12.07.2013 Views

The Cambridge Handbook of Expertise and Expert Performance

The Cambridge Handbook of Expertise and Expert Performance

The Cambridge Handbook of Expertise and Expert Performance

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

462 the cambridge h<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> expertise <strong>and</strong> expert performance<br />

played through the piece when disinterested<br />

in it. Similarly, when the goal is to master<br />

a certain piece or a specific difficulty, adults<br />

may work hard <strong>and</strong> use more practice strategies<br />

than when they want to enjoy themselves<br />

or relax rather than mastering the<br />

instrument (Lehmann & Papousek, 2003).<br />

<strong>The</strong> Development <strong>of</strong> Musical <strong><strong>Expert</strong>ise</strong><br />

Stages <strong>and</strong> Phases<br />

Demarcating points along the time line <strong>of</strong><br />

acquisition <strong>of</strong> a new skill allows us to better<br />

conceptualize the process as a whole,<br />

<strong>and</strong> several stage <strong>and</strong> phase models have<br />

been proposed (see Proctor & Vu, Chapter<br />

15). Fitts <strong>and</strong> Posner (1967), for example,<br />

in their well-known model describe skills as<br />

being first cognitive, then associative, <strong>and</strong><br />

later autonomous, in essence requiring less<br />

<strong>and</strong> less cognitive mediation as skilled performance<br />

increases. Similar stages can be<br />

identified in Sudnow’s (1993) phenomenological<br />

account <strong>of</strong> his learning to improvise<br />

jazz on the piano. First, he had to<br />

decide consciously which chord to use next<br />

<strong>and</strong> then how to distribute the chordal<br />

notes on the keyboard (voicing). Later his<br />

fingers seemed to find the right notes by<br />

themselves. Much later, his aesthetic decision<br />

<strong>of</strong> what to play seemed to trigger<br />

the correct chord sequences with associated<br />

voicings. Whereas the focus <strong>of</strong> attention<br />

in novices is directed toward technical,<br />

low-level aspects, experts attend to higherlevel,<br />

strategic or aesthetic issues, a finding<br />

also demonstrated for composing (Colley,<br />

Banton, & Down, 1992) <strong>and</strong> improvising<br />

(Hargreaves, Cork, & Setton, 1991). For<br />

many musicians the earlier stages <strong>of</strong> skill<br />

development are successfully completed in<br />

(early) childhood.<br />

Fitts <strong>and</strong> Posner’s (1967) model is informative<br />

with regard to skill development<br />

<strong>of</strong> an individual, but it neglects the lifespan<br />

context. Bloom (1985) explicated how<br />

skills develop through life. First, the child<br />

is introduced to the domain in an informal<br />

phase, <strong>and</strong> it is here that children in “musi-<br />

cal” households may be at an advantage.<br />

<strong>The</strong>n comes a phase during which formal<br />

tuition is sought. This stage extends until the<br />

young musician makes a full-time commitment<br />

to music in order to become a pr<strong>of</strong>essional.<br />

In a later phase, once a pr<strong>of</strong>essional<br />

status has been reached, the expert<br />

is working at trying to make a lasting contribution.<br />

For a musician, this would entail<br />

making sound recordings for major record<br />

labels, playing in prestigious concert halls<br />

(e.g., Carnegie Hall), or winning certain<br />

competitions (e.g., Frederic Chopin International<br />

Piano Competition). Vitouch (2005)<br />

described in detail how parts <strong>of</strong> the expertise<br />

may get lost in old age – one could call<br />

this “de-expertization” – <strong>and</strong> how experts<br />

like the piano soloist Horowitz possibly<br />

compensate for it. Interestingly the psychomotor<br />

adaptations do not decline inevitably<br />

with old age but can be maintained for<br />

a long time through continuous practice.<br />

Krampe <strong>and</strong> Ericsson (1996) demonstrated<br />

that older pianists were able to counteract<br />

losses in motor performance through<br />

practice, whereas non-pianists did not show<br />

this advantage. However, both groups suffered<br />

age-related declines in other cognitive<br />

domains (see also Krampe & Charness,<br />

Chapter 40).<br />

<strong>The</strong> time needed for experts to develop<br />

sufficient skills for a pr<strong>of</strong>essional career is<br />

sometimes estimated to be roughly a decade<br />

(Ericsson & Crutcher, 1990). Hayes (1989)<br />

demonstrated that this “10-year rule” also<br />

applies to composers in classical music,<br />

including Mozart. Works from Mozart’s earliest<br />

phases were conspicuously underrepresented<br />

in selected lists <strong>of</strong> his recordings. Similarly,<br />

Weisberg (1999) demonstrated that it<br />

took <strong>The</strong> Beatles approximately a decade<br />

to acquire international reputation. Prior to<br />

writing their own songs they covered music<br />

by other b<strong>and</strong>s. It is a futile effort to dwell<br />

on exact number <strong>of</strong> years, but it is important<br />

to note that even famous exponents <strong>of</strong><br />

a domain take a long time to acquire their<br />

skills.<br />

In order to compete successfully for<br />

scholarships, prizes, <strong>and</strong> media attention,<br />

instrumentalists in the classical music

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!