consistency of parent-ratings in<br />

comparison to these more objective perceptions (Majd<strong>and</strong>zic & van den Boom, 2007;<br />

Pauli-Pott et al., 2003). Because <strong>temperament</strong> exerts its effect on the child’s everyday<br />

life, Rothbart’s measures also allow the empirical investigation of <strong>temperament</strong> in<br />

various situations <strong>and</strong> social contexts. The possibility of <strong>parental</strong> inaccuracy <strong>and</strong> bias is<br />

further minimized by asking for frequencies of recent, specific, <strong>and</strong> concrete behaviors<br />

in response to common everyday stimuli (Rothbart, 1981; Rothbart et al., 2001). The<br />

items in the infant <strong>and</strong> child behavior questionnaires are also worded in a simple <strong>and</strong><br />

straightforward manner, in order to minimize misinterpretation (Putnam et al., 2001).<br />

The relatively small sample sizes <strong>and</strong> response rates in this study also restrict the<br />

external validity of the findings. Especially, due to the inclusion criteria for family units<br />

in Study II <strong>and</strong> III, i.e., both parents had to provide complete data on all of the study<br />

variables at the baseline <strong>and</strong> at the follow-up, the drop-out rate was relatively high.<br />

Even though the study sample was representative of the initial population,<br />

generalizations should still be drawn with caution.<br />

Further, using the same informant for parents’ as well as infant’s/child’s<br />

characteristics evokes the question of shared method variance. However, the important<br />

advantage of path analysis <strong>and</strong> the analysis of correlated change is that it reduces shared<br />

method variance: because the path coefficients control for the indirect paths, which<br />

contain the full bias, the error is, at least partly eliminated (Neyer & Asendorpf, 2001).<br />

Beyond that, it should also be acknowledged that the causality between parent <strong>and</strong> child<br />

cannot be considered straightforward enough to enable the isolation of any single factor<br />

as the sole origin of a developmental outcome. In other words, the analyses of cross-<br />

paths <strong>and</strong> correlated change in the present study are unable to provide conclusive<br />

evidence of causal effects, because the study designs did not allow us to rule out<br />

alternative explanations, such as the coaction of other, for example genetic- or<br />

community-level -factors, that push the development forward.<br />

75


4.5 Theoretical <strong>and</strong> clinical implications<br />

Characteristics of <strong>personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>temperament</strong> are theoretically <strong>and</strong> empirically related<br />

(Caspi et al., 2005; Rothbart et al., 2000a), <strong>and</strong> closely associated with individuals’ well<br />

being <strong>and</strong> psychosocial adjustment (e.g., Auerbach et al., 2008; Costa & McCrae, 1980;<br />

Eisenberg, et al., 2007; Hampson, 2008; Löckenhoff, Sutin, Ferrucci, & Costa, 2008;<br />

Nigg, 2006; Ramos, Guerin, Gottfried, Bathurst, & Oliver, 2005; Roberts et al., 2007;<br />

Steel, Schmidt, & Shultz, 2008). While no clear consensus yet exists concerning the<br />

exact nature of <strong>temperament</strong> or <strong>personality</strong>, an integrated underst<strong>and</strong>ing of the<br />

constructs, their development <strong>and</strong> associations with environmental influences <strong>and</strong><br />

various clinically relevant outcomes is of increasing interest for both researchers <strong>and</strong><br />

clinicians. Therefore, evidence of <strong>continuity</strong>, as well as every association between<br />

changes in the child’s <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>parental</strong> <strong>personality</strong> is both theoretically <strong>and</strong><br />

clinically meaningful.<br />

In the current study, <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>personality</strong> were seen as broadly overlapping<br />

domains, with <strong>temperament</strong> providing the primarily biological basis for the developing<br />

<strong>personality</strong> (Caspi et al., 2005; Costa & McCrae, 2000; Goldsmith et al., 1987; Rothbart<br />

et al., 2000a). Besides the genetic association between <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>personality</strong><br />

(e.g., Goldsmith et al., 1987), it was also acknowledged that <strong>temperament</strong> develops <strong>and</strong><br />

includes also later appearing elements, which further relates <strong>temperament</strong> to the<br />

construct of <strong>personality</strong>. Temperament <strong>and</strong> <strong>personality</strong> were also considered most<br />

meaningful in social contexts.<br />

Thereby, the results of the current study were in line with the definition of<br />

<strong>temperament</strong> as early appearing individual differences that; first, are relatively stable<br />

across situations; second, are not all visible at birth, but become visible during time; <strong>and</strong><br />

third, are shaped by both hereditary <strong>and</strong> environmental factors. Interaction between<br />

different <strong>temperament</strong> dimensions during development was also acknowledged. Further,<br />

it was acknowledged that the same environment may have a unique influence on the<br />

level of more than one dimension of <strong>temperament</strong> or <strong>personality</strong> at the same time.<br />

For <strong>temperament</strong>, the results of the current study were in line with Rothbart’s<br />

theoretical framework, highlighting especially the developmental coherence of positive<br />

<strong>and</strong> negative affectivity, as well as the interplay between reactivity <strong>and</strong> self-regulation<br />

in growth from infancy to middle childhood (Rothbart & Bates, 2006), from the<br />

76


perspectives of both of the child’s parents. The predictive validity of both parents’<br />

ratings was also supplemented, which further emphasizes the importance of both<br />

parents in developmental research. The confirmatory factor solution, shown to fit the<br />

data well among six to seven years olds in Rothbart et al. (2001), was also replicated in<br />

this Finnish sample. Thereby, as an additional contribution, the current study further<br />

validated the measures <strong>and</strong> constructs of Rothbart <strong>and</strong> her colleagues (Rothbart et al.,<br />

2001).<br />

The individual level <strong>temperament</strong> profiles further illustrated how the common basis<br />

of <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>personality</strong> can be profiled in terms of infant/child <strong>temperament</strong><br />

dimensions. In a more practical or clinical sense, the resilient, under- <strong>and</strong> overcontrolled<br />

<strong>temperament</strong> types showed how the early <strong>temperament</strong> variables may assume their<br />

actual meaning in relation to other variables in an individual’s profile (Janson &<br />

Mathiesen, 2008).<br />

The current study also highlighted the relevance of <strong>temperament</strong> as a relational <strong>and</strong><br />

interactional construct (e.g., Rothbart & Bates, 2006), in which individual differences in<br />

ways of regulating experiences are particularly susceptible to social influences. Instead<br />

of conceptualizing <strong>temperament</strong> only as a set of traits inherent in the child, <strong>temperament</strong><br />

was, thus, also seen as a set of individual differences in the way children regulate<br />

experience (Rothbart & Bates, 2006). In terms of theory-based change or lawful<br />

dis<strong>continuity</strong> (Caspi & Roberts, 1999), the results of the present study were in line with<br />

literature suggesting that the development of infant <strong>temperament</strong>, <strong>and</strong> especially the<br />

characteristics related to the development of negative affectivity <strong>and</strong> attentional <strong>and</strong><br />

self-regulative capacities may be affected, besides genetic influence, by the unique<br />

environment of the infant (Belsky, 1997; Belsky, Goldsmith et al., 1999; Hsieh, &<br />

Crnic, 1998; Gilissen, Bakermans-Kranenburg, van IJzendoorn, & van der Veer, 2008;<br />

Kochanska, 1995, 1997), here conceptualized as <strong>parental</strong> <strong>personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> experienced<br />

overall stress.<br />

For adult <strong>personality</strong> traits extraversion <strong>and</strong> neuroticism, theory-based high rank-<br />

order stability over five years was shown (Roberts & DelVecchio, 2000; Terracciano et<br />

al., 2006). However, despite high consistency, evidence of the possibility of<br />

theoretically relevant non-normative change in these <strong>temperament</strong>-based <strong>personality</strong><br />

traits, due to important <strong>and</strong> enduring interpersonal relations (Neyer & Asendorpf, 2001;<br />

77


Robins et al., 2002) was also gained. More specifically, the current study suggests that<br />

individuals seem to be able to maintain their level of extraversion, perhaps even in less<br />

favorable environments, <strong>and</strong> be able to utilize a chance, at least in the form of the<br />

child’s positive affectivity, to become more extraverted. In the case of neuroticism, <strong>and</strong><br />

also in the more situational experience of life as stressful, this study suggests that<br />

individuals may be prone to detect reasons for decreasing the levels of these less<br />

favorable behaviors <strong>and</strong> feelings (see Neyer & Asendorpf, 2001), while also the risk for<br />

cumulative effects of the more negative parent <strong>and</strong> child characteristics seem to be true<br />

for higher <strong>parental</strong> neuroticism <strong>and</strong> negatively tuned situational experiences like higher<br />

<strong>parental</strong> overall stress.<br />

The similarities found in the <strong>temperament</strong>-associations for neuroticism <strong>and</strong> stress are<br />

underst<strong>and</strong>able, as individuals who score high on neuroticism are also known to be<br />

easily overwhelmed by stressful experiences (e.g., Costa & McCrae, 1992). However,<br />

whereas self-ratings of neuroticism rise from a range of individual <strong>temperament</strong>-based<br />

characteristics, parents’ perceptions of stress may also result from situational factors,<br />

like objectively higher amounts of stressful phenomena in their life. Parental experience<br />

of life as stressful, caused by either environmental or individual-level causes, may still<br />

have a detrimental effect on the proximal processes between parent <strong>and</strong> child, evidenced<br />

here as increases in negative aspects of child <strong>temperament</strong>.<br />

