05.04.2013 Views

Factors and Processes Contributing to Resilience

Factors and Processes Contributing to Resilience

Factors and Processes Contributing to Resilience

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

9 - ---- --<br />

<strong>Fac<strong>to</strong>rs</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Processes</strong> <strong>Contributing</strong><br />

<strong>to</strong> <strong>Resilience</strong><br />

The <strong>Resilience</strong> Framework<br />

Karol L. Kumpfer<br />

Introduction<br />

<strong>Resilience</strong> is becoming an increasingly popular concept for research <strong>and</strong><br />

application in the field of prevention. Because of reduced funding for services<br />

<strong>to</strong> help at-risk children <strong>and</strong> families, information on low cost methods<br />

for increasing resilience <strong>to</strong> negative life events is critically needed. A better<br />

underst<strong>and</strong>ing of ways <strong>to</strong> increase resilience in all children holds great<br />

promise for improving the effectiveness of preventive community, school,<br />

<strong>and</strong> family services.<br />

A shift in focus from risk <strong>to</strong> resilience, according <strong>to</strong> Thrner (1995), has<br />

developed partially from a frustration with such a pervasive emphasis on<br />

the identification of risk fac<strong>to</strong>rs. Whi1e a risk-focused approach has been<br />

very helpful in the public health field in the reduction of infectious diseases,<br />

more complex diseases of life style require a more comprehensive approach<br />

including proteelive <strong>and</strong> resilience mechanisms (Rutter, 1993).A paradigmshift<br />

appears <strong>to</strong> be occurring <strong>to</strong>wards an increasing emphasis on optimism<br />

<strong>and</strong> hope as opposed <strong>to</strong> the frustration <strong>and</strong> despair that can occur from an<br />

emphasis on risk processes.<br />

Karol L. Kumprer • Center ror Substance Abuse Prevention, Substance Abuse <strong>and</strong> Mental<br />

Health Services Administration, Rockville, Maryl<strong>and</strong> 20857.<br />

<strong>Resilience</strong> <strong>and</strong> l>evelopment: Positive Ufe Adaptations, edited by Glantz <strong>and</strong> Johnson. Kluwer<br />

Academic/Pienum Publishers, New York, 1999.<br />

179


184 Karol L. Kumpfer<br />

or caring others either passively or actively attempt <strong>to</strong> perceive,<br />

interpret <strong>and</strong> surmount threats, eh allenges or difficult environments<br />

<strong>to</strong> construct more protective environments.<br />

4. Internat Self Characteristics include internal individual spiritual,<br />

cognitive, social/behavioral, physical <strong>and</strong> emotional/affective competencies<br />

or strengths needed <strong>to</strong> be successful in different developmental<br />

tasks, different cultures, <strong>and</strong> different personal environrnents.<br />

5. <strong>Resilience</strong> <strong>Processes</strong> include unique short-term or long-term<br />

resilience or stress/coping processes learned by the individual<br />

through gradual exposure <strong>to</strong> increasing chanenges <strong>and</strong> stressors that<br />

help the individual <strong>to</strong> bounce-back with resilient reintegration<br />

(Richardson, Neiger, Jensen, & Kumpfer, 1990).<br />

6. Positive Outcomes or successfu11ife adaptation in specific developmental<br />

tasks which are supportive of later positive adaptation in<br />

specific new developmental tasks culminating in a higher likelihood<br />

of reaching a global designation in adulthood as a "resilient child<br />

or adult". While this is an outcome, in a dynamic model, a positive<br />

outcome suggesting resiHence is also predictive of later resilient<br />

reintegration after disruption or stress.<br />

Alt six of these major duster variables or constructs are needed <strong>to</strong> organize<br />

predic<strong>to</strong>rs of resilient outcomes in high-risk youth because research<br />

studies have reviewed these different constructs as predictive of resilience<br />

in an individual. Organizing research findings by these six areas would<br />

help <strong>to</strong> clarify the differences between environmental stimuli, transactional<br />

environment buffering processes, internal mediating self fac<strong>to</strong>rs, resilience<br />

processes used <strong>to</strong> bounce-back after a challenge, <strong>and</strong> the final developmental<br />

outcomes of resilient children. Because it is a daunting task <strong>to</strong><br />

simultaneously test multicausal models (Sameroff, Seifer, & Barocas, 1983),<br />

most of the predictive research cited in this review includes only parts of<br />

this framework in a single study. This resilience framework has been empirically<br />

tested in doc<strong>to</strong>ral dissertations at the University of Utah using<br />

structural equation models in simultaneous measures of multiple domains<br />

in college students (Neiger, 1991), working mothers (Dunn, 1994), <strong>and</strong><br />

children of alcoholics (Walker, 1995). As highlighted by Egel<strong>and</strong>, Carlson,<br />

& Sroufe (1993), an organizational approach or framework for the study<br />

of resilience provides a means of integrating findings on risk <strong>and</strong> protective<br />

fac<strong>to</strong>rs in individuals <strong>and</strong> environments <strong>and</strong> focuses attention on<br />

processes of adaptation. "From such an organizalional view, the capacity for<br />

resilience is seen as developing over time through an integration of constitutional<br />

<strong>and</strong> experiential fac<strong>to</strong>rs in the context of a supportive environment"<br />

(p. 525).


186 Karol L Kumprer<br />

Types of <strong>Resilience</strong> Research<br />

Different research designs have been used <strong>to</strong> study resilience or related<br />

concepts (e.g., stress-coping, ego-resilience, personality, motivation, <strong>and</strong><br />

health). Each of these different research methodologies can produce discrepant,<br />

but perhaps eventually eonverging, results concerning the most<br />

critical elements predicting resilience <strong>to</strong> negative life events or high-risk<br />

environments. Five major types of resilience research were reviewed.<br />

Retrospective, Single Sample, or Cross-Sectional Studies<br />

This type of research design is used primarily by life events researchers<br />

<strong>and</strong> some other resilience researchers. Because of time constraints, tbis<br />

design is popular with graduate students. Life events researchers, typically<br />

Iimit their collection of data <strong>to</strong> determining the relationship of negative life<br />

events <strong>and</strong> adaptation. If children or adolescents are studied, they are generally<br />

asked <strong>to</strong> rate stressful life events experienced in the recent past <strong>and</strong><br />

whether they perceive the event as positive or negative (Swearingen &<br />

Cohen, 1985). Simple correlational data analyses suggest that major life<br />

stressors impact life adjustment, but these relationships are more complicated<br />

in individual cases <strong>and</strong> appear <strong>to</strong> be moderated by daily hassles <strong>and</strong><br />

stressors (DeLongis, Coyne, Dakof, Folkman, & Lazarus 1982). Retrospective,<br />

small sample studies are also conducted using qualitative, in-depth<br />

interview studies of successful adults who have Hmade it despite the odds"<br />

(Gordon & Song, 1993). Life events analysis is used <strong>to</strong> develop grounded<br />

theory for later hypothesis testing (Strauss & Glaser, 1967).<br />

Retrospective, Cross-Sectional, Multivariate Studies<br />

<strong>Resilience</strong> researchers using cross-sectional designs tend <strong>to</strong> include<br />

intervening variables (i.e., daily hassles, personality traits, environmental<br />

context, <strong>and</strong> interactive processes with significant others). In adults, a retrospective<br />

rating of majornegative life events occurring at any time in their<br />

lives combined with daily hassles has been used <strong>to</strong> determine "high-risk"<br />

status (Dunn, 1994). High <strong>and</strong> low risk groups have been created post·hoc<br />

or chosen in advance. General population comparison groups are sometimes<br />

compared <strong>to</strong> high-risk populations-children of alcoholics, schizophrenics,<br />

<strong>and</strong> families living in poverty. More sophisticated multivariate<br />

statistics are used, such as multiple regression <strong>and</strong> structural equation<br />

modeling. Including multiple variables from environmental, individual,<br />

<strong>and</strong> adjustment domains, researchers increased correlation coefficients <strong>to</strong><br />

.,.,.."....<br />

I


188 Karol L. KumpFer<br />

from highwrisk samples function comparably <strong>to</strong> wellwfunctioning children in<br />

general population samples (Luthar & Zigler, 1991).<br />

Occasionally these prospective studies begin after the occurrence of<br />

a specific traumatic event, such as child abuse or parental divorce. Such<br />

studies can be criticized for not having data on the child's adjustment prior<br />

<strong>to</strong> the onset of the major stressor or risk fac<strong>to</strong>r. Causality is difficult <strong>to</strong><br />

determine since these children may have poor life adjustment or behaviors<br />

prior <strong>to</strong> the trauma.<br />

Prospective, Multiple Sampie Studies<br />

This type of design is used by developmental researchers interested in<br />

comparing children in the general population with a high-risk population<br />

over time (Conger et al., 1992; Kumpfer, Molgaard, & Spoth, in press).<br />

Matched pairs or families are sometimes used as control groups (Johnson,<br />

Glassman, Fiks, & Rosen, 1990). This design could be used with many prevention<br />

sturlies where one group is given an intervention, another is not,<br />

<strong>and</strong> a generat population sample exists for comparison. Using this type of<br />

design, Abelson, Zigler, & DeBlasi (1974) <strong>and</strong> Mulholl<strong>and</strong> <strong>and</strong> associates<br />

(1991) discovered that the resilient children in high-risk groups did not<br />

equal the attainment of the normal, low-risk groups. This type of design<br />

is encouraged by Luthar <strong>and</strong> Cushing (1996) <strong>to</strong> answer the question of<br />

whether the most resilient individuals of a high-risk group truly resilient or<br />

are simply the best of a generally poorly functioning group.<br />

Outcome Research Pertaining <strong>to</strong> the Six Resiliency Predic<strong>to</strong>r<br />

