Preaspiration in the Nordic Languages: Synchronic and Diachronic ...

Preaspiration in the Nordic Languages: Synchronic and Diachronic ... Preaspiration in the Nordic Languages: Synchronic and Diachronic ...

www2.lingfil.uu.se
from www2.lingfil.uu.se More from this publisher
26.07.2013 Views

For word-medial contexts data are available from all four subjects. In VC syllables, the three subjects who produced unscripted speech have mean preaspiration durations ranging from 8 to 11 ms, while subject ES has a mean duration of 19 ms. In VC syllables the unscripted speech subjects have mean preaspiration durations of 8–17 ms, which are shorter than ES’s mean duration of 26 ms. As regards voicing conditions preceding the occlusion phase in sequences of nasal + stop, there is little indication of voiceless nasality. Therefore, no analysis of the timing relations in voice offset and stop occlusion is provided here. 4.5.3.6 Summary and discussion It was shown in section 4.2.4 that the Central Standard Swedish (CSw) speakers have a much greater difference in duration between word-initial and word-medial postaspirations than the Western Åland subjects. In the CSw data (cf. section 4.2), word-initial stops (tennis type words) have mean postaspiration durations ranging from 44 to 58 ms. Word-medial stops (båten and vatten type words) have mean postaspiration durations of 20–32 ms. In a third group of words, the taverna type, which have a word-initial stop in a lexically unstressed syllable, the initial stop has mean postaspiration durations ranging from 40 to 58 ms. 1 In CSw, therefore, word-initial position as such induces postaspiration in stop consonants (cf. discussion in 4.2.4). The fact that a syllable is unstressed does not, in itself, lead to unaspirated stop production. In the Western Åland data, we observe relatively small differences between postaspiration in word-initial and word-medial stops, at least for the unscripted speech data. Certainly, these differences are much smaller than in CSw. Unlike CSw, these durational differences in the Western Åland dialect can be attributed to stress differences. In fact, the duration of word-medial postaspiration in the Western Åland data seems to be directly comparable to that in CSw taverna type words, i.e. word-initial stops in unstressed syllables. 1 Words referred to as taverna type words in the CSw data occur very infrequently in the Western Åland data. Therefore, such words are not discussed specifically for the Western Åland dialect. – 200 –

The results from the Western Åland durational measurements are in accordance with the auditory impression that ON word-medial p, t, k are reflected as postaspirated stops in the Western Åland dialect. Auditorily, the word-medial postaspirations in the speech of subjects AJ and NK appear to be “stronger” or more “prominent” than those of Northern Icelandic. The postaspirations of subjects MG and ES are more similar to what one normally hears in the Northern Icelandic dialect. Considering the fact that both AJ and NK generally produce shorter postaspirations than MG, it may seem strange that their postaspirations could be more auditorily salient. However, as noted above, voice offset and onset times are unlikely to be the only determinants of the prominence of the aspiration percept. Thus it is possible that AJ and NK utilise some means besides modal voice onset time to achieve the aspiration percept to a greater degree than does MG. In general, ES’s postaspiration durations are shorter than for the remaining three Western Åland subjects, despite the fact that ES’s data are from read speech in contrast to the data for MG, AJ and NK that come from unscripted spontaneous speech. The difference is stronger in the duration of word-medial intervocalic stops than in the other syllable types, and, in fact, many of the word-medial stops do not yield a strong aspiration percept. The impression that ES produces stops in a way similar to Northern Icelandic speakers may have to do with the fact that her postaspiration durations in VC syllables are shorter than those in other contexts, and that she tends to produce preaspirated variants instead, or to have both preaspiration and postaspiration simultaneously. Since the Northern Icelandic dialect has preaspiration in VC syllables, whereas postaspiration is the rule in other contexts, ES comes much closer to the Northern Icelandic norm than do the other Western Åland subjects. The production of simultaneous preaspiration and postaspiration, especially notable in ES’s VCV sequences, seems to be typologically unusual. In ES’s speech, both preaspiration and postaspiration duration in VCV sequences are quite variable. Some instances have both preaspiration and postaspiration, as in the example in Figure 4–95. Other – 201 –

For word-medial contexts data are available from all four subjects. In<br />

VC syllables, <strong>the</strong> three subjects who produced unscripted speech have<br />

mean preaspiration durations rang<strong>in</strong>g from 8 to 11 ms, while subject ES<br />

has a mean duration of 19 ms. In VC syllables <strong>the</strong> unscripted speech<br />

subjects have mean preaspiration durations of 8–17 ms, which are shorter<br />

than ES’s mean duration of 26 ms.<br />

As regards voic<strong>in</strong>g conditions preced<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> occlusion phase <strong>in</strong><br />

sequences of nasal + stop, <strong>the</strong>re is little <strong>in</strong>dication of voiceless nasality.<br />

Therefore, no analysis of <strong>the</strong> tim<strong>in</strong>g relations <strong>in</strong> voice offset <strong>and</strong> stop<br />

occlusion is provided here.<br />

4.5.3.6 Summary <strong>and</strong> discussion<br />

It was shown <strong>in</strong> section 4.2.4 that <strong>the</strong> Central St<strong>and</strong>ard Swedish<br />

(CSw) speakers have a much greater difference <strong>in</strong> duration between<br />

word-<strong>in</strong>itial <strong>and</strong> word-medial postaspirations than <strong>the</strong> Western Ål<strong>and</strong><br />

subjects. In <strong>the</strong> CSw data (cf. section 4.2), word-<strong>in</strong>itial stops (tennis type<br />

words) have mean postaspiration durations rang<strong>in</strong>g from 44 to 58 ms.<br />

Word-medial stops (båten <strong>and</strong> vatten type words) have mean postaspiration<br />

durations of 20–32 ms. In a third group of words, <strong>the</strong> taverna type,<br />

which have a word-<strong>in</strong>itial stop <strong>in</strong> a lexically unstressed syllable, <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>itial<br />

stop has mean postaspiration durations rang<strong>in</strong>g from 40 to 58 ms. 1 In<br />

CSw, <strong>the</strong>refore, word-<strong>in</strong>itial position as such <strong>in</strong>duces postaspiration <strong>in</strong><br />

stop consonants (cf. discussion <strong>in</strong> 4.2.4). The fact that a syllable is unstressed<br />

does not, <strong>in</strong> itself, lead to unaspirated stop production.<br />

In <strong>the</strong> Western Ål<strong>and</strong> data, we observe relatively small differences<br />

between postaspiration <strong>in</strong> word-<strong>in</strong>itial <strong>and</strong> word-medial stops, at least for<br />

<strong>the</strong> unscripted speech data. Certa<strong>in</strong>ly, <strong>the</strong>se differences are much smaller<br />

than <strong>in</strong> CSw. Unlike CSw, <strong>the</strong>se durational differences <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Western<br />

Ål<strong>and</strong> dialect can be attributed to stress differences. In fact, <strong>the</strong> duration<br />

of word-medial postaspiration <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> Western Ål<strong>and</strong> data seems to be directly<br />

comparable to that <strong>in</strong> CSw taverna type words, i.e. word-<strong>in</strong>itial<br />

stops <strong>in</strong> unstressed syllables.<br />

1 Words referred to as taverna type words <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> CSw data occur very <strong>in</strong>frequently <strong>in</strong><br />

<strong>the</strong> Western Ål<strong>and</strong> data. Therefore, such words are not discussed specifically for <strong>the</strong><br />

Western Ål<strong>and</strong> dialect.<br />

– 200 –

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!