In a transactional sense, the predictive associations <strong>and</strong> simultaneous change between<br />

parent <strong>and</strong> child characteristics detected in this study may have involved both evocative<br />

<strong>and</strong> reactive person–environment transactions that, within a family, may be suggested to<br />

lead to discontinuities as well as continuities in <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>personality</strong>, causing<br />

simultaneous change (Scarr & McCartney, 1983). Or it may have been that parents <strong>and</strong><br />

children proactively created environments that fitted <strong>and</strong> strengthened their<br />

characteristics, <strong>and</strong> these environments further promoted their genetically concordant<br />

characteristics (Scarr & McCartney, 1983). The current study also provided evidence of<br />

how these within-family experiences may differentially influence an individual’s<br />

development, in terms of <strong>temperament</strong>- or <strong>personality</strong>-related differences in<br />

susceptibility of unique environments in childhood as in adulthood. In terms of Belsky’s<br />

(1984) ecological model, the idea of intertwined developmental influence between the<br />

78


three main classes of factors within a family, namely the parents’ own <strong>personality</strong> traits,<br />

perceived overall stress <strong>and</strong> the child’s <strong>temperament</strong>al characteristics, was supported.<br />

Considering the developmental relevance of <strong>parental</strong> views, it may be suggested that<br />

the over-time <strong>continuity</strong> of <strong>parental</strong> views of the child’s <strong>temperament</strong>, <strong>and</strong> the<br />

bidirectional influences between parent’s views of their own <strong>personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> the child’s<br />

<strong>temperament</strong> may be those that matter the most, because this is the intimate <strong>and</strong><br />

enduring environment that is probably the most influential for both parent <strong>and</strong> child.<br />

Whether parents’ individual characteristics shape their subjective interpretations of their<br />

child or not, these interpretations are those that come to life between parent <strong>and</strong> child.<br />

Similarly, <strong>parental</strong> perceptions of situational characteristics like overall stress can be<br />

viewed as a combination of objective stressful events, coping processes <strong>and</strong> <strong>personality</strong><br />

factors that together form the outcome variable of perceived stress, which may then put<br />

a strain on the everyday interaction of the parent <strong>and</strong> the child.<br />

Further, considering the clinical relevance of early <strong>temperament</strong>, a child’s<br />

<strong>temperament</strong> as it interacts with the environment, here conceptualized as the interaction<br />

between parent <strong>and</strong> child characteristics may be a better predictor of developmental<br />

outcomes than <strong>temperament</strong> alone (Rothbart & Putnam, 2002). Given the increasing<br />

evidence of the influence of contextual characteristics upon central nervous system<br />

development (e.g., Nelson & Bloom, 1997; Schore, 2001), it can be assumed that the<br />

proximal processes between parent <strong>and</strong> child could also have an influence on the<br />

constitutional underpinnings of <strong>temperament</strong>al characteristics. Within <strong>personality</strong> trait<br />

theory, it is also acknowledged that an increased underst<strong>and</strong>ing of the mechanisms or<br />

correlates of rank-order change in the <strong>temperament</strong>-based <strong>personality</strong> traits may further<br />

allow the development of effective therapeutic interventions even during adulthood<br />

(McCrae, 2001).<br />

Furthermore, the predictive associations, along with the parallel changes in parents’<br />

self-rated neuroticism <strong>and</strong> overall stress <strong>and</strong> the child’s <strong>temperament</strong> highlight the<br />

importance of paying attention to the cumulative effects of parents’ self-representations<br />

<strong>and</strong> the child’s <strong>temperament</strong>, at least when evaluating the developmental risks or<br />

planning any therapeutic endeavors for a specific family (see e.g. Gartstein & Sheeber,<br />

2004; Martorell & Bugental, 2006). Additional clinical relevance for recognizing the<br />

negative cumulative effects of parent <strong>and</strong> child characteristics comes from the existing<br />

79


esearch showing that especially the more negative or challenging <strong>temperament</strong>al<br />

characteristics in a young child are more susceptible to the harmful influence of less<br />

optimal <strong>parental</strong> characteristics (Belsky, 1997).<br />

However, the present study also suggests that, if offered a strong <strong>and</strong> enduring<br />

enough environmental chance for change, parents’ <strong>and</strong> children’s reasonably stable<br />

psychological characteristics may also be changed for the better. It may be proposed<br />

that extraversion <strong>and</strong> individual <strong>temperament</strong>-dimensions linked with that trait can be<br />

considered as buffering or protective factors in the child’s as well as in the adult’s life,<br />

especially within their intertwined development during the child’s early years.<br />

In sum, the current study showed that parent-rated <strong>temperament</strong> shows moderate to<br />

high levels of <strong>continuity</strong> from infancy to middle childhood, <strong>and</strong> that the <strong>continuity</strong> of<br />

mother- <strong>and</strong> father-rated <strong>temperament</strong> is quantitatively <strong>and</strong> qualitatively similar.<br />

Theoretically relevant links between childhood <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>personality</strong> types were<br />

also shown. Furthermore, this study offered evidence of concurrent <strong>and</strong> predictive<br />

bidirectional associations between child <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>parental</strong> <strong>personality</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

perceived stress. Thereby, this study also emphasizes the importance of paying attention<br />

to the transactional effects of parents’ <strong>personality</strong>- <strong>and</strong> stress-related characteristics <strong>and</strong><br />

views of the child’s <strong>temperament</strong>, especially when evaluating the risks <strong>and</strong> protective<br />

factors for a specific family.<br />

80


REFERENCES<br />

Abe, J. A. & Izard, C. E. (1999). A longitudinal study of emotion expression <strong>and</strong> <strong>personality</strong> relations in<br />

early development. Journal of Personality <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology, 77, 566-577.<br />

Abidin, R. R. (1990). Parenting stress index (PSI) manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment<br />

Resources Inc.<br />

Ahadi, S. A., Rothbart, M. K., & Ye, R. (1993). <strong>Child</strong>ren's <strong>temperament</strong> in the US <strong>and</strong> China:<br />

Similarities <strong>and</strong> differences. European Journal of Personality, 7, 359-377.<br />

Aksan, N., Goldsmith, H. H., Smider, N. A., Essex, M. J., Clark, R., Hyde, J. S., Klein, M. H., &<br />

V<strong>and</strong>ell, D. L. (1999). Derivation <strong>and</strong> prediction of <strong>temperament</strong>al types among preschoolers.<br />

Developmental Psychology, 35, 958–971.<br />

Amato, P. R. (1994). Father-child relations, mother-child relations, <strong>and</strong> offspring psychological<br />

well-being in early adulthood. Journal of Marriage <strong>and</strong> the Family, 56, 1031–1042.<br />

Amato, P. R., & Gilbreth, J. G. (1999). Nonresident fathers <strong>and</strong> children’s well-being: A meta- analysis.<br />

Journal of Marriage <strong>and</strong> the Family, 61, 557–573.<br />

Ansell, E. B., & Pincus, A. L. (2004). Interpersonal perceptions of the five-factor model of <strong>personality</strong>:<br />

An examination using the structural summary method for circumplex data. Multivariate Behavioral<br />

Research, 39, 167–201.<br />

Anthony, L. G., Anthony, B. J., Glanville, D. N., Naiman, D. Q., Wa<strong>and</strong>ers, C., & Shaffer, S. (2005).<br />

The Relationships between parenting stress, parenting behavior <strong>and</strong> preschoolers' social competence<br />

<strong>and</strong> behavior problems in the classroom. Infant <strong>and</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Development,14, 133-154.<br />

Ardelt, M. (2000). Still stable after all these years? Personality stability theory revisited. Social<br />

Psychology Quarterly, 63, 392-405.<br />

Asendorpf, J. B., Borkenau, P., Ostendorf, F., & Van Aken, M. A. G. (2001). Carving <strong>personality</strong><br />

description at its joints: Confirmation of three replicable <strong>personality</strong> prototypes for both children<br />

<strong>and</strong> adults. European Journal of Personality, 15, 169-198.<br />

Asendorpf, J. B., & van Aken, M. A. G. (1999). Resilient, overcontrolled, <strong>and</strong> undercontrolled<br />

<strong>personality</strong> prototypes in childhood: Replicability, predictive power, <strong>and</strong> the trait-type issue. Journal<br />

of Personality <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology, 77, 815–832.<br />

Asendorpf, J. B., & Van Aken, M. A. G. (2003). Personality-relationship transaction in adolescence: Core<br />

versus surface <strong>personality</strong> characteristics. Journal of Personality, 71, 629–666.<br />

Auerbach, J. G., Berger, A., Atzaba-Poria, N., Arbelle, S., Cypin, N., Friedman, A., & L<strong>and</strong>au, R.<br />

(2008).Temperament at 7, 12, <strong>and</strong> 25 months in children at familial risk for ADHD. Infant <strong>and</strong> <strong>Child</strong><br />

development, 17, 321-338.<br />

Belsky, J. (1984). The determinants of parenting: A process model. <strong>Child</strong> Development, 55, 83-96.<br />