Areas of the <strong>Resilience</strong> Framework<br />

Outcome research on resilience was organized in<strong>to</strong> the six predic<strong>to</strong>r<br />

areas of the proposed resiliency framework, namely: 1) the acute stressor or<br />

challenge, 2) the cxternal environmental context, 3) person-environment<br />

interactional processes, 4) internalself characteristics or resiliency fac<strong>to</strong>rs, 5)<br />

resiliency processes, <strong>and</strong> 6) the positive outcome. Research on each of these<br />

six resilience predic<strong>to</strong>r areas are discussed below in their own sections.<br />

The Acute Stressor or Cha/lenge<br />

The resiliency framework or process begins with an initiating event<br />

<strong>and</strong> ends with an outcome, hopefully a successful one that demonstrates


190 Karol L. Kumpfer<br />

"high risk". Merely assuming that a child is high risk because of belanging<br />

<strong>to</strong> a high-risk category has been questioned.<br />

This review of environmental risk <strong>and</strong> proteetive fae<strong>to</strong>rs _will not<br />

attempl an exhaustive Iist of the most eritical risk <strong>and</strong> proteelive fae<strong>to</strong>rs<br />

beeause 1) many existing reviews of risk <strong>and</strong> proteelive fac<strong>to</strong>rs for different<br />

adjustment problems exist, <strong>and</strong> 2) the most important risk <strong>and</strong> proteetive<br />

fae<strong>to</strong>rs differ for eaeh field. For reviews of risk fac<strong>to</strong>rs for substance<br />

abuse, the reader is referred <strong>to</strong> Kuropfer (1987) for a review that includes<br />

biological risk fac<strong>to</strong>rs. Hawkins, Artbur <strong>and</strong> Catalano (1994) or Hawkins,<br />

Catalano <strong>and</strong> Miller (1992) provide more recent summaries of risk fac<strong>to</strong>rs<br />

for substance abuse as weil. What is diseussed in this section are dimensions<br />

of risk <strong>and</strong> proteelive fae<strong>to</strong>rs that must be considered when attempting<br />

<strong>to</strong> determine the most predictive risk or proteelive fac<strong>to</strong>rs for positive life<br />

outcomes.<br />

In most research "high-risk" means ehildren from high-risk environments,<br />

such as children from dysfunetional families. However, some<br />

researchers also include personal risky behaviors (i.e., substance use,<br />

conduct disorders, attention deficit disorder, <strong>and</strong> deJinquency) in their<br />

definition of high risk samples (S<strong>to</strong>uthamer-Loeber et al., 1993). According<br />

<strong>to</strong> Luthar <strong>and</strong> Zigler (1991), it is important <strong>to</strong> differentiale risk fac<strong>to</strong>rs or<br />

stressors that the individual can influenee (e.g., failing in school) <strong>and</strong> those<br />

generallyout of theit eontrol (e.g., death of a parent, war, being born <strong>to</strong> a<br />

dysfunctional parent). If this is not done, predictive results are confounded<br />

by having maladjustment predict maladjustment or the convcrse.<br />

In this review, risk fac<strong>to</strong>rs are defined primarily by ehronic adversity<br />

in the environment of the child. Acute stressors are considered the stimuli<br />

for disruption <strong>and</strong> integration, thus beginning the resiliency process<br />

(Richardson, Neiger, Jensen, & Kumpfer, 1990) <strong>to</strong>wards maladaptation or<br />

resilient reintegration. The environmental eontext of stressors <strong>and</strong> supports<br />

can help the child through psychosocial facilitation processes. Critieal<br />

dimensions of risk fac<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>and</strong> processes must be considered in resilience<br />

research in defining risk, namely:<br />

1. Whether the ebild aetually experiences the risk fae<strong>to</strong>rs (Piomin &<br />

Daniels, 1987; Werner & Smith, 1992),<br />

2. Perception or attribution of risk or threat by ehild ( Gordon & Song,<br />

1994),<br />

3. Degree of direct or indirect effects on the child because of proximal<br />

or distal status in a chain of causal variables,<br />

4. Degree of transactional buffering by child or caring others (Radke­<br />

Yarrow & Brown, 1993),


,.<br />

"<br />

<strong>Fac<strong>to</strong>rs</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Processes</strong> <strong>Contributing</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>Resilience</strong> 193<br />

propose a unidirectional, linear model in which low IQ predicts adjustment<br />

difficulties directly or through intervening variables (White, Moffitt, &<br />

Silva, 1989). The St. Louis Risk Research Project found the childhood intelligence<br />

measures predicted mental health in children of mentally ill parents<br />

(Anthony, 1987; Worl<strong>and</strong>, Weeks, & Jones, 1987). Other studies with children<br />

of mentally ill parentS support this relationship (Bleuler, 1984;<br />

Garmezy, 1985; Long & Vaillant, 1984).<br />

lncreased intellectual capabilities, particularly verbal skills, have<br />

been found in resilient children of schizophrenic <strong>and</strong> depressed parents<br />

(Garmezy, 1985; Masten, Best, & Garmezy, 1990) <strong>and</strong> children of alcoholics<br />

(COAs) (Werner, 1985). This latter study found that none of the resilient<br />

COAs were judged below average at two years in intellectual development,<br />

compared <strong>to</strong> 16% of COAs who later manifested adaptive prob­<br />

\ems. Verbal capabilities <strong>and</strong> aptitude were significantly higher by age 10 as<br />

weil as scholastic aptitude <strong>and</strong> educational achievement tests in grades 5,<br />

8-10, <strong>and</strong> 12 in COAs who did not develop serious coping problems compared<br />

<strong>to</strong> those who did. The resilient COAs also had fewer errors on the<br />

Bender-Gestalt Test, considered a measure of central nervous system<br />

integrity.<br />

Arecent study by Luthar <strong>and</strong> Zigler (1992) examined the role of intelligence<br />

looking for interaction effects with Ievel of stressors <strong>and</strong> personal<br />

characteristics. They found that l.Q. can be a vulnerability fac<strong>to</strong>r in high<br />

stress Situations for high IQ children. Inner-city. multi-cultural ninth grade<br />

students with higher intelligence showed considerably more variation in<br />

school-based performance depending on locus of control <strong>and</strong> impulse<br />

control (ego development). Hence, youth with higher levels of internal<br />

locus of control <strong>and</strong> impulse control were more likely <strong>to</strong> be motivated <strong>to</strong><br />

use their intelligence <strong>to</strong> achieve academically. However, if these youth have<br />

learned powerlessness through social <strong>and</strong> educational inequities leading<br />

them <strong>to</strong> believe that school success does not lead <strong>to</strong> life success (Fordham<br />

& Ogbu. 1986), they tended <strong>to</strong> underachieve dramatically. Possibly, their<br />

achievement motivation is directed <strong>to</strong> other areas where they perceive<br />

greater chances of successfully applying their talents <strong>and</strong> intelligence, such<br />

as illegal activities <strong>and</strong> drug dealing (Myers, 1990).<br />

Gender. Another major genetic fac<strong>to</strong>r related <strong>to</strong> increased resiliency<br />

in high-risk children is female gender. Repeatedly, developmental studies<br />

of children living in at-risk environments <strong>and</strong> families have found girls <strong>to</strong><br />

be more resilient than boys (Werner, 1985). Boys appear <strong>to</strong> be more vulnerable<br />

<strong>to</strong> out-of-home care (Gamble & Zigler; 1986). Some researchers<br />

(Rutter 1982) sliggest that boys react emotionally <strong>and</strong> behaviorally in more<br />

negative ways than girJs <strong>to</strong> negative family Situations.


194 Karol L. Kumpfer<br />

Temperament <strong>and</strong> Personality. Most resiliency researchers (Garmezy,<br />

1985; Rutter, Maughan, Mortimore, Ous<strong>to</strong>n, & Smith, 1979) regardless of<br />

their discipline, agree that constitutional <strong>and</strong> temperamental disposition is<br />

a major fac<strong>to</strong>r in resilience. Both Garmezy (1985) <strong>and</strong> Rutter (1979) have<br />

discussed positive temperament or positive personality disposition as one<br />

of the three major precursors of resilience; the other two fac<strong>to</strong>rs are supportive<br />

family milieu (family cohesion <strong>and</strong> warmth) <strong>and</strong> the availability <strong>and</strong><br />

use of external support systems by parent <strong>and</strong> child. In their 14-year study<br />

of ego resilience from pre-school <strong>to</strong> late adolescence, Block <strong>and</strong> Block<br />

(1980) found precursors of resilience in children <strong>to</strong> be positive temperament<br />

traits, such as responsiveness <strong>to</strong> environmental change, ability <strong>to</strong> be<br />

comforted after stress, <strong>and</strong> ability <strong>to</strong> maintain physiological equilibrium, as<br />

weil as <strong>to</strong> modify sleep-wakefulness states. Wertlieb <strong>and</strong> associates (1989)<br />

found three temperament traits related <strong>to</strong> effects of stress: distractibility,<br />

stimulus threshold sensitivity, <strong>and</strong> response <strong>to</strong> novel stimuli.<br />

A number of sturlies have supported the hypothesis that "difficult"<br />

temperament is associated with alcohol <strong>and</strong> other drug use in later life.<br />

A "difficult" temperament as defined by frequent negative moods <strong>and</strong><br />

withdrawal was found by Lerner <strong>and</strong> Vicary (1984) as correlated with later<br />

drug probJems in a longitudinal study that tracked kindergartners until<br />

adulthood. Children characterized as "easy" children. defined by greater<br />

adaptability <strong>and</strong> happier dispositions. were significantly less likely than<br />

