26.07.2013 Views

Preaspiration in the Nordic Languages: Synchronic and Diachronic ...

Preaspiration in the Nordic Languages: Synchronic and Diachronic ...

Preaspiration in the Nordic Languages: Synchronic and Diachronic ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Table 4–1. Overview of <strong>the</strong> data on word-<strong>in</strong>itial fortis stops analysed for <strong>the</strong><br />

four CSw subjects.<br />

GT CK MP FS Total<br />

Stop closure 44 86 154 127 411<br />

Fricative 2 2 17 5 26<br />

Laughter etc. 3 2 6 9 20<br />

Total 49 90 177 141 457<br />

In total, <strong>the</strong>n, 46 cases (approximately 10% of <strong>the</strong> data) were excluded<br />

from <strong>the</strong> durational analysis. This is a fairly low figure given that<br />

<strong>the</strong> data are from spontaneous speech. One can conclude that <strong>the</strong> CSw<br />

subjects <strong>in</strong> this experiment generally tended to produce word-<strong>in</strong>itial stops<br />

with a full stop closure.<br />

4.2.2.2 Postaspiration duration<br />

The duration of postaspiration (Po), measured as <strong>the</strong> time lag from<br />

release to modal voice onset (mVOT; cf. section 4.1.2), varied considerably.<br />

As <strong>in</strong>dicated <strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> box-<strong>and</strong>-whisker plot (Figure 4–4), <strong>the</strong> measurements<br />

ranged between approximately 10–90 ms (disregard<strong>in</strong>g outliers<br />

<strong>and</strong> far outliers), with <strong>the</strong> bulk of <strong>the</strong> variation conta<strong>in</strong>ed with<strong>in</strong> <strong>the</strong> <strong>in</strong>terval<br />

30–65 ms.<br />

In Table 4–2, <strong>the</strong> data on word-<strong>in</strong>itial fortis stops have been divided<br />

<strong>in</strong>to three ma<strong>in</strong> groups with regard to <strong>the</strong> placement of <strong>the</strong> stop. First,<br />

<strong>the</strong>re are content words with primary lexical stress (e.g. palmer ‘palm<br />

trees,’ tennis ‘tennis’ <strong>and</strong> kust ‘coast’). Second, <strong>the</strong>re are function words<br />

with primary lexical stress (e.g. på ‘on,’ till ‘to,’ kan ‘can, is able to’).<br />

And third, <strong>the</strong>re are lexically unstressed syllables <strong>in</strong> content words (e.g.<br />

parasollet ‘<strong>the</strong> parasol,’ taverna ‘pub,’ kompassros ‘compass rose’;<br />

underscores <strong>in</strong>dicate <strong>the</strong> syllable with primary lexical stress). These three<br />

groups will henceforth be referred to as tennis type, till type <strong>and</strong> taverna<br />

type words respectively.<br />

On average, <strong>in</strong>itial stops <strong>in</strong> tennis type words had longer postaspirations<br />

than <strong>in</strong> till type words (see Table 4–2). A two-tailed t-test <strong>in</strong>dicates<br />

that this is significant for 3 out of 4 subjects, FS, CK <strong>and</strong> MP (p < 0.02,<br />

p < 0.01 <strong>and</strong> p < 0.001 respectively). However, <strong>the</strong> difference was not<br />

– 113 –

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!