Belsky, J. (1996). Parent, infant, <strong>and</strong> social-contextual antecedents of father-son attachment security.<br />

Developmental Psychology, 32, 905–913.<br />

Belsky, J. (1997). Variation in susceptibility to environmental influences: An evolutionary argument.<br />

Psychological Inquiry, 8, 182–186.<br />

Belsky, J., & Barends, N. (2002). Personality <strong>and</strong> parenting. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.). H<strong>and</strong>book of<br />

parenting: Vol. 3: Being <strong>and</strong> becoming a parent (2nd ed., pp. 415-438). Mahwah, NJ, US: Erlbaum.<br />

Belsky, J., Crnic, K., & Woodworth, S. (1995). Personality <strong>and</strong> parenting: Exploring the mediating role of<br />

transient mood <strong>and</strong> daily hassles. Journal of Personality, 63, 905-929.<br />

Belsky, J., Fish, M., & Isabella, R. A. (1991). Continuity <strong>and</strong> dis<strong>continuity</strong> in infant negative <strong>and</strong> positive<br />

emotionality: Family antecedents <strong>and</strong> attachment consequences. Developmental Psychology, 27,<br />

421–431.<br />

Belsky, J., Hsieh, K.-H., & Crnic, K. (1998). Mothering, fathering, <strong>and</strong> infant negativity as antecedents of<br />

boys’ externalizing problems <strong>and</strong> inhibition at age 3 years: Differential susceptibility to rearing<br />

experience? Development <strong>and</strong> Psychopathology, 10, 301 – 319<br />

Belsky, J., Rha, J.-H., & Park, S.-Y. (2000). Exploring reciprocal parent <strong>and</strong> child effects in the<br />

case of child inhibition in US <strong>and</strong> Korean samples. International Journal of Behavioral<br />

Development, 24, 338–347.<br />

Belsky, J., Woodworth, S., & Crnic, K. (1996). Trouble in the second year: Three questions about family<br />

interaction. <strong>Child</strong> Development, 67, 556–578.<br />

Berdan, L. E., Keane, S. P., & Calkins, S. D. (2008). Temperament <strong>and</strong> externalizing behavior: Social<br />

preference <strong>and</strong> perceived acceptance as protective factors. Developmental Psychology, 44, 957-968.<br />

81


Bergman, L. R., & Magnusson, D. (2001). Person-centered research. In T. Cook & C. Ragin (Eds.), The<br />

international encyclopedia of the social <strong>and</strong> behavioral sciences: Logic of inquiry <strong>and</strong> research<br />

design (pp. 11333–11339). Oxford: Elsevier.<br />

Bergman, K., P. Sarkar, P., Glover V., & O'Connor T. G. (2008). Quality of child–parent attachment<br />

moderates the impact of antenatal stress on child fearfulness. The Journal of <strong>Child</strong> Psychology <strong>and</strong><br />

Psychiatry, 49, 1089-1098<br />

Bishop, G., Spence, S. H., & McDonald, C. (2003). Can parents <strong>and</strong> teachers provide a reliable <strong>and</strong> valid<br />

report of behavioral inhibition? <strong>Child</strong> Development, 74, 1899–1917.<br />

Blatny, M., Jelinek, M., & Osecka, T. (2007). Assertive toddler, self-efficacious adult: <strong>Child</strong> <strong>temperament</strong><br />

predicts <strong>personality</strong> over forty years. Personality <strong>and</strong> Individual Differences, 43, 2127-2136.<br />

Boivin, M., Pérusse, D., & Dionne, G. (2005). The genetic-environmental etiology of parents’ perceptions<br />

<strong>and</strong> self-assessed behaviors toward their 5-month-old infants in a large twin <strong>and</strong> singleton sample.<br />

Journal of <strong>Child</strong> Psychology <strong>and</strong> Psychiatry, 46, 612–630.<br />

Bouchard, T. J., & Loehlin, J. C. (2001). Genes, evolution, <strong>and</strong> <strong>personality</strong>. Behavior Genetics, 31, 243-<br />

273.<br />

Breckenridge, J. N. (2000). Validating cluster analysis: Consistent replication <strong>and</strong> symmetry. Multivariate<br />

Behavioral Research, 35, 261–285.<br />

Bridges, L. J., Palmer, S. A., Morales, M., Hurtado, M. (1993). Agreement between affectively based<br />

observational <strong>and</strong> parent-report measures of <strong>temperament</strong> at infant age 6 months. Infant Behavior<br />

<strong>and</strong> Development, 16, 501-506.<br />

Buss, A. H., & Plomin, R. (1984). Temperament: Early developing <strong>personality</strong> traits. Hillsdale, NJ:<br />

Erlbaum.<br />

Calkins, S. D., Bl<strong>and</strong>on, A.Y., Williford, A. P., & Keane, S. P. (2007). Biological, behavioral, <strong>and</strong><br />

relational levels of resilience in the context of risk for early childhood behavior problems.<br />

Development <strong>and</strong> Psychopathology, 19, 675-700.<br />

Calkins, S. D., Hungerford, A., & Dedmon, S. E. (2004). Mothers’ interactions with <strong>temperament</strong>ally<br />

frustrated infants. Infant Mental Health Journal, 25, 219–239.<br />

Carnicero, J. A. C., Pérez-López, J., Salinas, M. C. G., & Martinéz-Fuentes, M. T. (2000). A longitudinal<br />

study of <strong>temperament</strong> in infancy: Stability <strong>and</strong> convergence of measures. European Journal of<br />

Personality, 14, 21–37.<br />

Carson, J. L., & Parke, R. D. (1996). Reciprocal negative affect in parent-child interactions <strong>and</strong> children's<br />

peer competency. <strong>Child</strong> Development, 67, 2217-2226.<br />

Carver, C. S., Sutton, S. K., & Scheier, M. F. (2000). Action, emotion, <strong>and</strong> <strong>personality</strong>: Emerging<br />

conceptual integration. Personality <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 741-751.<br />

Caspi, A. (1998). Personality development across the life course. In W. Damon & N. Eisenberg, (Eds.),<br />

H<strong>and</strong>book of child psychology, Vol 3. Social, emotional, <strong>and</strong> <strong>personality</strong> development (5th ed., pp.<br />

311-388). New York: Wiley.<br />

Caspi, A. (2000). The child is father of the man: Personality continuities from childhood to adulthood.<br />

Journal of Personality <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology, 78, 158-172.<br />

Caspi, A., & Silva, P. A. (1995). Temperamental qualities at age 3 predict <strong>personality</strong> traits in young<br />

adulthood: Longitudinal evidence from a birth cohort. <strong>Child</strong> Development, 66, 486–498.<br />

Caspi, A., & Roberts, B. W. (1999). Personality <strong>continuity</strong> <strong>and</strong> change across the life course. In Pervin, L.<br />

A. & John O. P. (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book of <strong>personality</strong>: Theory <strong>and</strong> research (2nd ed., pp. 300–326). New<br />

York: Guilford Press.<br />

Caspi, A., & Roberts, B. W. (2001). Target article: Personality development across the life course: The<br />

argument for change <strong>and</strong> <strong>continuity</strong>. Psychological Inquiry, 12, 49-66.<br />

Caspi, A., Roberts, B. W., & Shiner, R. L. (2005). Personality development: Stability <strong>and</strong> change. Annual<br />

Review of Psychology, 56, 453–484.<br />

Chess, S., & Thomas, A. (1986). Temperament in clinical practice. New York: Guilford.<br />

Clark, L.A., Kochanska, G., & Ready, R. (2000). Mothers' <strong>personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> its interaction with child<br />

<strong>temperament</strong> as predictors of parenting behavior. Journal of Personality <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology, 79,<br />

274-285.<br />

Clark, L. A., & Watson, D. (1999). Temperament: A new paradigm for trait psychology. In L. A. Pervin,<br />

& O. P. John (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book of <strong>personality</strong>: Theory <strong>and</strong> research (2nd ed., pp. 399–423). New<br />

York: Guilford Press.<br />

Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of<br />

Health <strong>and</strong> Social Behavior, 24, 385–396.<br />

82


Cole, P. M., Teti, L. O., & Sahn-Waxler, C. (2003). Mutual emotion regulation <strong>and</strong> the stability of<br />

conduct problems between preschool <strong>and</strong> early school age. Development <strong>and</strong> Psychopathology, 15,<br />

1-18.<br />

Collins, W. A., Maccoby, E. E., Steinberg, L., Hetherington, M., & Bornstein, M. H. (2000).<br />

Contemporary research on parenting: The case for nature <strong>and</strong> nurture. American Psychologist, 55,<br />

218–232.<br />

Conger, R. D., McCarty, J. A., Yang, R. K., Lahey, B. B., & Kropp, J. P. (1984). Perceptions of child,<br />

child-rearing values <strong>and</strong> emotional distress as mediating links between environmental stressors <strong>and</strong><br />

observed maternal behavior. <strong>Child</strong> Development, 55, 2234–2247.<br />

Conger, R. D., Wallace, L. E., Sun, Y., Simons, R. L., McLoyd, V. C., & Brody, G. H. (2002). Economic<br />

pressure in African American families: A replication <strong>and</strong> extension of the family stress model.<br />