"difficult" children <strong>to</strong> become adult regular users of <strong>to</strong>bacco, alcohol, <strong>and</strong><br />

ffi3T1JU8na.<br />

As described by Lerner<strong>and</strong> Vicary (1984), as well as Brook <strong>and</strong> associates<br />

(1990), two major temperament traits in children who Jater used<br />

drugs are: 1) negative mood states ( anxiety, irritability, sadness, emotional<br />

upset, anger <strong>and</strong> crying) <strong>and</strong> 2) social withdrawal. These are very similar<br />

<strong>to</strong> the two major temperament characteristics-aggression <strong>and</strong> shynessfound<br />

in the Kellam <strong>and</strong> Brown (1982) longitudinal study of adolescents<br />

who later used drugs. lf youth continue aggressive behaviors until13 years<br />

of age, they have a strong likelihood of developing alcoholism (Loeber,<br />

1988) or drug abuse (Barnes & Weite, 1986) <strong>and</strong> delinquency (S<strong>to</strong>uthamer­<br />

Loeber et al., 1993). Hyperactivity, attention deficit disorder <strong>and</strong> oppositional<br />

defiant behavior in juniorhigh school aged children have been found<br />

<strong>to</strong> increase odds ratios for delinquency (S<strong>to</strong>uthamer-Loeber et al., 1993).<br />

Neurotransmitter lmbalances. Because of the consistent finding of<br />

temperament differences in negative life adjustment, temperament should<br />

be considered a significant risk fac<strong>to</strong>r leading <strong>to</strong> risk processes involving<br />

parents, teachers, <strong>and</strong> peers. Youth with difficulty, unpleasant, or aggressive<br />

temperaments are often rejected by other prosocial peers. The role of biological<br />

chemical imbalances (neurotransmitter or hormonal imbalances)


Fac<strong>to</strong>n <strong>and</strong> <strong>Processes</strong> Contrlbuting <strong>to</strong> <strong>Resilience</strong> 195<br />

should be more thoroughly investigated in this powerful risk process. Such<br />

chemical imbalances can be due <strong>to</strong> genetic inheritance, in utero chemical<br />

exposure, <strong>and</strong> postnatal exposure. The use of alcohol, <strong>to</strong>bacco, <strong>and</strong> other<br />

drugs also Ieads <strong>to</strong> neurotransmitter imbalances that can increase craving<br />

for drugs ( Goodwin, 1986). In addition <strong>to</strong> complex interactional processes<br />

between external environmental variables, internal self fac<strong>to</strong>rs have complex<br />

relationships.<br />

Interna/ Psychological Se/f Resiliency Factars<br />

Organization of Interna/ Personal Traits or Self <strong>Fac<strong>to</strong>rs</strong>. To develop a<br />

categorical framework for improving underst<strong>and</strong>ing of self fac<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>to</strong><br />

resilience, the internal individual fac<strong>to</strong>rs have been reviewed in the child<br />

development <strong>and</strong> child psychopathology literature. Following traditional<br />

wisdom, they have been organized in<strong>to</strong> five major overlapping domains:<br />

spiritual, cognitive, social/behavioral, emotional, <strong>and</strong> physical.<br />

This organization maps reasonably weil on the mind, body, <strong>and</strong> spirit<br />

division of traditional wisdom. The "Four Worlds" Native American<br />

tradition consider four major developmental tasks for youth-namely<br />

physica), emotional, cognitive, <strong>and</strong> spiritual. An additional cluster variable<br />

of social/behavior competencies has been added because of. the Iarge<br />

number of resilience sturlies that equate resilience with social competen·<br />

cies. Luthar (1993) has distinguished three of these five domains in her longitudinal<br />

study, namely social. emotional <strong>and</strong> physical. lt is of interest that<br />

very few researchers have foctised on the spiritual or affective domain<br />

which is critical <strong>to</strong> the concepts of ftexibility, perseverance, hopefulness,<br />

optimism <strong>and</strong> the ability <strong>to</strong> bounce back after failure.<br />

- Individual characteristics within each of these five internal domains<br />

will be discussed in detail including those that are more determined by<br />

genetics or biological, in utero environment effecting biology or temperament,<br />

<strong>and</strong> the five types of self fac<strong>to</strong>rs. In addition, resilience processes will<br />

be discussed a1though few researchers currently focus their research on<br />

these unique processes despite resilience specifically being considered the<br />

ability <strong>to</strong>bounceback after failure. A nurober of longitudinal studies of positive<br />

life adaptation despite stressful or high-risk environments (Garmezy,<br />

1985; Luthar, 1991; Radke-Yarrow & Brown, 1993; Werner & Smith, 1982,<br />

1992) indicate that strength, hardiness <strong>and</strong> competence are predictive of<br />

successful interactions with their environments.<br />

<strong>Resilience</strong> as Internat Capacity or Competence. Resilient children<br />

have coping skills <strong>and</strong> competencies <strong>to</strong> minimize stress of negative impacts,<br />

maintain self-esteem <strong>and</strong> gaining access <strong>to</strong> opportunities. In studying<br />

resilience, researchers have often focused on the internal characteristics of


Euvironmental Risk <strong>Fac<strong>to</strong>rs</strong><br />

•Anti-social values<br />

•ATOD Abusing Parents<br />

•Poverty<br />

•Farnily Dysfunction<br />

•Family Conflict<br />

\<br />

Spirituality<br />

•Motivation<br />

•Purpose in Life<br />

•Perseverance<br />

I<br />

Environmental Protective Fac:<strong>to</strong>rs<br />

•Prosocial Family Values<br />

•Low Family Stress<br />

•Good Parent/Child Relationship<br />

•Good Parenting Skills (Supervision <strong>and</strong> Discipline)<br />

•Parent/Child Attachment<br />

•Positive RoJe Models<br />

•Strong Extended Family<br />

•High Expectations<br />

• Family T eaching!Support<br />

•Family Guidance/Counsel<br />

•Opportunities for Meaningful Family Involvement<br />

!<br />

Figure 2. Interna} Self Resmency Characteristics.<br />

!---+<br />

.J


198 K•rol L. Kumpfer<br />

depends on direction or focus. Hence, the variables hypothesized for inclusion<br />

are the following:<br />

• Dreams <strong>and</strong> Goals (B<strong>and</strong>ura, 1989; Quin<strong>to</strong>n et al., 1993; Rutter &<br />

Quin<strong>to</strong>n, 1984)<br />

• Purpose in Life (Neiger, 1992)<br />

• Existential Meaning for Life (Frankel, 1959)<br />

• Spirituality (Dunn, 1994; Gordon & Song, 1994).<br />

• Belief in Uniqueness or in Oneself (Gordon & Song, 1994).<br />

• lnternal Locus of Control (Luthar, 1991a; Murphy & Moriarty, 1976;<br />

Parker, Cowen, Work, & Wyman, 1990; Werner & Smith, 1992).<br />

• Hopefulness <strong>and</strong> Optimism (Seligman, 1975)<br />

• Determination <strong>and</strong> Perseverance (B<strong>and</strong>ura, 1989; Werner, 1986)<br />

Dreams!Goals <strong>and</strong> Purpose in Life. One very important psychological<br />

characteristic of resilient children living in high-risk environments is<br />

their ability <strong>to</strong> dream (B<strong>and</strong>ura, 1989; Rutter & Quin<strong>to</strong>n, 1984; Quin<strong>to</strong>n,<br />

Pickeles, Maughan, & Rutter, 1993), create plausible fantasies for themse]ves<br />

<strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> deveJop a mission or purpose for their lives (Bernard, 1991;<br />

Richardson, Neiger, Jensen, & Kumpfer, 1990). Surviving difficuJt chiJdhoods<br />

often was achieved by a sense of uniqueness or specialness <strong>and</strong><br />

spiritual belief that they are here for some cosmic purpose-often a<br />

general commitment <strong>to</strong> making the world a better place for similar types<br />

of children.<br />

This purpose in life or existential meaning (Frankel, 1959), helps these<br />

resilient individuals <strong>to</strong> endure hardships, because they believe they must<br />

survive <strong>to</strong> complete their mission. Beardslee's research (1983; 1989) on civil<br />

rights workers in South <strong>and</strong> Segal's research (1986) on prisoners ofwar discovered<br />

that a predominant characteristic of resilient individuals was<br />

having a purpose in life reJated <strong>to</strong> helping others. Creating a perceived<br />

purpose for their pain <strong>and</strong> suffering (Beardslee, 1989; Segal, 1986) <strong>and</strong><br />

healing through helping or caring for others (Werner, 1986; Segal, 1986)<br />

helps individuals <strong>to</strong> regain environmental mastery <strong>and</strong> perceived control<br />

(Taylor, 1983) which has been found critical <strong>to</strong> maintaining hope in adverse<br />

life threatening situations ( e.g., concentration camps, prisons, war zones,<br />

acute care hospital units, <strong>and</strong> abusive families). Neiger (1991) confirmed in<br />

a structural equations modeling study of college students in South Carolina<br />

that resiliency was the final pathway <strong>to</strong> positive life adaptation. The most<br />

predictive variable in his multifac<strong>to</strong>riaJ resiliency duster was life purpose,<br />

next was problern solving, followed by self-efficacy.<br />

r


202 Karol L. Kumpfer<br />

inteUigence dramatically underachieve in school performance if they have<br />

a low internallocus of control or poor ability <strong>to</strong> delay gratification (Luthar<br />

