05.07.2013 Views

California Preschool Learning Foundations - ECEZero2Three ...

California Preschool Learning Foundations - ECEZero2Three ...

California Preschool Learning Foundations - ECEZero2Three ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Griffin 2004; Dickinson and Snow<br />

1987; Dickinson and Tabors 1991;<br />

Snow 1983).<br />

Children learn much of their vocabulary<br />

and basic language concepts<br />

indirectly through their interaction<br />

with others, especially adults (Cunningham<br />

and Stanovich 1998; Hayes<br />

and Ahrens 1988; Miller and Gildea<br />

1987; Nagy and Anderson 1984; Nagy,<br />

Herman, and Anderson 1985; Nagy<br />

and Herman1987; Sternberg 1987;<br />

Swanborn and De Glopper 1999).<br />

Children also acquire vocabulary<br />

through direct, explicit instruction.<br />

For example, Biemiller (1999) and<br />

Stahl (1999) reviewed several studies<br />

and found that children can acquire<br />

and retain two or three words a day<br />

through instruction involving contextualized<br />

introduction and explanation<br />

of new words. Other researchers have<br />

also found that direct and explicit<br />

approaches are effective in increasing<br />

children’s vocabulary (see Elley<br />

1989; Feitelson, Kita, and Goldstein<br />

1986; Whitehurst and others 1988).<br />

With adequate instruction most children<br />

can acquire new vocabulary at<br />

rates necessary to reach “grade level”<br />

vocabulary by the middle years in<br />

elementary school (Biemiller 2001).<br />

For example, Hart and Risley (1995)<br />

found that when teachers provided<br />

40 or more hours of rich linguistic<br />

interactions per week, children were<br />

able to perform linguistic tasks at the<br />

expected level. Similarly, Landry’s<br />

study (in press) demonstrates that parents<br />

who do not frequently engage in<br />

quality conversations with their children<br />

can be coached to support their<br />

children’s language and literacy skills<br />

more effectively.<br />

<strong>California</strong> Department of Education • <strong>Preschool</strong> <strong>Learning</strong> <strong>Foundations</strong>, Volume 1<br />

75<br />

Grammar. During the first five years<br />

of a child’s life, language acquisition<br />

develops toward an adult-like grammar.<br />

“Grammar” refers to the way<br />

in which phrases and sentences are<br />

structured to make meaning. Children<br />

seem to have an innate propensity<br />

toward learning the grammatical rules<br />

that govern their language. The grammatical<br />

rules include the organization<br />

of basic sentences (e.g., subject + verb<br />

+ object: Juan drew the picture) and<br />

the joining of clauses and phrases to<br />

elaborate the basic sentence structure<br />

(e.g., subject + verb + object and<br />

subject + verb + object: Juan drew the<br />

picture and he is hanging it) (Chomsky<br />

1957). Grammar also provides rules<br />

about the way in which nouns can be<br />

elaborated with determiners and adjectives<br />

(e.g., the big brown dog), verbs<br />

can be elaborated to share information<br />

about time (e.g., will be running), and<br />

phrases can be created for prepositions<br />

(e.g., on the table), adverbs (e.g., very,<br />

very slowly), and other parts of speech.<br />

Between the second and fifth year of<br />

life, children master virtually all the<br />

rules required for an adult-like grammar<br />

and can comprehend and produce<br />

sentences with embedded clauses<br />

(e.g., “That boy who came today is<br />

my friend”), as well as sentences with<br />

multiple subjects and predicates (e.g.,<br />

“I am going and then he will get me”).<br />

The rate at which children achieve<br />

syntactic precision, however, varies<br />

from child to child (Chapman 2000).<br />

The proportion of complex sentences<br />

contained in children’s language use<br />

ranges from 5 percent to 30 percent<br />

(Huttenlocher and others 2002). This<br />

variability in the rate of growth has<br />

been linked to children’s experiences<br />

LANGUAGE AND LITERACY

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!