Developmental Psychology, 38, 179-193.<br />

Coplan, R. J., Bowker, A., & Cooper, S. M. (2003). Parenting daily hassles, child <strong>temperament</strong>, <strong>and</strong><br />

social adjustment in preschool. Early <strong>Child</strong>hood Research Quarterly, 18, 376–395.<br />

Costa, P. T., Jr., Herbst, J. H., McCrae, R. R., & Siegler, I. C. (2000). Personality at midlife: Stability,<br />

intrinsic maturation, <strong>and</strong> response to life events. Assessment, 7, 365–378.<br />

Costa, P. T. Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1980). Influence of Extraversion <strong>and</strong> Neuroticism on subjective<br />

well-being: Happy <strong>and</strong> unhappy people. Journal of Personality <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology, 38,<br />

668–678.<br />

Costa, P. T. Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1985). The NEO <strong>personality</strong> inventory manual. Odessa, FL:<br />

Psychological Assessment Resources.<br />

Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1988). Personality in adulthood: A six-year longitudinal study of<br />

self-reports <strong>and</strong> spouse ratings on the NEO <strong>personality</strong> inventory. Journal of Personality <strong>and</strong> Social<br />

Psychology, 54, 853–863.<br />

Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Normal <strong>personality</strong> assessment in clinical practice: The NEO<br />

<strong>personality</strong> inventory. Psychological Assessment, 4, 5–13.<br />

Costa, P. T. Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1997). Stability <strong>and</strong> change in <strong>personality</strong> assessment: The Revised<br />

NEO Personality Inventory in the Year 2000. Journal of Personality Assessment, 68, 86-94.<br />

Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (2000). A theoretical context for adult development. In T. D. Wachs &<br />

G. A. Kohnstamm (Eds.), Temperament in context (pp. 1–21). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Cowan, C. P., Cowan, P. A., Heming, G., & Miller, N. B. (1991). Becoming a family: Marriage,<br />

parenting, <strong>and</strong> child development. In Cowan, P. A. & Hetherington E. M. (Eds.), Family transitions<br />

(pp. 79–109). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Creasey, G., & Reese, M. (1996). Mothers’ <strong>and</strong> fathers’ perception of parenting hassles: Associations<br />

with psychological symptoms, nonparenting hassles, <strong>and</strong> child behavior problems. Journal of Applied<br />

Developmental Psychology, 17, 393–406.<br />

Crnic, K. A., Gaze, C., & Hoffman, C. (2005). Cumulative parenting stress across the preschool period:<br />

Relations to maternal parenting <strong>and</strong> child behavior at age 5. Infant <strong>and</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Development, 14, 117–<br />

132.<br />

Crnic, K. A., & Greenberg, M. T. (1990). Parenting stresses with young children. <strong>Child</strong> Development, 61,<br />

1628–1637.<br />

Crnic, K., & Low, C. (2002). Everyday stresses <strong>and</strong> parenting. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.). H<strong>and</strong>book of<br />

parenting, Vol. 5: Practical issues in parenting (2nd ed., pp. 243-267). Mahwah, NJ, US: Erlbaum.<br />

Crockenberg, S. C., & Leerkes, E. M. (2006). Infant <strong>and</strong> maternal behavior moderate reactivity to novelty<br />

to predict anxious behavior at 2, 5 years. Development <strong>and</strong> Psychopathology, 18, 17–34.<br />

Crockenberg, S. B., & Smith, P. (2002). Antecedents of mother-infant interaction <strong>and</strong> infant irritability in<br />

the first 3 months of life. Infant Behavior <strong>and</strong> Development, 25, 2-15.<br />

De Fruyt, F., & Vollrath, M. (2003). Inter-parent agreement on higher <strong>and</strong> lower level traits in two<br />

countries: Effects of parent <strong>and</strong> child gender. Personality <strong>and</strong> Individual Differences, 35, 289–301.<br />

Deater-Deckard, K., & Scarr, S. (1996). Parenting stress among dual-earner mothers <strong>and</strong> fathers: Are<br />

there gender differences? Journal of Family Psychology, 10, 45–59.<br />

Deater-Deckard, K., Scarr, S., McCartney, K., & Eisenberg, M. (1994). Paternal separation anxiety:<br />

Relationships with parenting stress, child-rearing attitudes, <strong>and</strong> maternal anxieties. Psychological<br />

Science, 5, 341-346.<br />

Denham, S. A., Lehman, E. B., Moser, M. H., & Reeves, S. (1995). Continuity <strong>and</strong> change in emotional<br />

components of infant <strong>temperament</strong>. <strong>Child</strong> Study Journal, 25, 289–308.<br />

Derryberry, D., & Rothbart, M. K. (1997). Reactive <strong>and</strong> effortful processes in the organization of<br />

<strong>temperament</strong>. Development <strong>and</strong> Psychopathology, 9, 633–652.<br />

83


Derryberry, D., & Rothbart, M. K. (2001). Early <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> emotional development. In A. F.<br />

Kalverboer & A. Gramsbergen (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book of brain <strong>and</strong> behavior in human development (pp.<br />

967–987). GB: Kluwer Academic Publishers.<br />

Digman, J. M. (1994). <strong>Child</strong> <strong>personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>temperament</strong>: Does the five-factor model embrace both<br />

domains. In: C. F. Jr Halverson, G. A. Kohnstamm, & R. P. Martin (Eds.), The developing structure<br />

of <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>personality</strong> from infancy to adulthood (pp. 323-338). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Eaton, W. Q. (1994). Temperament, development, <strong>and</strong> the five-factor model: Lessons from activity<br />

level. In C. F. Jr. Halverson, G. A. Kohnstamm, & R. P. Martin (Eds.), The developing structure of<br />

<strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>personality</strong> from infancy to adulthood (pp. 173–188). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.<br />

Eisenberg, N., Ma, Y., Chang, L., Zhou, Q., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. (2007). Relations of effortful<br />

control, reactive undercontrol, <strong>and</strong> anger to Chinese children's adjustment. Development <strong>and</strong><br />

Psychopathology, 19, 385-409.<br />

Eisenberg, N., Zhou, Q., Spinrad, T. L., Valiente, C., Fabes, R. A., 6 Liew, J. (2005). Relations among<br />

positive parenting, children's effortful control, <strong>and</strong> externalizing problems: A three-wave longitudinal<br />

study. <strong>Child</strong> Development, 5, 1055-1071.<br />

Elder, G. H. Jr., (1994). Time, human aging, <strong>and</strong> social change: Perspectives on the life course. Social<br />

Psychology Quarterly, 57, 4–15.<br />

Feldman, R., Greenbaum, C. W., & Yirmiya, N. (1999). Mother-infant affect synchrony as an antecedent<br />

of the emergence of self-control. Developmental Psychology, 35, 223-231.<br />

Fleeson, W. (2007). Situation-based contingencies underlying trait-content manifestation in behavior.<br />

Journal of Personality, 75, 825–861.<br />

Fox, N. A., Henderson, H. A., Rubin, K. H., Calkins, S. D., & Schmidt, L. A. (2001). Continuity <strong>and</strong><br />

dis<strong>continuity</strong> of behavioral inhibition <strong>and</strong> exuberance: Psychophysiological <strong>and</strong> behavioral<br />

influences across the first four years of life. <strong>Child</strong> Development, 72, 1-21.<br />

Fraley, R. C., & Roberts, B.W. (2005). Patterns of <strong>continuity</strong>: A dynamic model for conceptualizing the<br />

stability of individual differences in psychological constructs across the life course. Psychological<br />

Review, 112, 60–74.<br />

Gaertner, B. M., Spinrad, T. L., & Eisenberg, N. (2008). Focused attention in toddlers: measurement,<br />

stability, <strong>and</strong> relations to negative emotion <strong>and</strong> parenting. Infant <strong>and</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Development, 17, 339-<br />

363.<br />

G<strong>and</strong>our,M. J. (1989). Activity level as a dimension of <strong>temperament</strong> in toddlers: Its relevance for the<br />

organismic specificity hypothesis. <strong>Child</strong> Development, 60, 1092–1098.<br />

Gartstein, M. A., & Bateman, A. E. (2008). Early manifestations of childhood depression: Influences of<br />

infant <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>parental</strong> depressive symptoms. Infant <strong>and</strong> <strong>Child</strong> Development, 17, 223–248.<br />

Gartstein, M. A., Gonzalez, C., Carranza, J.A., Ahadi, S. A., Ye, R., Rothbart, M. K., & Yang, S. W.<br />

(2006). Studying cross-cultural differences in the development of infant <strong>temperament</strong>: People's<br />

Republic of China, the United States of America, <strong>and</strong> Spain. <strong>Child</strong> Psychiatry & Human<br />

Development, 37, 145-161.<br />

Gartstein, M. A., & Marmion, J. (2008). Fear <strong>and</strong> positive affectivity in infancy:<br />

Convergence/discrepancy between parent-report <strong>and</strong> laboratory-based indicators. Infant Behavior<br />

<strong>and</strong> Development, 31, 227-238.<br />

Gartstein, M. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (2003). Studying infant <strong>temperament</strong> via the revised infant behavior<br />

questionnaire. Infant Behavior <strong>and</strong> Development, 26, 64–86.<br />