& Zigler, 1992). Unfortunately, some ethnic youth (particularly African­<br />

American boys) equate academic achievement <strong>to</strong> "acting White", because<br />

of few concrete roJe models of success through the traditional academic<br />

achievement (Fordham & Ogbu, 1986). High verbal skills are critically<br />

important for success in early grades of elementary school, unfortunately,<br />

children of substance abusers <strong>and</strong> children of deprived families tend <strong>to</strong><br />

have lower verbal scores on st<strong>and</strong>ardized tests (Kumpfer, 1987). Increasing<br />

verbal competence by teaching phonetic reading, speJiing, <strong>and</strong> encouraging<br />

reading will help disadvantaged youth. High verbal<strong>and</strong> reading skills tend<br />

<strong>to</strong> help resilient children <strong>to</strong> leam about the world outside of their family<br />

<strong>and</strong> neighborhoods (Luthar & Zigler, 1992). Fairy tales <strong>and</strong> children's<br />

s<strong>to</strong>ries frequently portray disadvantaged children overcoming the odds,<br />

hence exposing children <strong>to</strong> these cultural s<strong>to</strong>ries can increase dreams <strong>and</strong><br />

belief in achievement against the odds. Girls, however, often experience a<br />

downward academic performance trajec<strong>to</strong>ry in middle school related <strong>to</strong><br />

sex-rote sociaJization processes sometimes promoted by traditional cultural<br />

s<strong>to</strong>ries, teacher's differential response <strong>to</strong> girls, <strong>and</strong> an increasing social orientation<br />

that sometimes overwhelms academic priorities. Maternal educational<br />

achievement <strong>and</strong> roJe modefing tends <strong>to</strong> help girls <strong>to</strong> achieve.<br />

Moral Reasoning. A product of higher intellectual thought is the<br />

higher moral reasoning Ievels these children are capable of attaining <strong>and</strong><br />

demonstrating. Jacobs <strong>and</strong> Wolin (1991) suggest that resilient children separate<br />

themselves from the value systems of their families by becoming their<br />

own moral guardians. Coles (1989) has also discussed the importance of<br />

"moral energy" in resilient children from impoverished homes around the<br />

world. Moral energy creates a life-sustaining force that can move resilient<br />

children <strong>to</strong>wards positive Jives. Wolin <strong>and</strong> Wolin (1993) consider morality<br />

as one of seven resiliencies in their <strong>Resilience</strong> M<strong>and</strong>ala, which defines<br />

morality as an informed conscience that extends wishes of a good personal<br />

life <strong>to</strong> all of humankind. Cognitive aspects of morality include judging right<br />

<strong>and</strong> wrong,_ developing internal images or st<strong>and</strong>ards for the way things<br />

should be or what is normative, valuing compassion, fairness <strong>and</strong> decency,<br />

<strong>and</strong> serving others. Kagan's (1984) research suggests that children develop<br />

by two years of age internal images or st<strong>and</strong>ards about how things or events<br />

should be. By age seven, Seiman (1980) reports children have a good idea<br />

about how parents should treat them- with sensitivity <strong>to</strong> tbeir feelings <strong>and</strong><br />

with generosity.<br />

lnsight <strong>and</strong> Intrapersonal Reflective Skills. ResiJient children have<br />

been described as "mini-psychologists'' capable of a quality of "early


<strong>Fac<strong>to</strong>rs</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Processes</strong> Contrlbuting <strong>to</strong> Resillence 103<br />

knowing" (Wolin, 1989) <strong>and</strong> personal insight (Flach, 1988). According <strong>to</strong><br />

Wolin <strong>and</strong> Wolin (1993), insight is the nurober one resiliency. Insight is the<br />

mental habit of asking penetrating questions of oneself <strong>and</strong> subsequently,<br />

providing honest answers. Resilient children from dysfunctional parents<br />

often are aware very early in life that they are different from <strong>and</strong> stronger<br />

than their sick parent. While empathetic <strong>and</strong> caring, they develop "adaptive<br />

distancing" <strong>to</strong> protect their sense of healthy separation from the parent's<br />

maladaptive coping skills <strong>and</strong> life patterns. This failure <strong>to</strong> identify with their<br />

dysfuncüonal parent <strong>and</strong> <strong>to</strong> find more successful role models is adaptive for<br />

these children (Beardslee & Podorefsky, 1988). Adaptive distancing has<br />

been found <strong>to</strong> be a crucial mechanism in children of alcoholics (Bennett,<br />

Wolin, Reiss, & Teitelbaum, 1987; Berlin & Davis, 1989).<br />

The capability <strong>to</strong> analyze one's psychological <strong>and</strong> physical strengths<br />

<strong>and</strong> compare them <strong>to</strong> others takes a certain Ievel of intrapersonal<strong>and</strong> interpersonal<br />

reflective skill, which not all children possess. For some reason only<br />

a few children excel in analyzing their intrapersonal skills <strong>and</strong> judging their<br />

strengths <strong>and</strong> limitations, whereas others living in similar family <strong>and</strong> peer<br />

environments are completely unaware of these psychological differences.<br />

Possibly, the early development of certain conceptual centers in the brain<br />

make this type of relational thinking possible in resilient youth.<br />

Self-Esteem <strong>and</strong> Abilily ToRes<strong>to</strong>re Self-Esteem. Resilient youth have<br />

higher self-esteem associated with an accurate appraisal of their increased<br />

strengths <strong>and</strong> capabilities. They have resilienl self-efficacy (B<strong>and</strong>ura, 1977;<br />

1989) <strong>and</strong> the ability <strong>to</strong> res<strong>to</strong>re self-esteem (Flach, 1988) after failure or disruption<br />

in homeostasis. Self-efficacy is a self-perception about competence<br />

<strong>to</strong> perform specific behavioral tasks (B<strong>and</strong>ura, 1977) <strong>and</strong> influences choice<br />

of tasks or challenges attempted, the degree of effort employed, <strong>and</strong> emotional<br />

reactions <strong>to</strong> threat of failure (Lawrence & McLeroy, 1986).<br />

Youth who avoid opportunities <strong>to</strong> master challenges because of Iow<br />

self-esteem or specific task self-efficacy will have a more difficult time developing<br />

resilience (Schunk & Carbonari, 1984). B<strong>and</strong>ura (1989) believes that<br />

overcoming stressors or taking on challenges is necessary for the development<br />

of self-efficacy. Children who are overprotected or shy in accepting<br />

challenges are bindered in developing self-efficacy <strong>and</strong> competencies. He<br />

has written: "lf people experience only easy successes, they come <strong>to</strong> expect<br />

quick results <strong>and</strong> their sense of efficacy is easily undermined by failure" (p.<br />

1179). Perseverance <strong>and</strong> determination are possible by-products of resilient<br />

self-efficacy that in turn lead <strong>to</strong> increased self-efficacy when youth are successfuJ<br />

in "sticking it out through <strong>to</strong>ugh times '' (p. 1179).<br />

Planning Ability. Another cognitive skill probably related <strong>to</strong> intelligence<br />

is planning ability, which has becn found related <strong>to</strong> resilience in high-


206 Karol L Kumpfer<br />

Prevention programs that explicitly teach generat problem-solving<br />

have found increased positive outcomes in their participants, including<br />

reductions in precursors of substance use (DeMarsh & Kumpfer, 1986;<br />

Kumpfer, 1991; Spivack & Shur, 1982). Botvin <strong>and</strong> associates (1990) have<br />

also reported reductions in <strong>to</strong>bacco use (Botvin & Tortu, 1988) <strong>and</strong> alcohol<br />

use (Botvin, Baker, Renick, Filazzola, & Botvin, 1984) following a generic<br />

life skills or social problern solving skills program including advertising<br />

analysis, decision making, goal-setting, stress management, communication<br />

<strong>and</strong> dating skills. Follow-up studies (Botvin, Baker, Dusenbury, Tortu, &<br />

Botvin, 1990) repJicated these positive effects in seventh <strong>to</strong> ninth graders<br />

by providing booster sessions over three years. The exception <strong>to</strong> positive<br />

findings of the study was that, although the prevalence of self·reported<br />

in<strong>to</strong>xication was reduced, the quantity <strong>and</strong> frequency of alcohol use was not.<br />

Specific problern solving strategies for the resistance of alcohol <strong>and</strong><br />

other drugs, such as peer refusal skills, assertiveness training, advertising or<br />

peer appeal analysis, consequences analysis have been popular substance<br />

abuse prevention interventions. Specific problern solving skills are often<br />

scripted <strong>and</strong> taught through cognitive behavioral techniques involving modeling,<br />

role playing, behavioral reversal often involving repeated practice,<br />

audio or video feedback <strong>and</strong> reinforcement for new skills.<br />

Increased resilience <strong>to</strong> initiation <strong>to</strong> <strong>to</strong>bacco (Biglan et al., 1985) <strong>and</strong><br />

lower prevalence rates for weekly <strong>to</strong>bacco, alcohol <strong>and</strong> marijuana use<br />

(Pentz, 1983; Pentz et al., 1989) were found after two years of intervention<br />

using these specific skill training strategies in cm,nbination with other multicomponent<br />

education, parent <strong>and</strong> community strategies. After three years,<br />

the positive results for reduced alcohol use in participants was no Ionger<br />

supported (Johnson et al., 1989), which is a common finding for many<br />

specific skills training programs. Reducing alcohol use in youth appears <strong>to</strong><br />

be much more difficult. However, Dieiman <strong>and</strong> associates (Dielman,<br />

