28.06.2013 Views

Papers in PDF format

Papers in PDF format

Papers in PDF format

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

A number of other studies <strong>in</strong>vestigated the question of whether the simultaneous presentation of three symbol<br />

systems would impede or facilitate language learn<strong>in</strong>g. The subjects <strong>in</strong> most of these studies were second<br />

language learners. The media under <strong>in</strong>vestigation <strong>in</strong> this research was captioned or subtitled video. Aga<strong>in</strong> a<br />

primary question was whether the addition of another channel of <strong>in</strong><strong>format</strong>ion requir<strong>in</strong>g the decod<strong>in</strong>g of yet<br />

another symbol system would result <strong>in</strong> cognitive overload for students who are already engaged <strong>in</strong> a mentally<br />

tax<strong>in</strong>g process.<br />

Studies which looked at the effects of caption<strong>in</strong>g on language learn<strong>in</strong>g (Borras, I. 1994, Garza, T., 1991,<br />

Kosk<strong>in</strong>en, P. & Newman, S., 1991, Bean, R. M. & Wilson, R. M., 1989, Markham, P., 1989, Vanderplank,<br />

R., 1988, ) used designs which compared groups who were tested on materials which were presented us<strong>in</strong>g<br />

either pr<strong>in</strong>t, video, or video with captions. The design of the study reported by Garza (1991) is <strong>in</strong> many ways<br />

typical of this literature. In this analysis fourty students of Russian as a Foreign Language and seventy students<br />

of English as a Second Language participated <strong>in</strong> an experimental study to assess the effects of captioned video<br />

on language learn<strong>in</strong>g. Half the students of Russian watched videos with captions and half watched videos<br />

without captions. The students of English underwent the same treatments. On tests of language<br />

comprehension the groups that saw captioned video demonstrated a mean ga<strong>in</strong> of 75.2 percent of correct<br />

answers, a mean decrease of 61.16 percent of <strong>in</strong>correct answers and a mean decrease of 83.76 percent <strong>in</strong><br />

unanswered questions relative to groups who saw the same video without captions.<br />

While the ga<strong>in</strong> scores reported by Garza are atypically high, <strong>in</strong> none of the studies did the presence of captions<br />

result <strong>in</strong> lower scores on tests of comprehension. In the other studies <strong>in</strong> which the subjects were second<br />

language learners, (Borras, I., 1994, Kosk<strong>in</strong>en, P. & Newman, S., 1991, Markham, P., 1989) the presence of a<br />

captions improved comprehension relative to the other treatments. It would appear from these reports that the<br />

addition of a more stable sysmbol system, text, to a transient medium, video, results <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>creased<br />

mean<strong>in</strong>gfulness of the language. Garza (1991) concluded that the captions provide a bridge between the<br />

student's strong read<strong>in</strong>g ability and weaker listen<strong>in</strong>g skills (p. 259).<br />

It should probably not be surpris<strong>in</strong>g that even second language learners are able to attend to multiple symbol<br />

systems simultaneously when we consider the results of Pezdek and Hartman (1984). In this study sixty fiveyear-old<br />

children watched a videotape <strong>in</strong> one of three conditions, A) with toys available to play with, B) with a<br />

record play<strong>in</strong>g, C) with no such distractors. The dependent measures were percent of time that subject attended<br />

to the televison and recall accuracy on comprehension questions of the material viewed. Although attention to<br />

the televison varied by treatment, recall comprehension was not significantly different across treatment groups<br />

(p. 216) . Even five-year-old children were able to effectively distribute their attention such that they could<br />

process auditory and visual <strong>in</strong><strong>format</strong>ion while perform<strong>in</strong>g other tasks. The authors conclude that children learn<br />

a sophisticated cognitive process<strong>in</strong>g strategy for television view<strong>in</strong>g at a very young age. It appears that second<br />

language learners are equally prepared for the task of process<strong>in</strong>g multiple channels of <strong>in</strong><strong>format</strong>ion.<br />

In two of the studies of caption<strong>in</strong>g (Bean, 1989, Reese, 1983) the presence of caption<strong>in</strong>g did not result <strong>in</strong><br />

favorable effects on the learn<strong>in</strong>g. In both of these studies, subjects were us<strong>in</strong>g their native language. In the<br />

Bean article, subjects were adults learn<strong>in</strong>g to read. The author used caption<strong>in</strong>g to determ<strong>in</strong>e whether subjects<br />

could spontaneously learn new vocabulary. Subjects were assigned to one of three conditions: 1) Closed<br />

captioned with <strong>in</strong>struction (n=9), 2) Closed caption without <strong>in</strong>struction (n=7), 3) Script with <strong>in</strong>struction (n=8).<br />

Instruction entailed discuss<strong>in</strong>g essential vocabulary with the students before present<strong>in</strong>g the material. Two<br />

weaknesses seem evident <strong>in</strong> this study: 1)The sample size is too small and 2)the expectation that students who<br />

cannot read will learn vocabulary spontaneously is too large. It appears that for those who have not adequately<br />

learned the skill of decod<strong>in</strong>g letters to form words captioned video is not immediately beneficial.<br />

In Reese (1983) subjects were assigned to one of four conditions <strong>in</strong> which news stories were presented either <strong>in</strong><br />

their orig<strong>in</strong>al form, or with the addition of subtitles. In two conditions the audio and video were mismatched.<br />

The result<strong>in</strong>g groups were. 1)Audio and video match, no captions, 2)Audio and video mismatch, no captions,<br />

3)Audio and video match, captions present, 4)Audio and video mismatch, captions present. Subjects were told<br />

to evaluate the programs on a number of criteria <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>terest, clarity, accuracy, etc. The subjects were not<br />

told that they would be tested or that they should try to recall as much <strong>in</strong><strong>format</strong>ion as possible. In analyz<strong>in</strong>g<br />

data the author noted that subjects reacted negatively to the "captions for the hear<strong>in</strong>g impaired (p14)." These<br />

results seem to suggest that captions were viewed not as a potential aid to recall, (the subjects were unaware of<br />

their purpose <strong>in</strong> view<strong>in</strong>g), but as a compensatory meant for others. Kozma suggests that, "the perceptions<br />

students have about a medium and the purpose they have for view<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>fluence the amount of effort they put<br />

<strong>in</strong>to the process<strong>in</strong>g of the message and consequently, the depth of their understand<strong>in</strong>g of the story (p. 13)." In<br />

Reese's study it appears that the students were mis<strong>in</strong>formed about their purpose and had negative perceptions

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!