15.06.2013 Views

Selected Papers from the Fourteenth International ... - STIBA Malang

Selected Papers from the Fourteenth International ... - STIBA Malang

Selected Papers from the Fourteenth International ... - STIBA Malang

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Cleft and identificational constructions 219<br />

In any event, <strong>the</strong> discussion of findings <strong>from</strong> 17th and 18th-century plays<br />

which we have presented in this section of our paper should have illustrated<br />

that – for one thing – <strong>the</strong> correlation between IdCCs and ClCs might indeed<br />

be translated into an historical sequence. Moreover, we believe we have found<br />

a means of attempting to explain <strong>the</strong> rise of object pronouns in predicate position<br />

by indicating that <strong>the</strong>se are tied to referential conditions.<br />

4. Conclusions<br />

Let us begin our brief concluding section with yet ano<strong>the</strong>r quote <strong>from</strong> an 18thcentury<br />

grammarian, this time <strong>from</strong> John Burn who was Priestley’s contemporary.<br />

In his Practical Grammar he states:<br />

The substantive verb am or be admits a nominative before it and after it, False<br />

syntax: ‘It is me’, ‘It was <strong>the</strong>m that bought <strong>the</strong> goods, and it was me that bought for<br />

<strong>the</strong>m’. (Burn 1786: 85)<br />

If this reflects real usage of <strong>the</strong> predicative first person pronoun, <strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong>se constructions<br />

too were affected by a tendency that we have witnessed in <strong>the</strong> plays:<br />

<strong>the</strong>re we do indeed find alternating use of subject and object pronoun in <strong>the</strong> identificational<br />

use, sometimes even with <strong>the</strong> same character and within a few lines.<br />

However, we have not found a single felicitous instance with me in a subject ClC.<br />

This does not, of course, mean that speakers did not produce such constructions.<br />

But here we enter uncertain territory. In terms of frequency we may, however,<br />

infer that self-identification – as well as pronominal identification of ano<strong>the</strong>r<br />

person – was pragmatically a frequent matter. Then what of focussing by clefting?<br />

To begin with: was this a strategy in oral communication? Our data at least<br />

suggest this, although it would take a much more thorough quantitative analysis to<br />

get closer to an adequate answer.<br />

What is basically at stake is <strong>the</strong> question as to how ‘oral’ it-ClCs really were.<br />

All we know for sure is that <strong>the</strong>y were established to some extent in LME and that<br />

today <strong>the</strong>y are largely restricted to expository written prose (Biber et al. 1999: 961).<br />

What happened in <strong>the</strong> meantime is still open for fur<strong>the</strong>r research. Until <strong>the</strong> results<br />

of this research we probably have to stick with Figaro in Holcroft’s wording: ’Tis<br />

he, ’tis she, ’tis me ’tis – I don’t know who . . . 11<br />

11. We thank Derek Britton very much for checking our English. Moreover we thank <strong>the</strong><br />

referees and editors of this volume for <strong>the</strong>ir valuable comments during <strong>the</strong> revision process<br />

of this paper.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!