Gartstein, M. A., & Sheeber, L. (2004). <strong>Child</strong> behavior problems <strong>and</strong> maternal symptoms of depression:<br />

A mediational model. Journal of <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> Adolescent Psychiatric Nursing, 17, 141-150.<br />

Gartstein, M. A., Slobodskaya, H. R., & Kinsht, I. A. (2003). Cross-cultural differences in <strong>temperament</strong><br />

in the first year of life: United States of America (US) <strong>and</strong> Russia. International Journal of<br />

Behavioral Development, 27, 316–328.<br />

Ge, X., Conger, R. D., Cadoret, R. J., Neiderhiser, J. M., Yates, W., Troughton, E., & Stewart, M. A.<br />

(1996). The developmental interface between nature <strong>and</strong> nurture: A mutual influence model of child<br />

antisocial behavior <strong>and</strong> parent behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 32, 574-589.<br />

Gelf<strong>and</strong>, D. T., Teti, D. M., & Fox, C. R. (1992). Sources of parenting stress for depressed <strong>and</strong><br />

nondepressed mothers of infants. Journal of Clinical <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 21, 262–272.<br />

Gilissen, R., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H., & van der Veer, R. (2008). Parent<br />

child relationship, <strong>temperament</strong>, <strong>and</strong> physiological reactions to fear-inducing film clips: Further<br />

evidence for differential susceptibility. Journal of Experimental <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 99,182-195.<br />

Goldsmith, H. H. (1996). Studying <strong>temperament</strong> via construction of the Toddler Behavior Assessment<br />

Questionnaire. <strong>Child</strong> Development, 67, 218–235.<br />

84


Goldsmith, H. H., Buss, A. H., Plomin, R., & Rothbart, M. K. (1987). What is <strong>temperament</strong>? Four<br />

approaches. <strong>Child</strong> Development, 58, 505-529.<br />

Goldsmith, H. H., Lemery, K. S., Buss, K. A., & Campos, J. J. (1999). Genetic analyses of focal aspects<br />

of infant <strong>temperament</strong>. Developmental Psychology, 35, 972–985.<br />

Goldsmith, H. H., & Campos, J. J. (1990). The structure of <strong>temperament</strong>al fear <strong>and</strong> pleasure in infants: A<br />

psychometric perspective. <strong>Child</strong> Development, 61, 1944–1964.<br />

Goldsmith, H. H., & Rothbart, M. K. (1991). Contemporary instruments for assessing early <strong>temperament</strong><br />

by questionnaire <strong>and</strong> in the laboratory. In J. Strelau & A. Angeleitner (Eds.), Explorations in<br />

<strong>temperament</strong>: International perspectives on theory <strong>and</strong> measurement (pp. 249–272). New York:<br />

Plenum.<br />

Goldsmith, H. H., & Rothbart, M. K. (1996). Laboratory <strong>temperament</strong> assessment battery (LAB-TAB).<br />

Pre- <strong>and</strong> locomotor versions. University of Oregon.<br />

Graziano, W. G., Jensen-Campbell, L. A., & Sullivan-Logan, G. M. (1998). Temperament, activity, <strong>and</strong><br />

expectations for later <strong>personality</strong> development. Journal of Personality <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology, 74,<br />

1266-1277.<br />

Gray, J. A. (1982). The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of the septo<br />

hippocampal system. New York: Oxford University Press.<br />

Gray, J. A. (1987). The psychology of fear <strong>and</strong> stress. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.<br />

Gross, J. J., Sutton, S. K., & Ketelaar, T. (1998). Relations between affect <strong>and</strong> <strong>personality</strong>: Support for<br />

the affect-level <strong>and</strong> affective reactivity views. Personality <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 279-<br />

288.<br />

Guerin, D. W., & Gottfried, A. W. (1994). Developmental stability <strong>and</strong> change in parent reports of<br />

<strong>temperament</strong>: A ten-year longitudinal investigation from infancy through preadolescence. Merrill-<br />

Palmer Quarterly, 40, 334-355.<br />

Gutteling, B. M., de Weerth, C., Willemsen-Swinkels, S. H. N., Huizink, A. C., Mulder, E. J. H., Visser,<br />

G. H. A., & Buitelaar, J. K. (2005). The effects of prenatal stress on <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> problem<br />

behavior of 27-month-old toddlers. European <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> Adolescent Psychiatry, 14, 41-51.<br />

Hagekull, B., & Bohlin, G. (1998). Preschool <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> environmental factors related to the Five<br />

Factor Model of <strong>personality</strong> in middle childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 44, 194–215.<br />

Hampson, S. E. (2008). Mechanisms by which childhood <strong>personality</strong> traits influence adult well<br />

being. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 264-268.<br />

Helson, R., Kwan, V. S. Y., & Jones, O. P. (2002). The growing evidence for <strong>personality</strong> change in<br />

adulthood: Findings from research with <strong>personality</strong> inventories. Journal of Research in Personality,<br />

36, 287–306.<br />

Heinonen, K., Räikkönen, K., Scheier, M. F., Pesonen, A.-K., Järvenpää, A.-L., & Str<strong>and</strong>berg, T. E.<br />

(2006). Parents’ optimism is related to their ratings of their children’s behavior. European Journal of<br />

Personality, 20, 421–445.<br />

Honjo, S., Mizuno, R., Ajiki, M., Suzuki, A., Nagata, M., Goto, Y., & Nishide, T. (1999). Infant<br />

<strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> child-rearing stress: Birth order influences. Early Human Development, 51, 123–<br />

135.<br />

Huizink, A. C., Robles de Medina, P. G., Mulder, E. J. H., Visser, G. H. A., & Buitelaar, J. K. (2002).<br />

Psychological measures of prenatal stress as predictors of infant <strong>temperament</strong>. Journal of the<br />

American Academy of <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 1078–1085.<br />

Jang, K. L., McCrae, R. R., Angleitner, A., Riemann, R., Livesley, W. J. (1998). Heritability of facet<br />

level traits in a cross-cultural twin sample: Support for a hierarchical model of <strong>personality</strong>. Journal<br />

of Personality <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology, 74, 1556-1565.<br />

Janson, H., & Mathiesen, K. S. (2008). Temperament profiles from infancy to middle childhood:<br />

Development <strong>and</strong> associations with behavior problems. Developmental Psychology, 44, 1314-1328.<br />

Johnson, W., McGue, M., & Krueger, R. F. (2005). Personality stability in late adulthood: A behavioral<br />

genetic analysis. Journal of Personality, 73, 523–551.<br />

Jorm, A. F., Christensen, H., Henderson, A. S., Jacomb, P. A., Korten, A. E., & Rodgers, B. (1999).Using<br />

the BIS/BAS scales to measure behavioral inhibition <strong>and</strong> behavioral activation: Factor structure,<br />

validity <strong>and</strong> norms in a large community sample. Personality <strong>and</strong> Individual Differences, 26, 49-58.<br />

Kagan, J. (1994). Galen’s prophecy. New York: Basic.<br />

Kagan, J. (1998). Biology <strong>and</strong> the child. In W. Damon & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), H<strong>and</strong>book of child<br />

psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional <strong>and</strong> <strong>personality</strong> development (5th ed., pp.177–235). New<br />

York: Wiley.<br />

Kagan, J. (1971). Change <strong>and</strong> <strong>continuity</strong> in infancy. New York: Wiley.<br />

85


Kliewer, W., & Kung, E. (1998). Family moderators of the relations between hassles <strong>and</strong> behavior<br />

problems in inner city youth. Journal of Clinical <strong>Child</strong> Psychology, 27, 278–292.<br />

Kochanska, G. (1995). <strong>Child</strong>ren’s <strong>temperament</strong>, mothers’ discipline, <strong>and</strong> security of attachment: Multiple<br />

pathways to emerging internalization. <strong>Child</strong> Development, 66, 597 – 615.<br />

Kochanska G. (1997). Multiple pathways to conscience for children with different <strong>temperament</strong>s: from<br />

toddlerhood to age 5. Developmental Psychology, 33, 228–240.<br />

Kochanska, G., Aksan, N., Penney, S. J., & Doobay, A. F. (2007). Early positive emotionality as a<br />

heterogenous trait: Implications for children’s self-regulation. Journal of Personality <strong>and</strong> Social<br />

Psychology, 93, 1054–1066.<br />

Kochanska, G., Coy, K. C., Tjebkes, T. L., & Husarek, S. J. (1998). Individual differences in emotionality<br />

in infancy. <strong>Child</strong> Development, 64, 375–390.<br />

Kochanska, G., Friesenborg, A. E., Lange, L. A., & Martel, M. M. (2004). Parents’ <strong>personality</strong> <strong>and</strong><br />

infants’ <strong>temperament</strong> as contributors to their emerging relationship. Journal of Personality <strong>and</strong><br />

Social Psychology, 86, 744–759.<br />

Kochanska, G., & Knaack, A. (2003). Effortful control as a <strong>personality</strong> characteristic of young children:<br />

Antecedents, correlates, <strong>and</strong> consequences. Journal of Personality, 71, 1087–1112.<br />

Kochanska, G., Murray, K. T., & Harlan, E. T. (2000). Effortful control in early childhood: Continuity<br />

<strong>and</strong> change, antecedents, <strong>and</strong> implications for social development. Developmental Psychology, 36,<br />

