15.06.2013 Views

Selected Papers from the Fourteenth International ... - STIBA Malang

Selected Papers from the Fourteenth International ... - STIBA Malang

Selected Papers from the Fourteenth International ... - STIBA Malang

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

9 Letizia Vezzosi<br />

b. [Genesis 2382–2383] þa þæt wif ahloh wereda drihtnes nalles glædlice,<br />

ac heo gearum frod<br />

“<strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong> woman laughed at <strong>the</strong> lord of hosts, by no means kindly, for<br />

she, [was] advanced in years”<br />

In all those cases <strong>the</strong>re is a conflict between ‘semantic’ agreement and ‘syntactic’<br />

agreement: a linguistic fact cross-linguistically quite frequent in formal gender<br />

assignment systems, when <strong>the</strong> grammatical gender and <strong>the</strong> meaning of <strong>the</strong> noun<br />

clash. Especially in <strong>the</strong> case of pronominal gender, Moravcsik (1978) had already<br />

noticed that in noun phrase external agreement (e.g., agreement between<br />

nouns and verbs or anaphoric pronouns) grammatical gender agreement is often<br />

optional. 11 In his typological studies, by handling many instances of gender<br />

divergence and fluctuation between semantic and syntactic agreement, Corbett<br />

individuates four types of agreement targets, arranges <strong>the</strong>m into a hierarchy<br />

of agreement (5), and formulates constraints about <strong>the</strong> possible agreement<br />

patterns, as in (6a–c).<br />

(5) Agreement hierarchy (Corbett 1991: 20 )<br />

Attributive > Predicative > Relative Pronoun > Personal Pronoun<br />

(6) a. As we move rightwards along <strong>the</strong> hierarchy, <strong>the</strong> likelihood of semantic<br />

agreement will increase monotonically (that is with no intervening<br />

decrease). (Corbett 1991: 20 )<br />

b. If parallel targets show different agreement forms, <strong>the</strong>n <strong>the</strong> fur<strong>the</strong>r target<br />

will show semantic agreement. (Corbett 1991: 235)<br />

c. For any particular target type, <strong>the</strong> fur<strong>the</strong>r it is removed <strong>from</strong> its controller,<br />

<strong>the</strong> greater <strong>the</strong> likelihood of semantic agreement. (Corbett 1991: 235)<br />

These are also <strong>the</strong> agreement targets present in Old English and <strong>the</strong> discrepancies<br />

noted above are explicable in terms of Corbett’s maxims (6a–c). In a typological<br />

perspective, <strong>the</strong>n, Old English does not differ <strong>from</strong> many o<strong>the</strong>r languages<br />

with formal gender systems, at least with regard to this kind of gender fluctuation.<br />

When grammatical gender is not as expected, it is only because <strong>the</strong> referential<br />

gender of <strong>the</strong> noun overrides <strong>the</strong> lexical gender (Dahl 2000: 105–106). Like o<strong>the</strong>r<br />

languages in <strong>the</strong> world, this also happens in Old English when <strong>the</strong> morphology of<br />

<strong>the</strong> noun does not match its semantic content, and accordingly <strong>the</strong> gender of <strong>the</strong><br />

variable gender words may be determined by <strong>the</strong> gender of <strong>the</strong> conceptualised<br />

referent.<br />

11. Moravcsik (1978) distinguishes noun-phrase external agreement <strong>from</strong> noun phrase internal<br />

agreement (i.e., inflection of nouns, relative pronouns, adjectives, demonstratives, possessives,<br />

articles and numerals) where grammatical gender agreement is obligatory.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!