26.07.2013 Views

Preaspiration in the Nordic Languages: Synchronic and Diachronic ...

Preaspiration in the Nordic Languages: Synchronic and Diachronic ...

Preaspiration in the Nordic Languages: Synchronic and Diachronic ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

(i.e., short mVOffT <strong>and</strong> long mVOT). For unaspirated <strong>and</strong> preaspirated<br />

stops, <strong>the</strong>re is often a considerable time lag between modal voice offset<br />

<strong>and</strong> stop closure, but not between stop release <strong>and</strong> modal voice onset<br />

(i.e., long mVOffT <strong>and</strong> short mVOT). Such a difference <strong>in</strong> tim<strong>in</strong>g relations<br />

cannot be attributed to articulatory reduction, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>re is no reason<br />

to assume that preaspirated stops are derived from postaspirated stops by<br />

some type of articulatory or aerodynamic mechanism. At <strong>the</strong> same time,<br />

this does not mean that articulatory <strong>and</strong>/or aerodynamic factors are irrelevant<br />

<strong>in</strong> an explanation of why some CSw speakers exploit preaspiration<br />

(see Ch. 5).<br />

O<strong>the</strong>r aspects of variation <strong>in</strong> fortis stops can be attributed to hypospeech.<br />

For example, <strong>in</strong>stances <strong>in</strong> which <strong>the</strong> fortes are fricated <strong>and</strong> do not<br />

have a full stop closure can be seen as articulatory reduction. Also, <strong>the</strong><br />

with<strong>in</strong>-speaker variation <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> amount of postaspiration or preaspiration<br />

produced may be a function of <strong>the</strong> hyper-hypo cont<strong>in</strong>uum.<br />

Lastly, it may be suggested that postaspiration <strong>in</strong> CSw fortis stops<br />

functions as a boundary marker (i.e., a “Grenzensignal,” cf. Trubetzkoy<br />

1962:255), mark<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> onset of a word or a stressed syllable. Likewise,<br />

<strong>the</strong> absence of postaspiration, <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong> concomitant presence of preaspiration<br />

for some speakers, signals <strong>the</strong> absence of a word boundary (“negatives<br />

Grenzensignal,” ibid.) <strong>and</strong> <strong>in</strong>dicates that <strong>the</strong> follow<strong>in</strong>g vowel is unstressed.<br />

In CSw lenis stops, <strong>the</strong>re does not seem to be such a functional<br />

division between word-<strong>in</strong>itial <strong>and</strong> word-medial positions.<br />

4.3 Tórshavn Faroese<br />

In this section, <strong>the</strong> stop production of four Tórshavn Faroese<br />

speakers is analysed, both from a quantitative <strong>and</strong> a qualitative po<strong>in</strong>t of<br />

view. First, <strong>in</strong> order to make it easier to refer to stops <strong>in</strong> different contexts,<br />

<strong>the</strong>y will be designated to specific word types (section 4.3.2.1).<br />

Then, some problems regard<strong>in</strong>g <strong>the</strong> phonological distribution of preaspirated<br />

stop variants <strong>in</strong> Faroese are adressed. These problems become<br />

evident when <strong>the</strong> literature on Faroese preaspiration is reviewed (cf<br />

section 3.3). The approach adopted here is to analyse <strong>the</strong> stop duration<br />

data without mak<strong>in</strong>g a priori assumptions as to <strong>the</strong>ir phonological status<br />

as fortis or lenis stops (sections 4.3.2.3 <strong>and</strong> 4.3.2.4). Section 4.3.2.4 also<br />

– 146 –

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!