Schope, Leech, & Butchard, 1989) have had success in reducing the increasing<br />

rate of alcohol misuse in students who began using alcohol with their<br />

peers in unsupervised settings. Their specific skills training program also<br />

reduced the rate of increase in susceptibility <strong>to</strong> peer pressure.<br />

Unfortunately, many of the specific substance abuse resistance skiH<br />

training programs do not directly <strong>and</strong> independently test the efficacy of the<br />

skills training in reducing alcohol <strong>and</strong> other drug use. Several exceptions<br />

are the Ketchel <strong>and</strong> Bieger (1989) study <strong>and</strong> the Kim, McLeod, <strong>and</strong> Palmgren<br />

(1989) study which both found increased intentions <strong>to</strong> not use alcohol<br />

<strong>and</strong> other drugs, <strong>and</strong> decreased substance use.lt has been hypothesized that<br />

these positive drug use changes may be due more <strong>to</strong> reinforeerneut of the<br />

existing school or peer non-use norms (Kumpfer, Moskowitz & Klitzner,<br />

1986), or <strong>to</strong> recent changes in the social climate <strong>to</strong>wards <strong>to</strong>bacco <strong>and</strong> other


<strong>Fac<strong>to</strong>rs</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Processes</strong> Contrlbufing <strong>to</strong> <strong>Resilience</strong> 101<br />

drug use or the informal social control climate (Moskowitz, 1983): Hansen<br />

<strong>and</strong> Graham (1991) have empirical evidence that changing the perceptions<br />

of AOD norms is more salient than specific resistance skills training in<br />

reducing AOD use in youth. ·<br />

Multi-Cultural <strong>and</strong> Bi-Gender Competencies. Research studies have<br />

also found multicultural or bicultural competence as related <strong>to</strong> less sub·<br />

stance use in youth (Oetting & Beauvais, 19.90; Buriel, Calzada, & Vasquez,<br />

1982). Youth who are capable of acting competently in several cultures if<br />

needed are more successful. Additionally, bi·ge·nder competence is related<br />

<strong>to</strong> increased resilience <strong>and</strong> life success in women (Dunn, 1994).<br />

Empathy <strong>and</strong> Interpersonal Socia/ Skills. Another hallmark of<br />

resilient children is their sense of responsibility for others, willingness <strong>to</strong><br />

care for others, <strong>and</strong> ability<strong>to</strong>be empathetic of the needs of others (Werner,<br />

1985; 1986). Related coping skills include interpersonal social skills (Platt,<br />

Belding, & Husb<strong>and</strong>, in press), an engaging personality, good listening<br />

<strong>and</strong> communicating ski11s (Wolin, 1991), <strong>and</strong> politencss (Kumpfer, 1990a,b).<br />

Resilient children are responsive <strong>and</strong> active in their relationships with<br />

others <strong>and</strong> elicit more positive responses from their associates even from<br />

infancy (Demos, 1989; Werner & Smith, 1982). Because of their increased<br />

social skills, resilient children are popular <strong>and</strong> have increased choice of<br />

fdends allowing them <strong>to</strong> establish friendships with positive, prosocial peers<br />

if they desire (Berndt & Ladd, 1989).<br />

When combined with the availability of social support (Rutter, 1987)<br />

<strong>and</strong> the willingness <strong>to</strong> use external supports (Schwartz, Jacobson, Hauser,<br />

& Dornbush, 1989), resilient children are capable of getting the social supports<br />

they need <strong>to</strong> buffer stressors <strong>and</strong> teach them even more coping skills.<br />

Jacobs <strong>and</strong> Wolin (1991) report that resilient children of alcoholics even as<br />

young children sought ••oases of health in interacting wilh the healthiest parts<br />

of their troubled families" (p. 111 ). By middle childhood, these children are<br />

connecting with neighbors, teachers, <strong>and</strong> other substitute parents. Werner<br />

(1985) also identified attachment <strong>to</strong> community institutions <strong>and</strong> positive<br />

rote models of youth Ieaders as important. Felsman (1989) found in his<br />

study of resilient Colombian street children that they were skillful in locating<br />

existing social supports <strong>and</strong> using ther;n <strong>to</strong> their advantage. Children of<br />

mothers who are willing <strong>to</strong> use social agency support are reported <strong>to</strong> have<br />

more competencies (Musick, Scott, Spencer, Goldman, & Cohler, 1984),<br />

possibly because of the effective role modeling of the mother, increased<br />

resulting resources or increased social contact.<br />

Wolin <strong>and</strong> Wolin (1993) report resilient individuals have a capacity for<br />

intimacy. In addition, they are careful in their choice of intimates. Rutter


208 Karol L. Kumpfer<br />

<strong>and</strong> Quin<strong>to</strong>n (1994) found the more resilient females living in institutions<br />

married better than their less successful cohorts. They choose more prosocial<br />

<strong>and</strong> supportive spouses <strong>and</strong> friends.<br />

Emotional Stability <strong>and</strong> Emotional Management<br />

Characteristics of resilient individuals that could be considered primarily<br />

within the domain of emotional characteristics <strong>and</strong> skills would be:<br />

• Happiness ( versus Depression)<br />

• Recognition of Feelings<br />

• Emotional Management Skills <strong>and</strong> Ability <strong>to</strong> Control Anger <strong>and</strong><br />

Depression<br />

• Ability<strong>to</strong>Res<strong>to</strong>re Seif-Esteern<br />

• Humor (Masten, 1982; Wolin & Wolin, 1993)<br />

• Hopefulness<br />

Happiness. Resilient individuals are characterized as reasonably<br />

happy people, at least they are not prone <strong>to</strong> depression or negative<br />

appraisals of reality characteristic of depressed individuals. The ability <strong>to</strong><br />

be hopeful <strong>and</strong> optimistic may occur because of mastery experiences but<br />

also because of good mental health practices. avoiding psychotropic drugs,<br />

eating weil, reducing stress <strong>and</strong> getting exercise. After all, Garmezy ( 1974)<br />

has noted that resilient people work weil <strong>and</strong> play weiL<br />

Emotional Management Skills. A primary characteristic of resilient<br />

children is their optirnism <strong>and</strong> positiveness about life. Resilient individuals<br />

recognize feelings <strong>and</strong> can control undesirable feelings such as fear, anger<br />

<strong>and</strong> depression. For children of alcoholics <strong>and</strong> drug abusers, learning <strong>to</strong> recognize<br />

feelings may be more difficult. Special exercises are deveJoped in<br />

prevention programs <strong>to</strong> help these children learn <strong>to</strong> recognize their feelings,<br />

however, nothing is as effective as having parents who daily discuss<br />

feelings with the child. Learning <strong>to</strong> recognize <strong>and</strong> control destructive<br />

impulses based on these feelings are learned through roJe modeling <strong>and</strong><br />

transactions between a parent <strong>and</strong> a child.<br />

Humor. Many clinical <strong>and</strong> research descriptions of resilient individuals<br />

mention them as happy, energetic people who frequently use humor as<br />

a coping strategy. The ability <strong>to</strong> use humor <strong>to</strong> reduce tension <strong>and</strong> stress <strong>and</strong><br />

res<strong>to</strong>re perspective is a skill of many resilient children. The ability <strong>to</strong> find<br />

the comic in the tragic, <strong>to</strong> make themselves <strong>and</strong> others laugh, is considered<br />

by Wolin <strong>and</strong> Wolin (1993) as one of several basic resiliencies in children


Stressors., Life Events.<br />

Challenges<br />

Disruption<br />

Envirosocial<br />

Protec'ti ve<br />

<strong>Processes</strong><br />

V))<br />

Envirosocial<br />

Enhancing<br />

<strong>Processes</strong><br />

esses<br />

Borneostasis<br />

... .....<br />

..._ ...<br />

Envirosocial<br />

Reinlegrating<br />

<strong>Processes</strong><br />

Figure 3. The Resiliency Process Model.


I :I<br />

I<br />

214 Karol L. Kumpfer<br />

The primary implication of resiliency research for prevention programming<br />

with high-risk youth isthat prevention programs should focus more directly<br />

on the development of the primary resiJient characteristics identificd in this<br />

review. These resilient traits can be developed by modifying the external<br />

environment <strong>to</strong> increase proteelive processest employing small challenges,<br />

creating opportunities for involvement <strong>and</strong> the demonstration of competence,<br />

<strong>and</strong> increasing involvement, bonding <strong>and</strong> attachment <strong>to</strong> prosocial<br />

groups. Interventions should strive <strong>to</strong> improve the quality of caregiving,<br />

teaching parents <strong>and</strong> youth workers how <strong>to</strong> increase resiliency characteristics<br />

in children, reducing environmental inequities <strong>and</strong> stressors for children<br />

in high-risk or unhealthy environments. Some interventions should work<br />

directly with children <strong>and</strong> youth <strong>to</strong> increase personal power <strong>and</strong> resilience.<br />

Assessments of personal <strong>and</strong> academic competence could provide information<br />

for tailored resilience interventions, possibly through computer<br />

interactive programs or video tapes for caretakers <strong>and</strong> families.<br />

Summary<br />

Why is it that we have invested so much in<strong>to</strong> material <strong>and</strong> scientific<br />

technology <strong>to</strong> build better material goods, yet we know so little about how<br />

<strong>to</strong> build better children? The future success of our country depends on<br />

increasing our technology <strong>and</strong> interest in building better children. For this<br />

reason, increased research in<strong>to</strong> this field of resilience <strong>and</strong> child development<br />

is critical <strong>to</strong> the prevention field <strong>and</strong> our nation's prosperity <strong>and</strong> weil<br />

being. Promoting resilience in youth holds promise for creating more effective<br />

prevention programs. As can be concluded from this extensive review<br />

of research, there are no simple formulas for instiUing resilience. Research<br />

on resilience, while holding much promise for the alcohol <strong>and</strong> drug preverition<br />

field, is also plagued by a number of definitionaJ issues <strong>and</strong> research<br />

limitations. This review suggests that virtually all aspects of a child's personality<br />