220–232.<br />

Kurdek, L. A. (2003). Correlates of parents’ perceptions of behavioral problems in their young children.<br />

Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 24, 457–473.<br />

Larsen, R. J., & Ketelaar, T. (1991). Personality <strong>and</strong> susceptibility to positive <strong>and</strong> negative emotional<br />

states. Journal of Personality <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology, 61, 132-140.<br />

Lehnart, J., & Neyer, F. J. (2006). Should I stay or should I go? Attachment <strong>and</strong> <strong>personality</strong> in stable <strong>and</strong><br />

instable romantic relationships. European Journal of Personality, 20, 475-495.<br />

Lemery, K. S., Goldsmith, H. H., Klinnert, M. D., & Mrazek, D. A. (1999). Developmental models of<br />

infant <strong>and</strong> childhood <strong>temperament</strong>. Developmental Psychology, 35, 189–204.<br />

Lengua, L. J. (2006). Growth in <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> parenting as predictors of adjustment during children's<br />

transition to adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 42, 819-832.<br />

Lengua, L. J., & Kovacs, E. A. (2005). Bidirectional associations between <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> parenting<br />

<strong>and</strong> the prediction of adjustment problems in middle childhood. Journal of Applied Developmental<br />

Psychology, 26, 21–38.<br />

Leve, L. D., Scaramella, L. V., & Fagot, B. I. (2001). Infant <strong>temperament</strong>, pleasure in parenting, <strong>and</strong><br />

marital happiness in adoptive families. Infant Mental Health Journal, 22, 545–558.<br />

Losoya, S. H., Callor, S., Rowe, D. C., & Goldsmith, H. H. (1997). Origins of familial similarity in<br />

parenting: A study of twins <strong>and</strong> adoptive siblings. Developmental Psychology, 33, 1012-1023.<br />

Löckenhoff, C. E., Sutin, A. R., Ferrucci, L., & Costa, P. T. Jr. (2008). Personality traits <strong>and</strong> subjective<br />

health in the later years: The association between NEO-PI-R <strong>and</strong> SF-36 in advanced age is influenced<br />

by health status. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 1334-1346.<br />

Majd<strong>and</strong>zic, M., & van den Boom, D. C. (2007). Multimethod longitudinal assessment of <strong>temperament</strong> in<br />

early childhood. Journal of Personality, 75, 121–167.<br />

M<strong>and</strong>ara, J. (2003). The typological approach in child <strong>and</strong> family psychology: A review of theory,<br />

methods, <strong>and</strong> research. Clinical <strong>Child</strong> <strong>and</strong> Family Psychology Review, 6, 129–146.<br />

Martorell, G. A., & Bugental, D. B. (2006). Maternal variations in stress reactivity: Implications for harsh<br />

parenting practices with very young children. Journal of Family Psychology, 20, 641-647.<br />

McBride, B. A., Schoppe, S. J., & Rane, T. R. (2002). <strong>Child</strong> characteristics, parenting stress, <strong>and</strong> <strong>parental</strong><br />

involvement: Fathers versus mothers. Journal of Marriage <strong>and</strong> Family, 64, 998–1011.<br />

McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1987). Validation of a five-factor model of <strong>personality</strong> across<br />

instruments <strong>and</strong> observers. Journal of Personality <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology, 52, 81–90.<br />

McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P.T. Jr. (1994). The stability of <strong>personality</strong>: Observation <strong>and</strong> evaluations.<br />

Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3, 173-175.<br />

McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T., Jr., Ostendorf, F., Angleitner, A., Hebíková M., Avia, M. D., Sanz, J.,<br />

Sánchez-Bernardos, M. L., Kusdil, M. E., Woodfield, R., Saunders, P. R., & Smith, P. B. (2000).<br />

Nature over nurture: Temperament, <strong>personality</strong>, <strong>and</strong> life span development. Journal of Personality<br />

<strong>and</strong> Social Psychology, 78, 173–186.<br />

McCrae, R. R., & John, O. P. (1992). An introduction to the five-factor model <strong>and</strong> its applications.<br />

Journal of Personality, 60, 175–215.<br />

86


Mervielde, I., De Clercq, B., De Fruyt, F., & Van Leeuwen, K. (2005). Temperament, <strong>personality</strong>, <strong>and</strong><br />

developmental psychopathology as childhood antecedents of <strong>personality</strong> disorders. Journal of<br />

Personality Disorders, 19, 171-201.<br />

Metsäpelto, R-L., & Pulkkinen, L. (2003). Personality traits <strong>and</strong> parenting: Neuroticism, extraversion, <strong>and</strong><br />

openness to experience as discriminative factors European Journal of Personality, 17, 59-78.<br />

Morales, M. Mundy, P., Crowson, M. M., Neal, A.R., & Delgado, C. E. F. (2005). Individual differences<br />

in infant attention skills, joint attention, <strong>and</strong> emotion regulation behavior. International Journal of<br />

Behavioral Development, 29, 259-263.<br />

Mulsow, M., Caldera, Y. M., Pursley, M., Reifman, A., & Huston, A. C. (2002). Multilevel factors<br />

influencing maternal stress during the first three years. Journal of Marriage <strong>and</strong> Family, 64, 944-956.<br />

Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998–2006). Mplus user’s guide. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén <strong>and</strong><br />

Muthén.<br />

Nelson, C. A., & Bloom, F. E. (1997). <strong>Child</strong> development <strong>and</strong> neuroscience. <strong>Child</strong> Development, 68,<br />

970–987.<br />

Neyer, F. J., & Asendorpf, J. B. (2001). Personality-relationship transaction in young adulthood.<br />

Journal of Personality <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology, 81, 1190–1204.<br />

Neyer, F. J., & Lehnart, J. (2007). Relationships matter in <strong>personality</strong> development: Evidence from an<br />

8-year longitudinal study across young adulthood. Journal of Personality, 75, 535–568.<br />

Nigg, J. T. (2006). Temperament <strong>and</strong> developmental psychopathology. Journal of <strong>Child</strong> Psychology <strong>and</strong><br />

Psychiatry, 47, 395-422.<br />

Paris, R., & Helson, R. (2002). Early mothering experience <strong>and</strong> <strong>personality</strong> change. Journal of Family<br />

Psychology, 16, 172–185.<br />

Paquette, D. (2004). Theorizing the Father-<strong>Child</strong> Relationship: Mechanisms <strong>and</strong> Developmental<br />

Outcomes. Human Development, 47, 193-219.<br />

Parke, R. D. (2004). Fathers, families, <strong>and</strong> the future: A plethora of plausible predictions. Merrill-Palmer<br />

Quarterly, 50, 456–470.<br />

Patterson, G. R. (1983). Stress: A change agent for family process. In N. Garmezy & M. Rutter (Eds.),<br />

Stress, coping <strong>and</strong> development in children (pp. 235–264). New York: McGraw-Hill.<br />

Patterson, G. R., & Fisher, P. A. (2002). Recent developments in our underst<strong>and</strong>ing of parenting:<br />

Bidirectional effects, causal models, <strong>and</strong> the search for parsimony. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.),<br />

H<strong>and</strong>book of parenting, Vol. 5: Practical issues in parenting (2nd ed., pp. 59-88). Mahwah, NJ, US:<br />

Erlbaum.<br />

Pauli-Pott, U., Mertesacker, B., Bade, U., Havercock, A., & Beckmann, D. (2003). Parental perceptions<br />

<strong>and</strong> infant <strong>temperament</strong> development. Infant Behavior <strong>and</strong> Development, 26, 27–48.<br />

Pesonen, A.-K., Räikkönen, K., Heinonen, K., Komsi, N., Str<strong>and</strong>berg, T., & Järvenpää, A.-L. (2005).<br />

Continuity of maternal stress from the pre- to the postnatal period: Associations with infant’s<br />

positive, negative <strong>and</strong> overall reactivity. Infant Behavior <strong>and</strong> Development, 28, 36–45.<br />

Peterson, J., & Hawley, D. R. (1998). Effects of stressors on parenting attitudes <strong>and</strong> family functioning in<br />

a primary prevention program. Family Relations, 47, 221.<br />

Pedlow, R., Sanson, A., Prior, M., & Oberklaid, F. (1993). Stability of maternally reported <strong>temperament</strong><br />

from infancy to 8 years. Developmental Psychology, 29, 998-1007.<br />

Piedmont, R. L. (2001). Cracking the plaster cast: Big five <strong>personality</strong> change during intensive outpatient<br />

counseling. Journal of Research in Personality, 35, 500-520.<br />

Prior, M. (1992). <strong>Child</strong>hood <strong>temperament</strong>. Journal of <strong>Child</strong> Psychology <strong>and</strong> Psychiatry, (33), 249–297.<br />

Propper, C., & Moore, G. A. (2006). The influence of parenting on infant emotionality: A multi-level<br />

psychobiological perspective. Developmental Review, 26, 427–460.<br />

Pulver, A., Allik, J., Pulkkinen, L., & Hämäläinen, M. (1995). A big five <strong>personality</strong> inventory in two<br />

non-Indo-European languages. European Journal of Personality, 9, 109–124.<br />

Putnam, S. P., Ellis, L. K., & Rothbart, M. K. (2001). The structure of <strong>temperament</strong> from infancy<br />

through adolescence. In A. Eliasz, & A. Angleitner (Eds.), Advances in research on <strong>temperament</strong><br />

