12.07.2013 Views

The Cambridge Handbook of Expertise and Expert Performance

The Cambridge Handbook of Expertise and Expert Performance

The Cambridge Handbook of Expertise and Expert Performance

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

458 the cambridge h<strong>and</strong>book <strong>of</strong> expertise <strong>and</strong> expert performance<br />

Strauss families, which by their mere existence<br />

suggest a strong heritability <strong>of</strong> musical<br />

talents. Alas, the hope to identify heritability<br />

<strong>of</strong> excellence in families is not justified<br />

(see also discussion about Galton, 1979;e.g.,<br />

Simonton, Chapter 18). Genetic background<br />

<strong>and</strong> environmental effects are mingled<br />

inevitably, <strong>and</strong> alternative explanations cannot<br />

be refuted. Older heritability explanations<br />

failed to take into account the genetic<br />

contribution <strong>of</strong> females in the genealogy<br />

or to the socio-historic fact that sons frequently<br />

followed in their fathers’ pr<strong>of</strong>essions<br />

(Farnsworth, 1969). Hence, there are<br />

different explanations for why many musicians<br />

have parents who are musically active<br />

(Gembris, 1998).<br />

<strong>The</strong> home environment is obviously<br />

important for promoting musical excellence<br />

(Csikszentmihalyi, Rathunde, & Whalen,<br />

1993; Sosniak, 1985). Also, the socioeconomic<br />

conditions <strong>of</strong> a young musician’s<br />

family constitute obvious factors that influence<br />

the choice <strong>of</strong> a teacher, the quality<br />

<strong>of</strong> the instrument played, <strong>and</strong> other possibilities<br />

awarded to the learner. An analysis<br />

<strong>of</strong> successful Polish musicians by Manturzewska<br />

(1995) revealed a common pattern<br />

<strong>of</strong> attitudes, value systems, <strong>and</strong> family<br />

structure in the musicians’ families <strong>of</strong> origin.<br />

Families were emotionally stable, task oriented,<br />

<strong>and</strong> careful in selecting their children’s<br />

friends, <strong>and</strong> they strongly supported<br />

the musical activities. <strong>The</strong>se attitudes gain<br />

importance as they translate into behavioral<br />

consequences in the daily lives <strong>of</strong> musicians.<br />

For example, Csikszentmihalyi, Rathunde,<br />

<strong>and</strong> Whalen (1993) demonstrated that families<br />

<strong>of</strong> high-achieving children changed<br />

their lives to accommodate the needs <strong>of</strong><br />

their talented <strong>of</strong>fsprings, for example, by<br />

exempting them from household chores<br />

to give them additional time to practice.<br />

Biographies <strong>of</strong> famous musicians underline<br />

such practices: the cellist Jacqueline du Pre<br />

never did her own laundry <strong>and</strong> did not have<br />

any household responsibilities as an adolescent<br />

(Easton, 1989).<br />

Taken together, it is difficult to obtain<br />

clear evidence on the role <strong>of</strong> innate abilities,<br />

despite the fact that giftedness features<br />

prominently in everyday discourse. On the<br />

other h<strong>and</strong>, much evidence exists that practice<br />

<strong>and</strong> other environmental factors have<br />

a large impact on changes in many variables<br />

related to music performance. Some<br />

researchers have expressed serious doubts<br />

whether it is even possible to identify specific<br />

innate characteristics that mediate the<br />

development <strong>of</strong> expertise (Ericsson, 2003).<br />

However, the goal <strong>of</strong> this chapter is not<br />

to work out the nature-nurture debate for<br />

music but to focus on the role <strong>of</strong> practice<br />

for the attainment <strong>of</strong> expert performance.<br />

In brief, we do not know whether practice is<br />

a sufficient condition for high achievement,<br />

but it is certainly a necessary one for invoking<br />

the cognitive, physiological, <strong>and</strong> psychomotor<br />

adaptations observed in experts.<br />

Although practice is omnipresent during<br />

the development <strong>of</strong> expertise in music,<br />

its role <strong>and</strong> manifestation is not identical<br />

in all musical genres. Different musical<br />

styles are characteristic for specific musical<br />

cultures, <strong>and</strong> those different cultures<br />

have their respective types <strong>of</strong> practice. Most<br />

research on musical expertise has been conducted<br />

in the classical conservatoire tradition<br />

– also known as the “Western art music<br />

tradition”. Investigations about expert performance<br />

in jazz music, popular music, or<br />

vernacular genres may yield somewhat different<br />

results (Berliner, 1994). For example,<br />

whereas an early start <strong>of</strong> training is typical<br />

for pianists <strong>and</strong> violinists in the classical<br />

music domain, jazz guitarists start much<br />

later (Gruber, Degner, & Lehmann, 2004),<br />

<strong>and</strong> so do most singers today (Kopiez, 1998).<br />

Despite some differences, important commonalities<br />

regarding phases <strong>of</strong> development<br />

or deliberate practice should exist regardless<br />

<strong>of</strong> the specific music style in question. <strong>The</strong>se<br />

will be addressed below.<br />

Increasing <strong>Performance</strong><br />

through Practice<br />

Practice: Investing the Time<br />

<strong>The</strong> discussion about the role <strong>of</strong> traininginduced<br />

changes in performance was

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!