<strong>and</strong> environment as weil as transactional relationships between the<br />

child <strong>and</strong> his/her environment inßuence positive life adaptation. Determining<br />

the most salient variables is compJicated by gender, culture, his<strong>to</strong>ry,<br />

local environmental issues <strong>and</strong> developmental appropriateness. A number<br />

of caveats on the quality of the research have been discussed including<br />

issues in theoretical paradigmst definitions, measurement, designs <strong>and</strong><br />

statistical analysis procedures. Because of the increasing willingness <strong>to</strong><br />

see behaviors of individuals as influenced by dynamic transactions with<br />

significant others <strong>and</strong> the environment, studying the predic<strong>to</strong>rs of resilience<br />

have become much more complicated. Additional transactional, crosslagged<br />

prospectfve studies are needed incJuding multivariate data collec-


<strong>Fac<strong>to</strong>rs</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Processes</strong> ContributJng <strong>to</strong> <strong>Resilience</strong> ZlS<br />

tions strategies <strong>and</strong> advanced statistical methodologies capable of analyzing<br />

transactional data.<br />

The <strong>Resilience</strong> Framework is discussed <strong>to</strong> organized the findings of<br />

many cross-sectional <strong>and</strong> longitudinal resilience studies. This theoretical<br />

framework includes six major predic<strong>to</strong>rs of resilience: initiating stressors,<br />

environmental risk <strong>and</strong> proteelive mechanisms, personpenvironmental<br />

transactional processes, internal individual resiliency fac<strong>to</strong>rs (spiritual, cognitive,<br />

behavioral, emotional, <strong>and</strong> physical), resiliency processes, <strong>and</strong> the<br />

positive outcome. The results of this review suggest that there are many<br />

opportunities or possible targets for increasing resilience in a plannful<br />

manner in designing more supportive environments for children living in<br />

at-risk families.<br />

In order <strong>to</strong> increase drug prevention effectiveness with high-risk youth,<br />

selective <strong>and</strong> indicated prevention interventions, as recently defined by the<br />

Institute of Medicine (Mrazek & Haggerty, 1994), should be designed with<br />

a special focus on promoting resilience. The prevention field is not far from<br />

this goal. By <strong>and</strong> large prevention programs currently focus on increasing<br />

proteelive processes <strong>and</strong> fac<strong>to</strong>rs. They use risk fac<strong>to</strong>r assessments primarily<br />

<strong>to</strong> determine high-risk status only. While prevention interventionsoften<br />

focus on increasing proteelive <strong>and</strong> resilience processes, the evaluations<br />

often primarily assess reductions in risk fac<strong>to</strong>rs. While increasing protective<br />

fac<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>and</strong> resilience theoretically should reduce risk fac<strong>to</strong>rs, the<br />

degree of change in resilience <strong>and</strong> proteelive processes should also be<br />

measured. Empirically tested etiological models including both risk <strong>and</strong><br />

protective/resilience fac<strong>to</strong>rs <strong>and</strong> processes should be developed through<br />

prevention studies. Such multivariate studies should increase the field's<br />

information about proteelive <strong>and</strong> resilience building processes <strong>and</strong> hence,<br />

better inform prevention program methodology on ways <strong>to</strong> increase effectiveness<br />

with highprisk youth.<br />

References<br />

Abelson, W. 0., Zigler, E., & DeBlasi, C. L. (l974). Effects of a four-year follow through<br />

program on economicaHy disadvantaged children.Joumal of Educational Psychology, 66.<br />

756- 771 .<br />

Anthony, E. J. (1987). Risk, vulnerability <strong>and</strong> resilience: An overview. ln E. J. Anthony & B.<br />

Cohler (Eds.), The in vulnerable child (pp. 3-48). New York: The Guilrord Press.<br />

Baldwin, A. L., Baldwin, C. P., Kasser, T., Zax, M., Sameroff, A., & Seifer, R. {1993). Contextual<br />

risk <strong>and</strong> resiliency during late adolescence. Development <strong>and</strong> Psychopathology, 5,<br />

741 - 761.<br />

B<strong>and</strong>ura, A. ( 1977). Self4 efficacy: Toward a unifying lheory of behavior change. Psychological<br />

Review, 84, 191-215.


II<br />

116 Karol L. Kumpfer<br />

B<strong>and</strong>ura, A. (1989). Human agency in social cognitive theory. American Psychologist, 44(9),<br />

1175-1184.<br />

Barbarin, 0. (1993). Coping <strong>and</strong> <strong>Resilience</strong>: El(ploring the inner Jives of African-American<br />

Children. Journal of 8/ack Psychology.<br />

Barnes, G. M., & Welle, J. W. (1986). Patterns<strong>and</strong> predic<strong>to</strong>rs of alcohol use among 7-12th grade<br />

students in New York State. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 47,53-62.<br />

Beardslee, W. R. (1983). Commitment <strong>and</strong> endurance: A study of civil righls workers who<br />

stayed. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 53, 34-42.<br />

Beardslee, W. R. (1989). The role of self-underst<strong>and</strong>ing in resilient individuals: The development<br />

of a perspective. American Journal of Orclwpsychiatry, 59(2), 266-278.<br />

Beardslee, W. R., & Podorefsky, 0. (1988). Resilient adolescents whose parcnts have serious<br />

psychiatric disorders: Importance of self-underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>and</strong> relationships. American<br />

Journal of Psyclaiatry, 145(1}, 63-69.<br />

Bennett, L. A., Wolin, S. J., Re iss, 0., & Thilelbaum, M. A. (198J). Coupfes at risk for alcoholism<br />

transmission: Proteelive inHuences. Family Process, 26, 111-129.<br />

Berlin, R., & Oavis, R. B. ( 1989). Children from alcoholic families: Vulnerability <strong>and</strong> resilience.<br />

In 1imothy F. Dugan <strong>and</strong> Robert Coles (Eds.), The Child in Our Times: Studies in the<br />

Development of Resiliency (pp. 81-105). New York: Brunner/Mazel.<br />

Benard, B. (1991 ). Fastering resilience in kids: Proteelive fac<strong>to</strong>rs in fami/y, schoo/, <strong>and</strong> community.<br />

San Francisco: Western Center for Orug-free Schools <strong>and</strong> Communities,August 1991.<br />

Benard, B. (1993, Aprii).{Oiscussion du ring conference on "Pulling Resiliency in<strong>to</strong> Substance<br />

Abuse Prevention for Adolescents."J Unpublished prcsentation, New York.<br />

Berndt, T., & Ladd, G. ( 1989). Peer Relationships in Child Developmem, New York: John Wiley<br />

& Sons (Ed.).<br />

Biglan, A., Severson, H. H., Ary, 0. V., Faller, C., Thompson, R., Nautel, C., Lichtenstein, E., &<br />

Weissman, W. W. (1985). Refusal skills training <strong>and</strong> thc prevention of adolescent cigarette<br />

smoking. Preventative Medicine, 11, 199-2 t 1.<br />

Bleuler, M. (1984). Different forms of childhood stress <strong>and</strong> pattcrns of adult psychiatric<br />

outcome. In N. F. Watt, E. J. Anthony, & J. E. Rolf (Eds.). Clzildretr at risk for schizophrenia<br />

(pp. 537- 542}. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.<br />

Block, J. H., & Block, J. ( 1980). The role of ego-control <strong>and</strong> ego-resiliency in the organization<br />

of behavior. ln W. A. Collins (Ed.), Developmenc of cogtrition, affect, <strong>and</strong> social relations:<br />

The Minnesota Symposia on Child Psycho/ogy, 13, 39-lOl. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence<br />

Erlbaum Associates.<br />

Botvin, G. J., Baker, E., Dusenbury, L., Tortu, S. , & Botvin, E. M. (1990). Preventing adolescent<br />

drug use through a multimodal cognitive-behavioral approach: Results of a 3-year<br />

study. Journal of Consulting <strong>and</strong> Clinica/ Psycho/ogy, 58, 437-446.<br />

Botvin, G. J., Baker, E., Filazzola, A. 0., & Botvin, E. M. (1990). A Cognitive-Behavioral<br />

Approach <strong>to</strong> Substance Abusc Prevention: One-year follow-up. Addictive Behaviors, 15,<br />

47-63.<br />

Botvin, G. J., & Tortu, S. (1988). Peer relationships, social competence, <strong>and</strong> substance abuse<br />

· prevention: Implications for the family.Journal of Social Work und Human Sexuality, 6(2),<br />

105-119.<br />

Botvin, G. 1., Baker, E., Renick, N. L., Filazzola, A. 0., & Botvin, E . M. (1984). A cognitive<br />

behavioral approach <strong>to</strong> substance abuse prevcntion. Addictive Behaviors, 9, 137- 147.<br />

Bronfenbrenner, U. (1986). Ecology of the family as a context for human development:<br />

Research pcrspectives. Developmenta/ Psyclaology, 22,723- 742.<br />

Bronfenbrenner, U., & Crouter, A. C. (1983}. The evolution of environmental models in developmental<br />

research. ln P. 1-1. Mussen (Ed.), H<strong>and</strong>book of child psychology (4th edition).<br />

New York: Wiley.