(pp. 165–182). Germany: Pabst Science.<br />

Putnam, S. P., Gartstein, M. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (2006). Measurement of fine-grained aspects of<br />

toddler <strong>temperament</strong>: The early childhood behavior questionnaire. Infant Behavior <strong>and</strong> Development,<br />

29, 386–401.<br />

Putnam, S. P., Rothbart, M. K., & Gartstein, M. A. (2008). Homotypic <strong>and</strong> heterotypic <strong>continuity</strong> of<br />

fine-grained <strong>temperament</strong> during infancy, toddlerhood, <strong>and</strong> early childhood. Infant <strong>and</strong> <strong>Child</strong><br />

Development, 17, 387-405.<br />

87


Putnam, S. P., Sanson, A. V., & Rothbart, M. K. (2002). <strong>Child</strong> <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> parenting. In M.<br />

H.Bornstein (Ed.), H<strong>and</strong>book of parenting, Vol. 1 <strong>Child</strong>ren <strong>and</strong> parenting (2nd ed., pp. 255-277).<br />

Mahwah, NJ, US: Erlbaum.<br />

Putnam, S. P., Stifter, C. A. (2005). Behavioral approach–inhibition in toddlers: prediction from infancy,<br />

positive <strong>and</strong> negative affective components, <strong>and</strong> relations with behavior problems. <strong>Child</strong><br />

Development, 76, 212-226.<br />

Ramch<strong>and</strong>ani, P., Stein, A., & Evans, J. (2005). Paternal depression in the postnatal period <strong>and</strong> child<br />

development: A prospective population study. Lancet, 365, 2201–2205.<br />

Ramos, M. C., Guerin, D. W., Gottfried, A. W., Bathurst, K., & Oliver, P. H. (2005). Family Conflict <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>Child</strong>ren's Behavior Problems: The Moderating Role of <strong>Child</strong> Temperament. Structural Equation<br />

Modeling, 12, 278-298.<br />

Rantanen, J., Kinnunen, U., Feldt, T., & Pulkkinen, L. (2008). Work-family conflict <strong>and</strong> psychological<br />

well-being: Stability <strong>and</strong> cross-lagged relations within one- <strong>and</strong> six-year follow-ups. Journal of<br />

Vocational Behavior, 73, 37-51.<br />

Repetti, R. L., & Wood, J. (1997). Effects of daily stress at work on mothers’ interaction with<br />

preschoolers. Journal of Family Psychology, 11, 90–108.<br />

Roberts, B. W. (1997). Plaster or plasticity: Are work experiences associated with <strong>personality</strong> change in<br />

women? Journal of Personality, 65, 205–232.<br />

Roberts, B.W., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2003).Work experiences <strong>and</strong> <strong>personality</strong> development in<br />

young adulthood. Journal of Personality <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology, 84, 582–593.<br />

Roberts, B.W., & Chapman, C. N. (2000). Change in dispositional well-being <strong>and</strong> its relation to role<br />

quality: A 30-year longitudinal study. Journal of Research in Personality, 34, 26–41<br />

Roberts, B. W., & DelVecchio, W. F. (2000). The rank-order consistency of <strong>personality</strong> traits from<br />

childhood to old age: A quantitative review of longitudinal studies. Psychological Bulletin, 126,<br />

3-25.<br />

Roberts, B. W., Kuncel, N. R., Shiner, R., Caspi, A., & Goldberg, L. R. (2007). The power of <strong>personality</strong>:<br />

The comparative validity of <strong>personality</strong> traits, socioeconomic status, <strong>and</strong> cognitive ability for<br />

predicting important life outcomes. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 313-345.<br />

Roberts, B. W., & Mroczek, D. (2008). Personality trait change in adulthood. Current Directions in<br />

Psychological Science, 17, 31-35.<br />

Roberts, B. W., Walton, K., Bogg, T., & Caspi, A. (2006a). De-investment in work <strong>and</strong> non-normative<br />

<strong>personality</strong> trait change in young adulthood. European Journal of Personality, 20, 461-474.<br />

Roberts, B. W., Walton, K. E., & Viechtbauer, W. (2006b). Patterns of mean-level change in<br />

<strong>personality</strong> traits across the life course: A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychological<br />

Bulletin, 132, 1–25.<br />

Roberts, B.W., Wood, D., & Smith, J. L. (2005). Evaluating five factor theory <strong>and</strong> social investment<br />

perspectives on <strong>personality</strong> trait development. Journal of Research in Personality, 39, 166–184.<br />

Robins, R. W., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2002). It’s not just who you’re with, it’s who you are:<br />

Personality <strong>and</strong> relationship experiences across multiple relationships. Journal of Personality, 70,<br />

925–964.<br />

Robins, R. W., John, O. P., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1996). Resilient,<br />

overcontrolled, <strong>and</strong> undercontrolled boys: Three replicable <strong>personality</strong> types. Journal of Personality<br />

<strong>and</strong> Social Psychology, 70, 157-171.<br />

Robins, R. W., & Tracy, J. L. (2003). Setting an agenda for a person-centered approach to <strong>personality</strong><br />

development. Monographs of the Society for Research in child Development, 68, 110–122.<br />

Robinson, M., Oddy, W. H., Li, J., Kendall, G., de Klerk, N. H., Silburn, S. R., Zubrick, S. R., Newnham,<br />

J. P., Stanley, F. J., & Mattes, E. (2008). Pre- <strong>and</strong> postnatal influences on preschool mental health: a<br />

large-scale cohort study. Journal of <strong>Child</strong> Psychology <strong>and</strong> Psychiatry 49, 1118–1128.<br />

Rothbart, M. K. (1989). Temperament <strong>and</strong> development. In G. A. Kohnstamm, J. E. Bates, & M. K.<br />

Rothbart (Eds.), Temperament in childhood (pp.187–248). New York: Wiley.<br />

Rothbart, M. K. (1986). Longitudinal observation of infant <strong>temperament</strong>. Developmental Psychology, 22,<br />

356–365.<br />

Rothbart, M. K. (1981). Measurement of <strong>temperament</strong> in infancy. <strong>Child</strong> Development, 52, 569–578.<br />

Rothbart, M. K., & Ahadi, S. A. (1994). Temperament <strong>and</strong> the development of <strong>personality</strong>. Journal of<br />

Abnormal Psychology, 103, 55-66.<br />

Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., & Evans, D. E. (2000a). Temperament <strong>and</strong> <strong>personality</strong>: Origins <strong>and</strong><br />

outcomes. Journal of Personality <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology, 78, 122–135.<br />

88


Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., Hershey, K. L., & Fisher, P. (2001). Investigations of <strong>temperament</strong> at three<br />

to seven years: The <strong>Child</strong>ren’s Behavior Questionnaire. <strong>Child</strong> Development, 72, 1394–1408.<br />

Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. (2006). Temperament. In W. Damon (Series Ed), & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed),<br />

H<strong>and</strong>book of child psychology: Social emotional <strong>and</strong> <strong>personality</strong> development (Vol. 3, 6th ed., pp.<br />

99–166). New York: Wiley.<br />

Rothbart, M. K., & Derryberry, D. (1981). Development of individual differences in <strong>temperament</strong>. In M.<br />

E. Lamb & A. L. Brown (Eds.), Advances in developmental psychology (pp. 37–86). Hillsdale, NJ:<br />

Erlbaum.<br />

Rothbart, M. K., Derryberry, D., & Hershey, K. (2000b). Stability of <strong>temperament</strong> in childhood:<br />

Laboratory infant assessment to parent report at seven years. In V. J. Molfese & D. L. Molfese<br />

(Eds.), Temperament <strong>and</strong> <strong>personality</strong> development across the life span (pp. 85–119). Hillsdale, NJ:<br />

Erlbaum.<br />

Rothbart, M. K., Derryberry, D., & Posner, M. I. (1994). A psychobiological approach to the<br />

development of <strong>temperament</strong>. In J. E. Bates & T. D. Wachs (Eds.), Temperament: Individual<br />

differences at the interface of biology <strong>and</strong> behavior (pp. 83–116). Washington, DC: American<br />

Psychological Association.<br />

Rothbart, M. K., & Goldsmith, H. H. (1985). Three approaches to the study of infant<br />

<strong>temperament</strong>. Developmental Review, 5, 237-260.<br />

Rothbart, M. K., & Putnam, S. P. (2002). Temperament <strong>and</strong> socialization. In L. Pulkkinen & A. Caspi<br />

(Eds.), Paths to successful development: Personality in the life course (pp. 19–45). Cambridge,<br />

Engl<strong>and</strong>: Cambridge University Press.<br />

Rubin, K. H., Nelson, L. J., Hastings, P., & Asendorpf, J. (1999). The transaction between parents’<br />

perceptions of their children’s shyness <strong>and</strong> their parenting styles. International Journal of<br />

Behavioral Development, 23, 937–958.<br />

Räikkönen, K., Pesonen, A-K., Heinonen, K., Komsi, N., Järvenpää, A.-L., & Str<strong>and</strong>berg, T. E. (2006).<br />

