05.07.2013 Views

California Preschool Learning Foundations - ECEZero2Three ...

California Preschool Learning Foundations - ECEZero2Three ...

California Preschool Learning Foundations - ECEZero2Three ...

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

MATHEMATICS<br />

166<br />

of three-year-olds were able to solve<br />

these types of problems when they<br />

involved adding or subtracting a single<br />

item, but their performance decreased<br />

rapidly as the size of the second set<br />

increased.<br />

<strong>Preschool</strong>ers demonstrate the conceptual<br />

understanding and procedural<br />

fluency necessary for them to solve<br />

simple word problems (Fuson 1992b).<br />

Simple word problems are thought<br />

to be easier for preschool children to<br />

solve than number problems that are<br />

not cast in a context (Carpenter and<br />

others 1993). All ages of problemsolvers<br />

are influenced by the context<br />

of the problem and tend to perform<br />

better with more contextual information<br />

(Wason and Johnson-Laird 1972;<br />

Shannon 1999). However, preschool<br />

children tend to be more heavily influenced<br />

by the context of the problem<br />

than do older children and adults,<br />

thus limiting their ability to solve number<br />

problems that are not presented in<br />

context.<br />

Alexander, White, and Daugherty<br />

(1997) propose three conditions for<br />

reasoning in young children: the children<br />

must have a sufficient knowledge<br />

base, the task must be understandable<br />

and motivating, and the context of the<br />

task must be familiar and comfortable<br />

to the problem-solver. These conditions<br />

probably apply to all ages of problem-<br />

solvers (Wason and Johnson-Laird<br />

1972; Shannon 1999).<br />

Researchers indicate that four- and<br />

five-year-olds engage in advanced<br />

mathematical explorations spontaneously<br />

in their play (Ginsburg, Inoue,<br />

and Seo 1999; Seo and Ginsburg<br />

2004). In their everyday activities,<br />

young children spontaneously engage<br />

in a variety of mathematical explorations<br />

and applications such as pattern<br />

analysis, change and transformation,<br />

comparison of magnitude, and<br />

estimations. Any logical thinking that<br />

children exhibit to solve real-life problems<br />

could potentially be considered<br />

beginning mathematical reasoning.<br />

For example, children distributing<br />

the same (or almost same) amount of<br />

snack to classmates or using strategies<br />

to solve immediate situations in<br />

play are situations in which children<br />

begin to demonstrate their ability to<br />

solve mathematical problems. Thus,<br />

it is crucial for teachers to be attuned<br />

to the fact that mathematical reasoning<br />

happens all the time in children’s<br />

lives, and teachers would do well to<br />

use those occasions to nurture children’s<br />

mathematical thinking skills.<br />

The examples illustrate the authentic<br />

problems that occur in preschoolers’<br />

everyday activities and all the different<br />

skills involved in mathematical reasoning<br />

and problem solving.<br />

<strong>Preschool</strong> <strong>Learning</strong> <strong>Foundations</strong>, Volume 1 • <strong>California</strong> Department of Education

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!