218 Karol L. Kumprer<br />

an elementary school-based alcohol misuse prevention program by type of prior drinking<br />

experience. Journal of School Heallh, 59(6), 255-263.<br />

Doernberger, C. H. (1992). Aspects of competence: resilience among inner-city adolescents.<br />

Unpublished Masters' Thesis, Yale University, New Haven, Cf.<br />

Ounn, D. (1994). Rcsilient reintegration of married women with dependent children:<br />

Employed <strong>and</strong> unemployed. (Doc<strong>to</strong>ral Dissertation, Department of Health Education,<br />

University of Utah, Salt Lake City, Utah.)<br />

Dunst, C. (1995). Risk <strong>and</strong> opportunity fac<strong>to</strong>rs influencing child <strong>and</strong> family behavior <strong>and</strong> development.<br />

Presenlation, 4th National Early Intervention Meeting. Coimbra, Portugal, June,<br />

1995.<br />

D'Zurilla, T. J., & Nezu, A. M. (1990). Development <strong>and</strong> preliminary evaluation of the social<br />

problem-solving inven<strong>to</strong>ry. Psychological Assessmem: A Journal of Consu/ting <strong>and</strong> Clinical<br />

Behavior, 2, 156-163.<br />

Egel<strong>and</strong>, B., Carlson, E., & Sroufe, A. (1993). <strong>Resilience</strong> as process. Developmem a11d Psychoparhology,<br />

5, 517-528.<br />

Eider, G. H., Caspi, A., & Van Nguyen, T. (1986). Resourceful <strong>and</strong> vulnerable children: Family<br />

inßuence in hard times. In R. K. Silbereisen & K. Eyferth (Eds.), Development os oction<br />

in context (pp. 167-186). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.<br />

Evans, C. ( 1996). <strong>Resilience</strong> in Native Americans: A Qualitative Study. (Doc<strong>to</strong>ral Dissertation,<br />

Departmenl of Health Education, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, Utah 84112.)<br />

Felsman, 1. K. (1989). Risk <strong>and</strong> resiliency in childhood. In T. F. Dugan & R. Coles (Eds.),<br />

The child in our Iimes: Studies in the development of resiiiency, 56-80. New York:<br />

Brunncr/Mazel.<br />

Flach, f. F. (1988). <strong>Resilience</strong>: Discovering new strengthat times of stress. New York: Ballantine<br />

Books.<br />

Fordham, S., & Ogbu, J. U. (1986). Black students' school success: Coping with the "burden of<br />

'acting white.'" Urban Review, I 8, 176-206.<br />

Franke[, V. E. (1959). Man 's Search for Meaning: An lntroduction lo Logotherapy. Prefacc by<br />

Gordon W. Allport. Bos<strong>to</strong>n: Beacon Press.<br />

Freud, S. (1908). On the sexual theories of children.<br />

Gamble, T. J .• & Zigler, E. (1986). Effecls of infant day care: Another Iook at the evidence.<br />

American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 56, 26-42.<br />

Garmezy, N. (1974). Children at risk: The search for antecedents of schizophrenia. J. Conceptual<br />

models <strong>and</strong> research methods. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 8, 14-90.<br />

Garmezy, N. ( 1985). Stress-resistant children: The search for proteelive fac<strong>to</strong>rs. In J. E. Stevensan<br />

(Ed.), Recent research in developmental psychopathology (pp. 213-233). Journal of<br />

Child Psychology <strong>and</strong> Psychiatry, 4 (Book Supplement).<br />

Garmezy, N. (1991). Resiliency <strong>and</strong> vulnerability <strong>to</strong> adverse developmenlal outcomes associated<br />

with poverty. American Behavioral Scientist, 34(4), 416-430.<br />

Garmezy, N. (1993). Vulnerability <strong>and</strong> resilience. In D. C. Funder, R. D. Parker, C. Tomlinson­<br />

Keesey, & K. Widaman (Eds.), Studying lives through time: Approaches <strong>to</strong> personality <strong>and</strong><br />

development (pp. 377- 398). Washing<strong>to</strong>n, DC: American Psychological Association.<br />

Garmezy, N .• l/i. Masten, A. S. (1986). Stress, competence, <strong>and</strong> resilience: Common frontiers for<br />

therapist <strong>and</strong> psychopathologist. Behavior Therapy, 57(2), 159-174.<br />

Garmezy, N., & Masten, A. S. (1991 ). The protective role of competence indica<strong>to</strong>rs in children<br />

at risk. In E. M. Cummings, A. L. Greene, & K. H. Karraker (Eds.), Llfe-span developmenta/<br />

psycho/ogy: Perspectives on stress <strong>and</strong> coping (pp. 151-174). Hillsdale, NJ:<br />

Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.<br />

Garmezy, N., Masten, A. S., & Tellegen, A. (1984). The study or stress <strong>and</strong> competence in children:<br />

A building block for developmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55,<br />

97- 111.


222 Karol L. Kumpfer<br />

Parker, G. R., Cowen, E. L., Work, W. C., & Wyman, P. A. (1 990) Thst correlates of stress affected<br />

<strong>and</strong> stress rcsilient outcomes among urban children. Journal of Primary Prevelltion, I I,<br />

19-35.<br />

Pentz, M. A. (1983). Prevention of adolescent substance abuse through social skills development.<br />

In T. J. Glynn, C. G. Leukefeld, & J. P. Ludford (Eds.), Preventing Adolescent Drug<br />

Abuse: Intervention strategies (pp. 195-232). National Institute on Drug Abuse Research<br />

Monograph 47 (DHHS Publication No. ADM 83- 1280). Washing<strong>to</strong>n, DC: U. S. Government<br />

Printing Office. ·<br />

Pentz, M. A.,Johnson, C. A., Dwyer,J. H., MacKinnon, D. P., Hansen, W. B., & Flay, B. R. (1989).<br />

A Comprehensive Community Approach <strong>to</strong> Adolescent Drug Abuse Prevention: Effects<br />

of cardiovascular diseasc risk fac<strong>to</strong>rs. A1ma/s of Medicine. 21, 219-222.<br />

Plan, J. J., Belding, M. A., & Husb<strong>and</strong>, S. D. (in press). Competency building in adolescents. ln<br />

K. L. Kumpfer (Ed.), Promoting AOD Resiliency in High Risk Youth. Submitted <strong>to</strong> Office<br />

'of Substance Abuse Prevention, Rockville, MD.<br />

Plomin, R., & Daniels, D. (1 987). Why are children in the same family so different from one<br />

another? Behavior <strong>and</strong> Brain Sciences, 10, 1-60.<br />

Quin<strong>to</strong>n, 0., Pickcles, A., Maughan, B., & Rutter, M. (1993). Partners, peers, <strong>and</strong> pathway:<br />

Assortative pairing <strong>and</strong> continuities in conduct disordcr. Deve/opment <strong>and</strong> Psychopathology,<br />

5, 763- 783.<br />

Radke-Yarrow, M., & Brown, E. (1 993). <strong>Resilience</strong> <strong>and</strong> vulnerability in children of multiplerisk<br />

farnilies. Development <strong>and</strong> Psychopatho/ogy, 5, 581- 592.<br />

Radke- Yarrow, M., & Sherman, T. ( 1990). Hard growing: Children who survive. In J. E. Rolf,<br />

A. S. Masten, D. Cicchetti, K. Nuechterlein, & S. Weintraub (Eds.), Risk <strong>and</strong> Proteelive<br />

<strong>Fac<strong>to</strong>rs</strong> in the Developmenl of Psychopathology (pp. 97-119). New York: Cambridge University<br />

Press.<br />

Richardson, G. E., Neiger, B. L., Jensen, S., & Kumpfer, K. (1990). The Resiliency Model. Health<br />

Education, 2/(6), 33-39.<br />

Richters, J. E ., & Martinez, P. E. (1993). Violent communities. family choices, <strong>and</strong> children's:<br />

An algorithm for improving thc odds. Development <strong>and</strong> Psychopathology, 5, 609-<br />

627.<br />

Rot[, J. E., Masten, A. S., Cicchetti, D., Nuechterlein, K., & Weintraub, S. (1990). (Eds.), Risk<br />

<strong>and</strong> proteelive fac<strong>to</strong>r.s in the developmenr of psychopathology. New York: Cambridge University<br />

Press.<br />

Roosa, M .. Beals, J., S<strong>and</strong>ler, I. N., Pillow, D. R. (1990). The role of risk <strong>and</strong> protective fac<strong>to</strong>rs<br />

in predicting symp<strong>to</strong>ma<strong>to</strong>logy in adolescent self-identified children of alcohol parents.<br />

American Journal of Community Psychology, 18(5), 725-741.<br />

Rotter, J. B. (1954). Sociallearning <strong>and</strong> clinical psychology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice­<br />

Hall.<br />

Rutter, M. (1979). Protective fac<strong>to</strong>rs in children's responses <strong>to</strong> stress <strong>and</strong> disadvanlage. In M.<br />

W. Kent <strong>and</strong> J. Rolf (Eds.), Primary Prevention of Psychopathology, Vol. /II: Social Competence<br />

in Children (pp. 49-74). Hanover, N. H., University Press of New Engl<strong>and</strong>.<br />

Rutter, M. (1982). Epidemiological-longitudinal approaches <strong>to</strong> the study of development. Jn<br />

W. A. Collins (Ed.). Tlte concept of developme/11. The Minnesofa Symposia 011 Child Psyclwlogy<br />

(Vol. 15) (pp. 105-144). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrencc Erlbaum.<br />

Rutter, M. (1987). Psychosocial resilience <strong>and</strong> protective mechanisms. Amtrican Journal of<br />