Stressed parents: A dyadic perspective on perceived infant <strong>temperament</strong>. Infant <strong>and</strong> <strong>Child</strong><br />

Development, 15, 75–87.<br />

Sameroff, A. J. (1995). General systems theories <strong>and</strong> developmental psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti, &<br />

D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology (pp. 659–695). New York: Wiley.<br />

Sameroff, A. J., & Fiese, B. H. (2000). Models of development <strong>and</strong> developmental risk. In C. H. Jr.<br />

Zeanah (Ed.), H<strong>and</strong>book of infant mental health (2nd ed., pp. 3–19). New York, NY, US: Guilford<br />

Press.<br />

Saudino, K. J., Wertz, A. E., & Gagne, J. R. (2004). Night <strong>and</strong> day: Are siblings as different in<br />

<strong>temperament</strong> as parents say they are? Journal of Personality <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology, 87, 698–706.<br />

Scarr, S. (1992). Developmental theories for the 1990s: Development <strong>and</strong> individual differences. <strong>Child</strong><br />

Development, 63, 1–19.<br />

Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their own environments: A theory of genotype to<br />

environment effects. <strong>Child</strong> Development, 54, 424–435.<br />

Schore, A. N. (2001). The effects of a secure attachment relationship on right brain development, affect<br />

regulation, <strong>and</strong> infant mental health. Infant Mental Health Journal, 22, 7–66.<br />

Scollon, C. N., & Diener, E. (2006). Love, work, <strong>and</strong> changes in extraversion <strong>and</strong> neuroticism over time.<br />

Journal of Personality <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology, 91, 1152–1165.<br />

Seifer, R., Sameroff, A., Dickstein, S., Schiller, M., & Hayden, L. C. (2004). Your own children are<br />

special: Clues to the sources of reporting bias in <strong>temperament</strong> assessments. Infant Behavior &<br />

Development, 27, 323–341.<br />

Shannon, J. D., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Cabrera, N. J. (2006). Fathering in infancy: Mutuality <strong>and</strong><br />

stability between 8 <strong>and</strong> 16 months. Parenting: Science <strong>and</strong> Practice, 6, 167–188.<br />

Shipley, B. A., Weiss, A., Der, G., Taylor, M. D., & Deary, I. J. (2007). Neuroticism, extraversion, <strong>and</strong><br />

mortality in the UK Health <strong>and</strong> Lifestyle Survey: A 21-Year prospective cohort study. Psychosomatic<br />

Medicine, 69, 923-931.<br />

Sirignano, S. W., & Lachman, M. E. (1985). Personality change during the transition to parenthood: The<br />

role of perceived infant <strong>temperament</strong>. Developmental Psychology, 21, 558–567.<br />

Smits, D. J. M., & Boeck, P. D. (2006). From BIS/BAS to the Big Five. European Journal of Personality,<br />

20, 255-270.<br />

Snyder, J. (1991). Discipline as a mediator of the impact of maternal stress <strong>and</strong> mood on child conduct<br />

problems. Development <strong>and</strong> Psychopathology, 3, 263–276.<br />

89


Spinrad, T. L., Eisenberg, N., Cumberl<strong>and</strong>, A., Fabes, R. A., Valiente, C., Shepard, S. A., Reiser, M.,<br />

Losoya, S. H., & Guthrie, I. K. (2006). Relation of emotion-related regulation to children's social<br />

competence: A longitudinal study. Emotion, 6, 498-510.<br />

Steel, P., Schmidt, J., & Shultz, J. (2008). Refining the relationship between <strong>personality</strong> <strong>and</strong> subjective<br />

well-being. Psychological Bulletin,134, 138-161.<br />

Stifter, C. A., & Spinrad, T. L. (2002). The effect of excessive crying on the development of emotion<br />

regulation. Infancy, 3, 133–152.<br />

Stifter, C. A., Willoughby, M. T., & Towe-Goodman, N. (2008). Agree or agree to disagree? Assessing<br />

the convergence between parents <strong>and</strong> observers on infant <strong>temperament</strong>. Infant <strong>and</strong> <strong>Child</strong><br />

Development, 17, 407-426.<br />

Stranberg, T. E., Järvenpää, A.-L., Vanhanen, H., & McKeigue, P. M. (2001). Birth outcome in relation to<br />

licorice consumption during pregnancy. American Journal of Epidemiology, 153, 1085–1088.<br />

Stright, A. D., Gallagher, K. C. Kelley, K. (2008). Infant <strong>temperament</strong> moderates relations between<br />

maternal parenting in early childhood <strong>and</strong> children's adjustment in first grade. <strong>Child</strong> Development,<br />

79, 186-200.<br />

Tennen, H., Affleck, G., & Armeli, S. (2005). Personality <strong>and</strong> daily experience revisited. Journal of<br />

Personality, 73, 1–19.<br />

Terracciano, A., Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (2006). Personality plasticity after age 30. Personality<br />

<strong>and</strong> Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 999–1009.<br />

Terracciano, A., McCrae, R. R., Brant, L. J., & Costa, P. T. Jr. (2005). Hierarchical linear modeling<br />

analyses of the NEO-PI-R scales in the Baltimore longitudinal study of aging. Psychology <strong>and</strong> Aging,<br />

20, 493–506.<br />

Uziel, L. (2006). The extraverted <strong>and</strong> the neurotic glasses are of different colors. Personality <strong>and</strong><br />

Individual Differences, 41, 745–754.<br />

Vaidya, J. G., Gray, E. K., & Haig, J. (2002). On the temporal stability of <strong>personality</strong>: Evidence for<br />

differential stability <strong>and</strong> the role of life experiences. Journal of Personality <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology,<br />

83, 1469–1484.<br />

Van Aken, M. A. G., Denissen, J. J. A., Branje, S. J. T., Dubas, J. S., & Goossens, L. (2006). Midlife<br />

concerns <strong>and</strong> short-term <strong>personality</strong> change in middle adulthood. European Journal of Personality,<br />

20, 497–513<br />

van Brakel, A. M. L., & Muris, P. (2006). A brief scale for measuring 'Behavioral inhibition to the<br />

unfamiliar’ in children. Journal of Psychopathology <strong>and</strong> Behavioral Assessment, 28, 79-84<br />

Van Egeren, L. A. (2004). The Development of the coparenting relationship over the transition to<br />

parenthood. Infant Mental Health Journal, 25, 453–477.<br />

Van den Boom, D. C. (1989). Neonatal irritability <strong>and</strong> the development of attachment. In G. A.<br />

Kohnstamm, J. E. Bates, & M. K. Rothbart (Eds.), Temperament in childhood (pp. 299–318). Oxford:<br />

John Wiley & Sons.<br />

Van den Boom, D. C. (1994). The influence of <strong>temperament</strong> <strong>and</strong> mothering on attachment <strong>and</strong><br />

exploration: An experimental manipulation of sensitive responsiveness among lower-class mothers<br />

with irritable infants. <strong>Child</strong> Development, 65, 1457–1477.<br />

Verhoeven, M., Junger, M., Van Aken, C., Dekovic, M., & Van Aken, M. A. G. (2007). Parenting during<br />

toddlerhood: Contributions of <strong>parental</strong>, contextual, <strong>and</strong> child characteristics. Journal of Family<br />

Issues, 28, 1663-1691.<br />

Wachs, T. D. (1992). The nature of nurture. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.<br />

Wachs, T. D. (1987). Specificity of environmental action as manifest in environmental correlates of<br />

infant’s mastery motivation. Developmental psychology, 23, 782–790.<br />

Wachs, T. D. (2006). The nature, etiology, <strong>and</strong> consequences of individual differences in<br />

<strong>temperament</strong>. In L. Balter, & C. S. Tamis-LeMonda (Eds.), <strong>Child</strong> psychology: A h<strong>and</strong>book of<br />

contemporary issues (2nd ed., pp. 27–52). New York: Psychology Press.<br />

Watson, D., Suls, J., & Haig, J. (2002). Global self-esteem in relation to structural models of <strong>personality</strong><br />

<strong>and</strong> affectivity. Journal of Personality <strong>and</strong> Social Psychology, 83, 185–197.<br />

Widiger, T. A., Costa, P. T. Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (2002). A proposal for Axis II: Diagnosing <strong>personality</strong><br />

disorders using the five-factor model. In: P. T. Jr.; Costa, & T. A. Widiger (Eds), Personality<br />

disorders <strong>and</strong> the five-factor model of <strong>personality</strong> (2nd ed., pp. 431-456). Washington, DC, US:<br />

American Psychological Association.<br />

Worobey, J., & Blajda, V. M. (1989). Temperament ratings at 2 weeks, 2 months, <strong>and</strong> 1 year: Differential<br />

stability of activity <strong>and</strong> emotionality. Developmental Psychology, 25, 257–263.<br />

90


Yik, M. S. M., & Russell, J.A. (2001). Predicting the Big Two of affect from the Big Five of <strong>personality</strong>.<br />

Journal of Research in Personality, 35, 247-277.<br />

Östberg, M., Hagekull, B., & Hagelin, E. (2007). Stability <strong>and</strong> prediction of parenting stress. Infant <strong>and</strong><br />

<strong>Child</strong> Development, 16, 207-223.<br />

91

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!