08.06.2013 Views

the university of chicago the phonology and ... - SIL International

the university of chicago the phonology and ... - SIL International

the university of chicago the phonology and ... - SIL International

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

One could infer from this that in languages without compensatory leng<strong>the</strong>ning, or<br />

o<strong>the</strong>r processes which involve <strong>the</strong> presence <strong>of</strong> a second mora in a syllable, NC sequences<br />

should not be leng<strong>the</strong>ned with respect to singleton Ns <strong>and</strong> Cs. There is some evidence to<br />

suggest that this is <strong>the</strong> case. For example, Maddieson (1989) finds that prenasalized stops<br />

in Fijian have <strong>the</strong> same closure duration characteristics as plain stops <strong>and</strong> liquids. 3<br />

With <strong>the</strong>se observations in mind, I examined <strong>the</strong> closure duration <strong>of</strong> prenasalized<br />

stops in Mono to determine if <strong>the</strong>re is a noticeable increase in duration in comparison to<br />

plain stops. Mono is actually a better test case than Fijian for such a study. In Mono, like<br />

in Fijian, prenasalized stops unambiguously pattern as single segments. 4 But Mono has a<br />

three-way contrast between voiceless, voiced, <strong>and</strong> prenasalized stops, whereas Fijian only<br />

has a contrast between plain voiceless stops <strong>and</strong> prenasalized stops. In Fijian,<br />

prenasalized stops could be interpreted phonologically as plain voiced stops, but in Mono<br />

such an interpretation is impossible.<br />

I identified <strong>the</strong> start <strong>and</strong> end <strong>of</strong> closure via <strong>the</strong> following criteria. For plain stops,<br />

<strong>the</strong> beginning <strong>of</strong> closure was marked where vertical striations disappeared from across<br />

frequency range <strong>and</strong> only low-frequency striations remained visible. For prenasalized<br />

stops, <strong>the</strong> beginning <strong>of</strong> closure was marked where <strong>the</strong>re was a significant drop in <strong>the</strong><br />

strength <strong>of</strong> <strong>the</strong> formants <strong>and</strong> <strong>the</strong>re was a clear change from <strong>the</strong> oral resonance values to<br />

<strong>the</strong> nasal resonance values. The end <strong>of</strong> closure was identified by <strong>the</strong> spike representing<br />

<strong>the</strong> stop burst. In all cases, <strong>the</strong> closure start- <strong>and</strong> end-points were readily identifiable. For<br />

example, <strong>the</strong> beginning <strong>and</strong> end <strong>of</strong> closure is indicated by <strong>the</strong> arrows in Figures 8.11 <strong>and</strong><br />

8.28.<br />

3 Maddieson measured closure duration in his study. On <strong>the</strong> o<strong>the</strong>r h<strong>and</strong>, Hubbard measured<br />

individual segments following Peterson & Lehiste’s (1960) criteria for demarcation.<br />

4 Also, <strong>the</strong>re are no nasal + stop sequences across morpheme or word boundaries.<br />

149

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!