Orthopsychiatry, 57(3 ), 316-331.<br />

Rutter, M. (1990). Psychosocial resilience <strong>and</strong> protective mechanisms.ln J. Rolf, A. S. Masten,<br />

D. Cicchetri, K. H. Neuchterlcin, & S. Weintraub (Eds.). Risk <strong>and</strong> proteelive fac<strong>to</strong>rs in the<br />

developmem of psychopathology (pp. 181-214). New York: Ca mbridge Universily Press.<br />

1


<strong>Fac<strong>to</strong>rs</strong> <strong>and</strong> <strong>Processes</strong> <strong>Contributing</strong> <strong>to</strong> <strong>Resilience</strong> 223<br />

Rutter, M. (1992, December). Transitions<strong>and</strong> turning points in development. Paper presented<br />

at the British Psychological Society Conference, London.<br />

Rutter, M. (1993). <strong>Resilience</strong>: Some conceptual considerations. Journal of Adolescent Healtlt,<br />

14, 626--631.<br />

Rutter, M., & Quin<strong>to</strong>n, 0. (1994). Long-term follow-up of women institutionalized in childhood:<br />

Factars promoting good functioning in adult lifc. British Journal of Developmental<br />

Psychology. /8,225-234.<br />

Rutter, M., Maughan, ß., Mortimore, P., Ous<strong>to</strong>n, J., & Smith, A. (1979). Fifteenthous<strong>and</strong> hours:<br />

Secondary schools <strong>and</strong> their effects on children. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University<br />

Press.<br />

Sameroff, A. J., & Ch<strong>and</strong>ler, M. J. (1975). Reproductive risk <strong>and</strong> the continuum of caretaking<br />

casualty. In F. 0. Horawitz (Ed.), Review of child development research 4, (pp. 187-244).<br />

Chicago: Univcrsity of Chicago Press.<br />

Sameroff, A. J., Seifer, R., & Barocas. R. (1983). Impact or parental psychopathology: Diagnosis.<br />

severity, or social status effects. ft1/ant Mental Health Journal, 4(3), 236-249.<br />

Scarr, S., & McCarty, K. (1983). How people make their own environments: A theory of geno·<br />

type environment effects. Child Deve/oprnent, 54, 424-435.<br />

Schunk, D. H., & Carbonari, J. P. (1984). Self-erficacy models. In J. 0. Matarazzo, S. M. Weiss,<br />

J. A. Herd, <strong>and</strong> N. E. Miller (Eds.), Behavioral health: A h<strong>and</strong>book of hea/th (pp. 230-247).<br />

New York: Wiley.<br />

Schwarrz, J. M ., Jacobson, A. M., Hauser, S. T., & Dornbush, B. B. (1989). Explorations of vulncrability<br />

<strong>and</strong> resilience: Case studies of diabetic adolescents <strong>and</strong> their fa milies. Jn T. F.<br />

Dugan & R. Colcs (Eds.), The child in our times: Studies in the developmen.t of resiliency<br />

(pp. 134-156). New York: Brunner/Mazel:<br />

Segal, J. (1986). Winning life 's <strong>to</strong>ughest battles: Roots of human resifience. New York: McGraw­<br />

Hill.<br />

Seligman, M. (1975). Helplessness: On Depression, Development, <strong>and</strong> Death. San Francisco:<br />

Frecman.<br />

Selman, R. L. ( 1980). The growth of interpersonal underst<strong>and</strong>ing: Deve/opmenta{ <strong>and</strong> clinical<br />

analysis. New York: Academic Press.<br />

Spivack, G., & Shure, M. 8. (1982). The cognition of social adjustment: Interpersonal cognitive<br />

problem-solving thinking.ln B. B. Lahey & A. E. Kazdin (Eds.), Advances in Clinical Child<br />

Psychology (pp. 323-372). New York: Plenum Press.<br />

Spivack, G., Platt, J. J., & Shure, M. (1976). The problem-solving approach ro adjustmenr. San<br />

Francisco: Jossey-Bass.<br />

Staudinger, U. M., Marsiske, M., & Balles, P. B. (1993). <strong>Resilience</strong> <strong>and</strong> Ieveis of reserve capacity<br />

in latcr adulthood: Perspective from life-span theory. Developmenl an.d Psy·<br />

clzoparhology, 5, 541-566.<br />

S<strong>to</strong>uthamcr-Loeber, M., Loeber, R., Farring<strong>to</strong>n, D. P., Zhang, Q., Van Kammen, W., & Maguin,<br />

E. ( 1993). l11e double edge of proteelive <strong>and</strong> risk fac<strong>to</strong>rs ror delinquency: Interrelations<br />

<strong>and</strong> developmental patterns. Developmem <strong>and</strong> Psychopathology, 5, 683-701.<br />

Strauss, B. G., & Glaser, A. L. (1967). The Discovery of Grmmded Theory. Chicago: Aldine.<br />

Swearingen, E. M., & Cohen, L. H. (1985). Measurernent or adolescents' life events: A Junior<br />

HighLife Experiences Survey. American Journal of Community Psychology, 13,69-85.<br />

Tarter, R., & Mezzich, A. (1992). On<strong>to</strong>geny of substance abusc: Perspectives <strong>and</strong> findings.<br />

In M. Glantz <strong>and</strong> R. Pickens (Eds.), Vulnerabi/ity <strong>to</strong> drug abuse. American Psychiatrie<br />

Association.<br />

Tay lor, S. E. ( 1983 ). Adj ustmen t <strong>to</strong> threatening even ts: A theory of cogni tive adapt ation. A merican<br />

Psyclzologist, 38, 1161-1173.


Karol L. Kumpfer<br />

Thrner, S. (1995). Family variables related <strong>to</strong> adolescent substance misuse: Risk <strong>and</strong> resiliency<br />

fac<strong>to</strong>rs. In T. R. Oullot<strong>to</strong>, G. R. Adams, & R. Montemayor (Eds.}, Substance Misuse in<br />

Adolescence. Thous<strong>and</strong> Oaks, CA: Sage.<br />

Walker, R. J. (1995). Res·ilie11cy in adult children of alcoholic parents: Use of structural Equation<br />

Modeling. Dissertation submitted <strong>to</strong> The University of Utah.<br />

Wallerstein, J. (1983). Children of divorce: The psychological tasks or thc child. American<br />

Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 53(2), 230-243.<br />

Werner, E. E. (1985). Stress <strong>and</strong> proteelive fac<strong>to</strong>rs in children's lives.ln A. R. Nicol (Ed.), Longitudinal<br />

studies in child psychology <strong>and</strong> psychiatry (pp. 335-355). New York: John Wiley<br />

<strong>and</strong> Sons.<br />

Werner, E. E. (1986). Resilient offspring of alcoholics: A longitudinal study from birth <strong>to</strong> age<br />

18. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 47,34-40.<br />

Werner, E. E. (1989). High-risk children in young adulthood: a longiludinal study from birth<br />

<strong>to</strong> 32 years. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 59,72-81.<br />

Werner, E. E. (1993). Risk, resilience, <strong>and</strong> recovery: Perspectives from the Kauai Longitudinal<br />

Study. Development <strong>and</strong> Psychopathol.ogy, 5, 503- 515<br />

Werner, E. E., & Smith, R. S. (1989). Vulnerable but invincib/e: A longitudinal study of resdient<br />

chi/dren <strong>and</strong> youth. New York: Adams-Bannister-Cox. (Original work published 1982}.<br />

Werner, E. E., & Smith, R. S. (1992). Overcoming the odds: High risk chi/dren from birth <strong>to</strong><br />

adulthood. Ithaca, New York: Cornell University Press.<br />

Wertlieb, D., Weigel, C., Springer. T., & Feldstein, M. (1989). Temperament as a modera<strong>to</strong>r of<br />

children's strcssful experiences. In S. Chess, A. Thomas. & M. E. Hcrtzig (Eds.), Annual<br />

progress in child psychiatry <strong>and</strong> child development: /988. New York: Brunner/Mazel.<br />

White, J. L., Moffitt, T. E., & Silva, P. A. (1989). A prospective replication of the proteelive<br />

effccts of 10 in subjects at high risk for juvenile delinquency. Journut of Consu/ting <strong>and</strong><br />

Clinical Psychology, 57, 719- 724.<br />

Wolin, S. J. (1989). Resiliency in children of alcoholics. Paper presented at the American<br />

Academy of Child <strong>and</strong> Adolescent Psychiatry Institute on Substance Abuse, New York,<br />

New York, Oc<strong>to</strong>ber 13, 1989.<br />

Wolin, S. I. (1991 ). Paper prcsented atthe Protecting Vulnerable Childrcn Project, Children of<br />

Alcoholics Foundation, lnc. Prince<strong>to</strong>n University, Prince<strong>to</strong>n, New Jersey, November, 1991.<br />

Wolin, S. J., & Wolin, S. (1993). Bound <strong>and</strong> Determined: Growing up resilient in a troubled<br />

family. New York: Yilfard Press.<br />

Worl<strong>and</strong>, J., Weeks, D. G., & Janes, C. L. (1987). Predicting mental health in children at risk.<br />

In E. J. Anthony & B. J. Cohler (Eds.), Thein vulnerable child (pp. 185-210). New York:<br />

GuiJrord Press.<br />

Wyman, P. A., Cowen, E. L., Work, W. C., & Kerley, J. H. ( t 993). The role of children 's future<br />

cxpectations in self·system functioning <strong>and</strong> adjustment <strong>to</strong> life stress: A prospective study<br />

of urban at·risk children. Development <strong>and</strong> Psychopathology, 5, 649-661.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!